Intangible Assets and Analysts’ Overreaction and Underreaction to Earnings Information: Empirical Evidence from Saudi Arabia
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Sample and Variables
2.1. Sample
2.2. Variables
- EPSt and EPSt−1 denotes the actual earnings per share respectively for years t and t − 1.
- Ft represents the EPSt consensus forecast, calculated eight months before the fiscal year’s t end.
- FEt defines the forecast error for year t, computed as (EPSt − Ft)/|EPSt|.
- ECt−1 refers to the prior year’s earnings change (prior performance) from t − 2 to t − 1, normalized by earnings per share t − 2, ECt−1 = (EPSt−1 − EPSt−2)/|EPSt−2|.
- HINT is a dummy variable, taking the value 1 if the firm belongs to high intangible asset subsample; otherwise, it is 0. The subsample was divided according to the total intangible assets to total asset ratio (Barth et al. 2001; Barron et al. 2002). The median of this ratio distinguishes high intangible asset firms (upper half) from low intangible asset firms (lower half). We assumed that this ratio reflected the effect of intangible assets more than the sectoral decomposition. Intangible asset ratio (INT) is defined as recognized intangible assets to total assets.
3. Empirical Results
3.1. Prior-Earnings Change and Current Forecast Error
3.1.1. Descriptive Analysis
- Analysts’ overreaction to good news and underreaction to bad news: Descriptive test of the impact of positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior year earnings change on current forecast error.
- 2.
- Impact of intangible assets on analysts’ overreaction to good news and underreaction to bad news: Descriptive test of the association between positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year earnings change and the current-year forecast error for both subgroups of high and low intangible assets.
3.1.2. Regression Analysis
- Test of general underreaction or overreaction to prior-year earnings change: regression of the current-year forecast error on prior-year earnings.
- 2.
- Test of analysts’ overreaction to positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year earnings change: regression of the current-year forecast error on subgroups of positive and negative prior-year earnings change.
- 3.
- Test of the impact of intangible assets on analysts’ overreaction (good news) and underreaction to (bad news): regression of the current-year forecast error on both subgroups of positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year earnings change for high and low intangible asset firms.
3.2. Prior-Year and Current-Year Forecast Errors
3.2.1. Descriptive Analysis
- Analysts’ overreaction to goods news and underreaction to bad news: Descriptive test of the impact of the positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year forecast error on current forecast error.
- 2.
- Impact of intangible assets on analysts’ overreaction to good news and underreaction to bad news: Descriptive test of the association between the positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year forecast error and the current-year forecast error for both subgroups of high and low intangible assets.
3.2.2. Regression Analysis
- Test of general overreaction or underreaction to prior-year forecast error: regression of the current-year forecast error on prior-year forecast error.
- 2.
- Test of analysts’ overreaction to good news and underreaction to bad news: regression of the current-year forecast error on positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year forecast error.
- 3.
- Test of the impact of intangible assets on analysts’ overreaction to good news and underreaction to bad news: Regression of the current-year forecast error on the positive (good news) and negative (bad news) prior-year forecast error for high and low intangible asset firms.
4. Potential Implications for Investors and Market Efficiency
5. Conclusions and Discussion
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Abarbanell, Jeffry, and Victor Bernard. 1992. Tests of analysts’ overreaction/underreaction to earnings information as an explanation for anomalous stock price behaviour. The Journal of Finance 47: 1181–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ali, Ashiq, April Klein, and James Rosenfeld. 1992. Analysts’ use of information about permanent and transitory components in forecasting annual EPS. The Accounting Review 67: 183–98. [Google Scholar]
- Amir, Eli, and Yoav Ganzach. 1998. Overreaction and underreaction in analysts’ forecasts. Journal of Econonomic Behavior and Organization 37: 333–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bah, Rahim, and Pascal Dumontier. 2001. R&D intensity and corporate financial policy: Some international evidence. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting 28: 103–15. [Google Scholar]
- Banker, Rajiv, Rong Huang, Ram Natarajan, and Sha Zhao. 2019. Market valuation of intangible assets: Evidence on SG&A expenditure. The Accounting Review 94: 61–90. [Google Scholar]
- Barker, Richard, Andrew Lennard, Stephen Penman, and Alan Teixeira. 2021. Accounting for intangible assets: Suggested solutions. Accounting and Business Research 52: 1–30. [Google Scholar]
- Barron, Orie, Donal Byard, Charles Kile, and Edward Riedl. 2002. High-Technology Intangibles and Analysts’ Forecasts. Journal of Accounting Research 40: 289–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barth, Mary, Ron Kasnzik, and Maureen Mcnichols. 2001. Analyst Coverage and Intangible Assets. Journal of Accounting Research 39: 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bessière, Veronique, and Taoufik Elkemali. 2014. Does uncertainty boost overconfidence? The case of financial analysts’ forecasts. Managerial Finance 4: 300–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouteska, Ahmed, and Boutheina Regaieg. 2020. Psychology and behavioral finance: Anchoring bias by financial analysts on the Tunisian stock market. EuroMed Journal of Business 15: 39–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, Bergman, Guoyu Lin, and Aner Zhou. 2022. Analysts’ forecast optimism: The effects of managers’ incentives on analysts’ forecasts. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance 35: 100708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campbell, Sean, and Steven Sharpe. 2009. Anchoring Bias in Consensus Forecasts and Its Effect on Market Prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 44: 369–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, Jin Wou, and Hae Mi Choi. 2017. Analyst Optimism and Incentives under Market Uncertainty. Financial Review 52: 307–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Chang, Ganapathi Narayanamoorthy, Theodore Sougiannis, and Hui Zhou. 2020. Analyst underreaction and the post-forecast revision drift. Journal of Business Finance and Accounting 47: 1151–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Ester, Ilanit Gavious, and Baruch Lev. 2017. The positive externalities of IFRS R&D capitalization: Enhanced voluntary disclosure. Review of Accounting Studies 22: 677–714. [Google Scholar]
- Daniel, Kent, David Hirshleifer, and Avanidhar Subrahmanyam. 2001. Overconfidence, Arbitrage, and Equilibrium Asset Pricing. The Journal of Finance 56: 921–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Das, Somnath, Levine Carolyn, and Konduru Sivaramakrishnan. 1998. Earnings Predictability and Bias in Analysts’ Earnings Forecasts. The Accounting Review 73: 277–94. [Google Scholar]
- De Bondt, Werner, and Richard Thaler. 1990. Does security analysts overreact. The American Economic Review 80: 51–57. [Google Scholar]
- Easterwood, John, and Stacey Nutt. 1999. Inefficiency in analysts’ earnings forecasts: Systematic misreaction or systematic optimism? The Journal of Finance 54: 1777–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elkemali, Taoufik. 2023. Uncertainty and Financial Analysts’ Optimism: A Comparison between High-Tech and Low-Tech European Firms. Sustainability 15: 2270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elkemali, Taoufik, and Aymen Ben Rejeb. 2015. R&D intensity and financing decisions: Evidence from European firms. Economics Bulletin 35: 1042–55. [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, John, Donna Philbrick, and Christine Wiedman. 1995. Evidence from archival data on the relation between security analysts’ forecast errors and prior forecast revisions. Contemporary Accounting Research 11: 919–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, Jennifer, and Donna Philbrick. 1993. Analysts’ decisions as products of a multi-task environment. Journal of Accounting Research 31: 216–30. [Google Scholar]
- Griffin, Dale, and Amos Tversky. 1992. The Weighing of Evidence and the Determinants of Overconfidence. Cognitive Psychology 24: 411–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gu, Feng, and Weimin Wang. 2005. Intangible Assets, Information Complexity, and Analysts’ Earnings Forecasts. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting 32: 1673–702. [Google Scholar]
- Gu, Zhaoyang, and Jian Xue. 2007. Do analysts overreact to extreme good news in earnings? Review of Quantitative Finance and Accounting 29: 415–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirshleifer, David. 2001. Investor psychology and asset pricing. The Journal of Finance 56: 1533–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kadiyala, Padmaja, and Raghavendra Rau. 2004. Investor reaction to corporate event announcements: Underreaction or overreaction? The Journal of Business 77: 357–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1979. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 47: 263–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lichtenstein, Sarah, and Baruch Fischhoff. 1977. Do those who know more also know more about how much they know? The calibration of probability judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 3: 552–64. [Google Scholar]
- Lim, Terence. 2001. Rationality and Analysts’ Forecast Bias. The Journal of Finance 56: 369–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsden, Alastair, Madhu Veeraraghavan, and Min Ye. 2008. Heuristics of Representativeness, Anchoring and Adjustment, and Leniency: Impact on Earnings’ Forecasts by Australian Analysts. Quarterly Journal of Finance and Accounting 47: 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mendenhall, Richard. 1991. Evidence of possible underweighting of earnings-related information. Journal of Accounting Research 29: 170–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Brien, Patricia. 1988. Analysts’ forecasts as earnings expectations. Journal of Accounting and Economics 10: 53–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penman, Stephen. 1987. The distribution of earnings news over time and seasonalities in aggregate stock returns. Journal of Financial Economics 18: 199–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1974. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. The Journal of Finance 185: 1121–31. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, Frank. 2006. Information uncertainty and stock returns. The Journal of Finance 61: 105–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, Chen, Yubin Li, Suresh Govindaraj, and Zhaodong Zhong. 2022. CDS trading and analyst optimism. The British Accounting Review 54: 101109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variables | Number of Obs | Mean (Median) | Std Dev | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HINT&LINT | LINT | HINT | HINT&LINT | LINT | HINT | HINT&LINT | LINT | HINT | |
FEt | 1376 | 688 | 688 | −0.384 (−0.091) | −0.276 (−0.035) | −0.573 *** (−0.158 ***) | 1.066 | 0.718 | 1.267 *** |
ECt−1 | 1376 | 688 | 688 | 0.198 (0.154) | 0.124 (0.101) | 0.235 *** (0.185 **) | 0.325 | 0.201 | 0.413 *** |
INT | 1376 | 688 | 688 | 8.201(7.354) | 2.312 (1.901) | 15.311 *** (13.103 ***) | 5.023 | 3.012 | 7.201 *** |
ECt−1 > 0 | ECt−1 < 0 | Obs | |
---|---|---|---|
FEt MEAN | −0.245 *** | −0.351 | 1228 |
FEt MEDIAN | −0.135 *** | −0.241 | 1228 |
% Negative FEt | 60.5 *** | 69.8 *** | 1228 |
ECt−1 > 0 | ECt−1 < 0 | Obs | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
HINT | LINT | HINT | LINT | ||
FEt MEAN | −0.388 *** | −0.125 | −0.469 *** | −0.195 | 1228 |
FEt MEDIAN | −0.305 *** | −0.104 | −0.388 *** | −0.141 | 1228 |
% Negative FEt | 67.4 *** | 56.2 | 76.8 *** | 65.2 | 1228 |
α | β | R2 | Obs | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Whole Sample | −0.297 (−2.125 **) | 0.121 (8.125 ***) | 0.042 | 1228 |
ECt−1 > 0 | ECt−1 < 0 | |
---|---|---|
α | −0.138 (−1.614 *) | −0.407 (−4.524 ***) |
β | −0.062 (−4.084 ***) | 0.185 (12.091 ***) |
R2 | 0.024 | 0.083 |
Obs | 482 | 746 |
ECt−1 > 0 | ECt−1 < 0 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HINT | LINT | DIFFERENCE | HINT | LINT | DIFFERENCE | |
α0 + α1 | α0 | α1 | α0 + α1 | α0 | α1 | |
−0.268 (−1.853 **) | −0.075 (−1.375) | −0.193 (−1.782 *) | −0.658 (−7.846 ***) | −0.202 (−2.218 **) | −0.456 (−5.836 ***) | |
β0 + β1 | β0 | β1 | β0 + β1 | β0 | β1 | |
−0.196 (−7.412 ***) | −0.032 (−3.126 ***) | −0.164 (−5.297 ***) | 0.318 (15.223 ***) | 0.105 (8.451 ***) | 0.213 (12.041 ***) | |
R2 | 0.035 | 0.094 | ||||
Obs | 482 | 746 |
FEt−1 > 0 | FEt−1 < 0 | Obs | |
---|---|---|---|
FEt MEAN | −0.301 ** | −0.396 | 1192 |
FEt MEDIAN | −0.165 *** | −0.312 | 1192 |
% Negative FEt | 62.5 *** | 68.5 | 1192 |
FEt−1 > 0 | FEt−1 < 0 | Obs | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
HINT | LINT | HINT | LINT | ||
FEt MEAN | −0.391 *** | −0.163 | −0.445 *** | −0.213 | 1192 |
FEt MEDIAN | −0.289 *** | −0.122 | −0.361 *** | −0.158 | 1192 |
% Negative FEt | 66.1 *** | 57.5 | 75.5 *** | 64.1 | 1192 |
FEt = α + β FEt−1 + ζ | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
α | β | R2 | Obs | |
Whole Sample | −0.356 (−3.452 ***) | −0.285 (−12.446 ***) | 0.059 | 1192 |
Forecast Error FEt = α + β FEt−1 + ζ | ||
---|---|---|
FEt−1 > 0 | FEt−1 < 0 | |
α | −0.544 (−5.201 ***) | 0.188 (−2.101 **) |
β | −0.341 (−10.645 ***) | 0.098 (7.143 ***) |
R2 | 0.074 | 0.031 |
Obs | 507 | 685 |
FEt−1 > 0 | FEt−1 < 0 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HINT | LINT | DIFFERENCE | HINT | LINT | DIFFERENCE | |
α0 + α1 | α0 | α1 | α0 + α1 | α0 | α1 | |
−0.644 (−6.117 ***) | −0.341 (−4.085 ***) | −0.303 (−3.008 ***) | −0.325 (−3.821 ***) | −0.098 (−1.522 *) | −0.227 (−2.176 **) | |
β0 + β1 | β0 | β1 | β0 + β1 | β0 | β1 | |
−0.512 (−15.057 ***) | −0.284 (−12.102 ***) | −0.228 (−11.136 ***) | 0.201 (8.112 ***) | 0.078 (4.232 ***) | 0.123 (5.626 ***) | |
R2 | 0.096 | 0.051 | ||||
Obs | 507 | 685 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Elkemali, T. Intangible Assets and Analysts’ Overreaction and Underreaction to Earnings Information: Empirical Evidence from Saudi Arabia. Risks 2024, 12, 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/risks12040063
Elkemali T. Intangible Assets and Analysts’ Overreaction and Underreaction to Earnings Information: Empirical Evidence from Saudi Arabia. Risks. 2024; 12(4):63. https://doi.org/10.3390/risks12040063
Chicago/Turabian StyleElkemali, Taoufik. 2024. "Intangible Assets and Analysts’ Overreaction and Underreaction to Earnings Information: Empirical Evidence from Saudi Arabia" Risks 12, no. 4: 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/risks12040063