Childhood Cognitive Ability Predicts Adult Financial Well-Being
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Definition of Work Success
1.2. Multiple Variables
1.3. Cause and Consequence
1.3.1. The Current Study
1.3.2. Hypotheses
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
- Parental Social Class. Parental social class at birth was measured by the Registrar General’s measure of social class (RGSC). RGSC is defined according to occupational status [54]. Where the father was absent, the social class (RGSC) of the mother’s father was used. RGSC was coded on a six-point scale (numbers in brackets represent percentages in this study: I professional (6.1%); II managerial/technical (40.5%); IIIN skilled non-manual (22.0%); IIIM skilled manual (17.0%); IV semi-skilled (12.3%); and V unskilled (2.1%) occupations [55].
- Childhood Cognitive Ability. Childhood cognitive ability was assessed at age 11 in school using a general ability test [56] consisting of 40 verbal and 40 non-verbal items. For the verbal items, children were presented with an example set of four words that were linked either logically, semantically, or phonologically. For the non-verbal tasks, shapes or symbols were used. The children were then given another set of three words or shapes or symbols with a blank. Participants were required to select the missing item from a list of five alternatives. Scores from these two set of tests correlate strongly with scores on an IQ-type test used for secondary school selection (r = 0.93, [56]), suggesting a high degree of validity.
- Educational Qualifications. At age 33, participants were asked about their highest academic or vocational qualifications. Responses are coded to the six-point scale of National Vocational Qualifications levels (NVQ) ranging from ‘none’ to ‘higher degree level’: 0 = no qualifications; 1 = some qualifications (Certificate of Secondary Education Grades 2 to 5); 2 = O level (equivalent to qualifications taken at the end of compulsory schooling); 3 = A level (equivalent to university entrance level qualifications); 4 = diploma and equivalent; and 5 = university degrees and equivalent.
- Personality Traits. Personality traits were assessed at age 50, by the 50 questions from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) [57]. Responses (5-point, from “Strongly Agree” to “Strongly Disagree”) are summed to provide scores on the so called ‘Big-5’ personality traits: extraversion, emotional stability/neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeableness and intellect/openness. Scores on each trait range between 5 and 50 with higher scores equating to higher levels of each trait, of which 10 items for extraversion, 10 items for emotional stability, 10 items for agreeableness, 10 item for conscientiousness, and 10 items for intellect. Alpha was 0.73 for extraversion, 0.88 for emotional stability, 0.81 for agreeableness, 0.77 for conscientiousness, and 0.79 for openness.
- Marital Status. At age 54, cohort members provided information on their current marital status. The derived measure contains four groups: “married” (73%), “never married” (9.3%), “divorced/separated” (15.7%), and “widowed” (2%). Because of the small number in the group “widowed” (n = 91), this group was excluded from further analyses. Dummy variables were created for the following analyses. Married (1 = married, 0 = not married); never married (1 = never married, 0 = not never married); divorced/separated. (1 = divorced/separated, 0 = not divorced/separated).
- Occupational Prestige. Data on current or last occupation held by cohort members at age 54 are coded according to the RGSC described above, using a 6-point classification.
- Adult Earning Ability. At age 54, participants were asked about their current net payment per month. A small proportion of observations with very low or very high incomes are dropped (81 cases) from the sample so that the results are not unduly influenced by these outliers, and incomes were logged in the following analyses.
3. Results
3.1. Correlational Analysis
3.2. Regression Analysis
4. Discussion
5. Limitations
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Furnham, A. Personality and Occupational Success. In The SAGE Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Sage: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Strenze, T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success. Intelligence 2007, 35, 401–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Su, R. The Power of Vocational Interests and Interest Congruence in Predicting Career Success. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, B.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T. Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 582–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, T.W.H.; Eby, L.T.; Sorensen, K.L.; Feldman, D.C. Predictors of objective and subjective career success: A meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 2005, 58, 367–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, T.W.H.; Feldman, D.C. Subjective career success: A meta-analytic review. J. Vocat. Behav. 2014, 85, 169–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seibert, S.E.; Crant, J.M.; Kraimer, M.L. Proactive personality and career success. J. Appl. Psychol. 1999, 84, 416–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Judge, T.A.; Cable, D.M.; Boudreau, J.W.; Bretz, R.D. An empirical investigation of the predictors of executive career success. Pers. Psychol. 1995, 48, 485–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Judge, T.A.; Higgins, C.A.; Thoresen, C.J.; Barrick, M.R. The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Pers. Psychol. 1999, 52, 621–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, T.W.H.; Feldman, D.C. Human capital and objective indicators of career success: The mediating effects of cognitive ability and conscientiousness. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2010, 83, 207–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutin, A.; Costa, P.; Miech, R.; Eaton, W. Personality and career success. Eur. J. Personal. 2009, 23, 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hirschi, A.; Jaensch, V.K. Narcissism and career success: Occupational self-efficacy and career engagement as mediators. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 77, 205–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Child, D. A comparative study of personality, intelligence and social class in a technological university. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1969, 39, 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kiker, B.; Condon, C. The influence of socioeconomic background on the earnings on young men. Hum. Relat. 1981, 16, 94–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zagorsky, J. Do you have to be smart to be rich? Intelligence 2007, 35, 489–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, B.; Lester, D. Personality traits and economic activity. Appl. Econ. 2016, 48, 653–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furnham, A.; Cheng, H. Socio-demographic indicators, intelligence, and locus of control as predictors of adult financial status. J. Bus. Psychol. 2017. submitted. [Google Scholar]
- Rode, J.; Arthaud-Day, M.; Mooney, C.; Near, J.; Baldwin, T. Ability and personality predictors of salary, perceived job success, and perceived career success in the initial career change. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 2008, 16, 292–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wai, J.; Lincoln, D. Investigating the right tail of wealth. Intelligence 2016, 54, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abele, A.; Spurk, D. How do objective and subjective career success interrelate over time? J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2009, 82, 803–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spurk, D.; Abele, A. Who earns more and why? A multiple mediation model from personality to salary. J. Bus. Psychol. 2011, 26, 87–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Le, K.; Donnellan, M.; Conger, R. Personality development at work. J. Personal. 2014, 82, 44–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Converse, P.D.; Piccone, K.A.; Tocci, M.C. Childhood self-control, adolescent behavior, and career success. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 59, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, I.J.; Taylor, M.D.; Hart, C.L.; Wilson, V.; Smith, D.G.; Blane, D.; Starr, J.M. Intergenerational mobility and mid-life status attainment: Influences of childhood intelligence, childhood social factors, and education. Intelligence 2005, 33, 455–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoon, I. Childhood cognitive ability and adult academic attainment: Evidence from three British cohort studies. Longitud. Life Course Stud. 2010, 1, 241–258. [Google Scholar]
- Spinath, B.; Spinath, F.M.; Harlaar, N.; Plomin, R. Predicting school achievement from general cognitive ability, self-perceived ability, and intrinsic value. Intelligence 2006, 34, 363–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tong, S.; Baghurst, P.; Vimpani, G.; McMichael, A. Socioeconomic position, maternal IQ, home environment, and cognitive development. J. Paediatr. 2007, 151, 284–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Breen, R. Educational Expansion and Social Mobility in the 20th Century. Soc. Forces 2010, 89, 365–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erikson, R.; Goldthorpe, J.H. Has social mobility in Britain decreased? Reconciling divergent findings on income and class mobility. Br. J. Sociol. 2010, 61, 211–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Furnham, A.; Argyle, M. The Psychology of Money; Routledge: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer, S.; Hemstrom, O.; Peter, R.; Vagero, D. Education, income and occupational class cannot be used interchangeably in social epidemiology. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2006, 60, 804–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haveman, R.; Smeeding, T. The role of higher education in social mobility. Future Child. 2006, 16, 125–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heath, A. Social Mobility; Fontana: Glasgow, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Von Stumm, S.; Macintyre, S.; Batty, D.; Clark, H.; Deary, I. Intelligence, social class of origin, childhood behaviour disturbance and education as predictors of status attainment in midlife in men. Intelligence 2010, 38, 202–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrell, T. The personality of high earning MBA’s: A 20-year longitudinal study. Hum. Perform. 1969, 2, 457–463. [Google Scholar]
- Bowles, S.H.; Gintis, H.; Osborne, M. The determinants of earnings. J. Econ. Lit. 2001, 39, 1137–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boudreau, J.; Boswell, W.; Judge, T. Effects of personality and executive career success in the United States and Europe. J. Vocat. Behav. 2001, 58, 53–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Groves, M. How important is your personality? Labor market returns to personality for women in the US and UK. J. Econ. Psychol. 2005, 26, 827–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palifka, B. Personality and income in Mexico. J. Econ. Psychol. 2009, 30, 92–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linz, S.; Semykina, A. Personality traits as performance enhancers. J. Econ. Psychol. 2009, 30, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Judge, T. Core self-evaluations and work success. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 18, 58–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Judge, T.; Cable, D. The effect of physical height on workplace success and income. J. Appl. Psychol. 2004, 89, 428–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Judge, T.; Hurst, C.; Simon, L. Does it pay to be smart, attractive, or confident (or all three)? Relationships among general mental ability, physical attractiveness, core self-evaluations and income. J. Appl. Psychol. 2009, 94, 742–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Judge, T.; Hurst, C. How the rich (and happy) get richer (and happier): Relationship of core self-evaluations to trajectories in attaining work success. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 849–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmidt, F.; Hunter, J. General mental ability in the world of work. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 162–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nyhus, E.; Pons, E. The effects of personality on earnings. J. Econ. Psychol. 2005, 26, 363–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelissen, J.; de Graaf, P. Personality, social background and occupational career success. Soc. Sci. Res. 2006, 35, 702–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haberfeld, Y. Pay, valence of pay and gender. J. Econ. Psychol. 1992, 13, 93–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Judge, T.; Livingstone, B. Is the gap more than gender? A Longitudinal analysis of gender, gender role orientation, and earnings. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 994–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Semykina, A.; Linz, S. Gender differences in personality and earnings. J. Econ. Psychol. 2007, 28, 387–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferri, E.; Bynner, J.; Wadsworth, M. Changing Britain, Changing Lives: Three Generations at the Turn of the Century; Institute of Education: London, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Davie, R.; Butler, N.; Goldstein, H. From Birth to Seven; Longman: London, UK, 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Fogelman, K. Britain’s 16-Year-Olds; National Children’s Bureau: London, UK, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Marsh, C. Social class and occupation. In Key Variables in Social Investigation; Burgess, R., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Leete, R.; Fox, J. Registrar General’s social classes: Origins and users. Popul. Trends 1977, 8, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Douglas, J.W.B. The Home and the School; Panther Books: London, UK, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, L.R. A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lowerlevel facets of several five-factor models. In Personality Psychology in Europe; Mervielde, I., Deary, I., De Fruyt, F., Ostendorf, F., Eds.; Tilburg University Press: Tilburg, The Netherlands, 1999; Volume 7, pp. 7–28. [Google Scholar]
- Paterson, T.A.; Harms, P.D.; Steel, P.; Crede, M. An assessment of the magnitude of effect sizes. J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 2016, 23, 66–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furnham, A.; Cheng, H. The stability and change of malaise scores over 27 years: Findings from a nationally representative sample. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 79, 30–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dweck, C. Mindset; Constable and Robinson: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Deary, I.J.; Whalley, L.J.; Starr, J.M. A Lifetime of Intelligence; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Furnham, A. Personality and Intelligence at Work; Routledge: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Wille, B.; Hofmans, J.; Feys, M.; De Fruyt, F. Maturation of work attitudes: Correlated change with Big Five personality traits and reciprocal effects over 15 years. J. Organ. Behav. 2014, 35, 507–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, B.W.; Walton, K.E.; Viechtbauer, W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Variables | Mean Male SD | Correlation | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | |||
1. | Adult earning ability | 3.12 (0.42) | _ | ||||||||||||
2. | Gender | 0.53 (0.50) | −0.252 *** | _ | |||||||||||
3. | Parental social class | 3.28 (1.23) | 0.166 *** | −0.015 | _ | ||||||||||
4. | Childhood cognitive ability | 103.8 (12.90) | 0.235 *** | 0.070 *** | 0.260 *** | _ | |||||||||
5. | Educational qualifications | 2.67 (1.45) | 0.381 *** | −0.114 *** | 0.317 *** | 0.471 *** | _ | ||||||||
6. | Extraversion | 29.33 (6.75) | 0.092 *** | 0.087 *** | 0.020 | 0.019 | 0.061 *** | _ | |||||||
7. | Emotional stability | 28.91 (7.00) | 0.102 *** | −0.151 *** | −0.001 | 0.072 *** | 0.070 *** | 0.208 *** | _ | ||||||
8. | Agreeableness | 37.04 (5.27) | −0.002 | 0.395 *** | 0.057 *** | 0.106 *** | 0.066 *** | 0.368 *** | 0.045 ** | _ | |||||
9. | Conscientiousness | 34.11 (5.16) | 0.080 *** | 0.087 *** | −0.006 | 0.018 | 0.051 ** | 0.136 *** | 0.164 *** | 0.263 *** | _ | ||||
10. | Openness | 32.43 (5.12) | 0.221 *** | −0.030 | 0.138 *** | 0.246 *** | 0.312 *** | 0.404 *** | 0.094 *** | 0.322 *** | 0.209 *** | _ | |||
11. | Occupational prestige | 4.05 (1.23) | 0.467 *** | −0.012 | 0.221 *** | 0.359 *** | 0.473 *** | 0.126 *** | 0.073 *** | 0.122 *** | 0.102 *** | 0.255 *** | |||
12 | Married | 0.73 (0.44) | 0.039 ** | −0.069 *** | −0.002 | 0.035 * | 0.039 * | 0.021 | 0.034 * | 0.001 | 0.020 | −0.033 * | 0.045 ** | ||
13. | Never married | 0.9 (0.29) | 0.011 | −0.034 * | 0.019 | 0.004 | 0.052 ** | −0.094 *** | −0.021 | −0.082 *** | −0.040 * | 0.021 | −0.005 | _ | |
14. | Divorced/separated | 0.16 (0.36) | −0.034 * | −0.083 *** | 0.001 | −0.025 | −0.070 *** | 0.057 ** | −0.024 | 0.064 *** | 0.011 | 0.028 | −0.031 * | _ | _ |
Measures | β | t | p |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | −0.240 *** | 15.65 | <0.001 |
Parental social class | 0.030 | 1.81 | 0.071 |
Childhood cognitive ability | 0.046 ** | 2.91 | 0.004 |
Educational qualifications | 0.125 *** | 7.63 | <0.001 |
Personality traits | |||
Extraversion | 0.029 | 1.83 | 0.067 |
Emotional stability | 0.004 | 0.25 | 0.801 |
Agreeableness | −0.014 | 0.84 | 0.399 |
Conscientiousness | 0.039 ** | 2.76 | 0.006 |
Openness | 0.039 ** | 2.47 | 0.014 |
Marital status (being married as the reference group) | |||
Never married | 0.001 | 0.11 | 0.916 |
Divorced/separated | 0.011 | 0.79 | 0.432 |
Occupational prestige | 0.371 *** | 24.14 | <0.001 |
Variance explained | R2 adjusted = 0.30, F (13,4415) = 149.26 *** |
© 2016 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Furnham, A.; Cheng, H. Childhood Cognitive Ability Predicts Adult Financial Well-Being. J. Intell. 2017, 5, 3. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5010003
Furnham A, Cheng H. Childhood Cognitive Ability Predicts Adult Financial Well-Being. Journal of Intelligence. 2017; 5(1):3. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5010003
Chicago/Turabian StyleFurnham, Adrian, and Helen Cheng. 2017. "Childhood Cognitive Ability Predicts Adult Financial Well-Being" Journal of Intelligence 5, no. 1: 3. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5010003
APA StyleFurnham, A., & Cheng, H. (2017). Childhood Cognitive Ability Predicts Adult Financial Well-Being. Journal of Intelligence, 5(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5010003