Complied by Belief Consistency: The Cognitive-Information Lens of User-Generated Persuasion
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Foundation
2.1. The Cognitive-Information Lens
Theoretical Foundation | Antecedents | Methodology/Context | Results | Reference |
---|---|---|---|---|
A trade-off between perceived costs and perceived benefits of information searching. | Review extremity and review count. Products types: search versus experience products. | Review materials: online reviews of 6 products (3 experience and 3 search) on the Amazon website. Experience products: a music CD, an MP3 player, and a video game. Search products: a digital camera, a cell phone, and a laser printer. | Product type moderates the effect of review extremity on review helpfulness. Extreme ratings are less helpful than moderate ratings in the review evaluation of experience products. Regarding the evaluation of review helpfulness, review count is better for examining search products than examining experience products. | [4] |
Explanatory and predictive models based on machine learning. | Review subjectivity features and review readability features. Reviewer-related features. | Text mining and sentimental analysis. Review materials: a review set of selected products (e.g., audio and video players, digital cameras, and DVDs). Context: Amazon website and its voting systems. Measures of review helpfulness = [number of helpful votes/number of total votes] for a review. | Reviews that mixed subjective with objective messages are perceived as more helpful. | [76] |
Dual process theories—elaboration likelihood model, heuristic systematic model. | Central cues: word count and the percentage of negative words. Peripheral cues: rating consistency, reviewer ranking, reviewer real name. Product type: search versus experience, high-price versus low-price. | Web data mining of reviews and reviewers from the 28 product categories on the Amazon website. | Two peripheral cues (e.g., rating consistency and reviewer credibility) and one central cue (e.g., review content) affect the helpfulness of reviews. Central cues are better to judge the review helpfulness of search and high-price products. Peripheral cues are better to judge the review helpfulness of experience and low-price products. | [37] |
Bach’s (1967) helping behavior model. | Source-based review features. Content-based review features. Review helpfulness is a formative construct that is measured in terms of source credibility, content diagnosticity, and vicarious expression. | A 2×2 factorial experiment. Context: An online shopping website uses scenario-based surveys of making purchase decisions under two information types—product features and product reviews from either experts or customers. | Source- and content-based review features determine review helpfulness. Customer-generated reviews are perceived as more helpful than expert-generated reviews. A concrete product review is perceived as more helpful than an abstract one. The interaction between source- and context-based review features can shape review helpfulness. | [2] |
The emotion-cognition information processing model. | Emotions (e.g., anxiety and anger) embedded in product reviews. Perceived cognitive efforts. | Two lab experiments and one field study using archival data from the Yahoo!Shopping website. | Anxiety-embedded reviews are perceived more helpful than anger-embedded reviews. The effects of negative emotions on review helpfulness are better explained by the beliefs of reviewers’ cognitive effort. | [75] |
The search-experience paradigm. The source-content- context model. | Product type: search versus experience. Source factors: reviewer rank, the disclosure of reviewer identity. Context factor: number of reviews for a product. Content factors: review extremity, review depth. | A total of over 28,000 product reviews across 10 product types were collected from Amazon.com (Korea). | Positive determinants of review helpfulness: reviewer reputation, review depth. The number of reviews and the disclosure of reviewer identity have a stronger effect on perceived review helpfulness for experience products. Reviewer reputation, review extremity, and review depth have a stronger effect on perceived review helpfulness for search products. | [38] |
Dual-process theory | Informational influence: two-sided reviews, source trustworthiness, source credibility, source homophily. Normative influence: e-retailer’s, recommendation, service popularity. | Context: Hong Kong International Airport. Sample: Passengers. Materials: Customer reviews about accommodation and restaurants. | Two-sided reviews are perceived as more helpful. Reviews from source expertise are perceived as more helpful. Reviews of service popularity are perceived as more helpful. | [17] |
The source-content- context model | Source factors: reviewer experience, reviewer expertise. Content factors: review extremity, review inconsistency, review depth. Context factors: product intangibility, product satisfaction, product popularity, product variety. | A total of 14 million review data across 10 product types were collected on Amazon.com. | Positive effects on review helpfulness: review extremity, review depth, reviewer expertise. Negative effects on review helpfulness: review inconsistency, product intangibility, product satisfaction, product popularity, product variety, reviewer experience. Review extremity and review depth determine review helpfulness, depending on product intangibility. | [74] |
2.2. Research Model and Hypothesis Development
2.2.1. Belief Consistency
2.2.2. Argument Quality
2.2.3. Positive and Negative Confirmation
2.2.4. Expertise
3. Methodology
3.1. The Instrument
3.2. Data Collection
4. Results
4.1. Reliability, Validity, and Common Method Variance
4.2. Hypothesis Testing
5. Discussion
5.1. Implications for Theory
5.2. Implications for Practice
6. Conclusions and Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. The Measurement Items
No. | Sentimental Messages |
---|---|
1 | “iPhone is so expensive and iOS is a close system. While Android is not difficult to use, and it has more freedom of options and no function difference compared with iPhone. Why so many people like iPhone, just because of lost in fashion?” |
2 | “I use Sony (mobile phone), whereas my younger brother uses iPhone. I feel the choice of mobile phones depends on your budget. I will choose Sony Xperia Active Sport because this type of mobile phone is unique with small panel. I choose it because of my personal needs in sport, and it provides anti-water, anti-dust, and anti-friction functions. Moreover, it supports with sport-used sensors, and also provides step-counting and music functions. I have no special needs in videos and games. The price indeed suits my personal needs. I never take iPhone into account.” |
3 | “I feel iPhone is more valuable because it provides effective and efficient APPs.” |
4 | “iPhone is easy to use, and so fashion and attractive. But its APPs are not so cheap.” |
Appendix B. Common Method Variance
References
- Brown, J.; Broderick, A.J.; Lee, N. Word of mouth communication within online communities: Conceptualizing the online social network. J. Interact. Mark. 2007, 21, 2–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, M.; Huang, L.; Tan, C.-H.; Wei, K.-K. Helpfulness of online product reviews as seen by consumers: Source and content features. Int. J. Electron. Commer. 2013, 17, 101–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Usó-Doménech, J.L.; Nescolarde-Selva, J. What are belief systems? Found. Sci. 2016, 21, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mudambi, S.M.; Schuff, D. What makes a helpful online review? A study of consumer reviews on Amazon.com. MIS Q. 2010, 34, 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Garnefeld, I.; Helm, S.; Grötschel, A.-K. May we buy your love? Psychological effects of incentives on writing likelihood and valence of online product reviews. Electron. Mark. 2020, 30, 805–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. Am. Psychol. 2003, 58, 697–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 5, 207–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 1974, 185, 1124–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chaxel, A.-S. How misinformation taints our belief system: A focus on belief updating and relational reasoning. J. Consum. Psychol. 2022, 32, 370–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frank, R. Success and Luck; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Weiner, D.S.; Guenther, C.L. Self-enhancement bias. In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020; pp. 4727–4733. [Google Scholar]
- Vedejová, D.; Cavojová, V. Confirmation bias in information search, interpretation, and memory recall: Evidence from reasoning about four controversial topics. Think. Reason. 2022, 28, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engin, A.; Vetschera, R. Optimistic overconfidence in electronic reverse auctions. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2019, 35, 100842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armstrong, J.S. Combining forecasts. In Principles of Forecasting—International Series in Operations Research & Management Science; Armstrong, J.S., Ed.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2001; Volume 30, pp. 417–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vul, E.; Pashler, H. Measuring the crowd within: Probabilistic representations within individuals. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 645–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jones, B.D. Bounded rationality. Annu. Rev. Political Sci. 1999, 2, 297–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Filieri, R.; McLeay, F.; Tsui, B.; Lin, Z. Consumer perceptions of information helpfulness and determinants of purchase intention in online consumer reviews of services. Inf. Manag. 2018, 55, 956–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skowronski, J.J.; Carlston, D.E. Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 105, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gauri, D.K.; Bhatnagar, A.; Rao, R. Role of word of mouth in online store loyalty. Commun. ACM 2008, 51, 89–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nam, K.; Baker, J.; Ahmad, N.; Goo, J. Determinants of writing positive and negative electronic word-of-mouth: Empirical evidence for two types of expectation confirmation. Decis. Support Syst. 2020, 129, 113168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Bratslavsky, E.; Finkenauer, C. Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2001, 5, 323–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pan, Y.; Zhang, J.Q. Born unequal: A study of the helpfulness of user-generated product reviews. J. Retail. 2011, 87, 598–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cao, Q.; Duan, W.; Gan, Q. Exploring determinants of voting for the “helpfulness” of online user reviews: A text mining approach. Decis. Support Syst. 2011, 50, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schindler, R.M.; Bickart, B. Perceived helpfulness of online consumer reviews: The role of message content and style. J. Consum. Behav. 2012, 11, 234–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Statista. Total Number of User Reviews and Opinions on Tripadvisor Worldwide from 2014 to 2021. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/684862/tripadvisor-number-of-reviews/ (accessed on 18 September 2022).
- Petty, R.E.; Wegener, D.T.; Fabrigar, L.R. Attitudes and attitude change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1997, 48, 609–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tam, K.Y.; Ho, S.Y. Web personalization as a persuasion strategy: An elaboration likelihood model perspective. Inf. Syst. Res. 2005, 16, 271–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Karimi, S.; Wang, F. Online review helpfulness: Impact of reviewer profile image. Decis. Support Syst. 2017, 96, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chua, A.Y.K.; Banerjee, S. Helpfulness of user-generated reviews as a function of review sentiment, product type and information quality. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 54, 547–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, J.J.; Reingen, P.H. Social ties and word-of-mouth referral behavior. J. Consum. Res. 1987, 14, 350–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nickerson, R.S. Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 175–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, D.; Mitra, S.; Zhang, H. When do consumers value positive vs. negative reviews? An empirical investigation of confirmation bias in online word-of-mouth. Inf. Syst. Res. 2016, 27, 131–144. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/24700140 (accessed on 2 February 2023). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gwebu, K.L.; Wang, J.; Zifla, E. Can warnings curb the spread of fake news? The interplay between warning, trust, and confirmation bias. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2022, 41, 3552–3573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon, H.A. Bounded rationality and organizational learning. Organ. Sci. 1991, 2, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liang, L.J.; Choi, H.S.C.; Joppe, M. Understanding repurchase intention of Airbnb consumers: Perceived authenticity, electronic word-of-mouth, and price sensitivity. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2018, 35, 73–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matute, J.; Polo-Redondo, Y.; Utrilas, A. The influence of EWOM characteristics on online repurchase intention: Mediating roles of trust and perceived usefulness. Online Inf. Rev. 2016, 40, 1090–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baek, H.; Ahn, J.; Choi, Y. Helpfulness of online consumer reviews: Readers’ objectives and review cues. Int. J. Electron. Commer. 2012, 17, 99–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, S.; Choeh, J.Y. The determinants of helpfulness of online reviews. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2016, 35, 853–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, J.; Konana, P.; Gu, B.; Kumar, A.; Raghunathan, R. Information valuation and confirmation bias in virtual communities: Evidence from stock message boards. Inf. Syst. Res. 2013, 24, 1050–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cai, H.; Yao, T.; Zhang, X. Confirmation bias in analysts’ response to consensus forecasts. J. Behav. Financ. 2022; in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhattacherjee, A.; Sanford, C. Influence processes for information technology acceptance: An elaboration likelihood model. MIS Q. 2006, 30, 805–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sussman, S.W.; Siegal, W.S. Informational influence in organizations: An integrated approach to knowledge adoption. Inf. Syst. Res. 2003, 14, 47–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance; Stanford University Press: Redwood, CA, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Xu, X.; Liu, J.; Cai, R. How do customers navigate perceived inappropriateness of collective emotion in group service recovery? An application of cognitive dissonance theory. Tour. Manag. 2022, 93, 104615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maegherman, E.; Ask, K.; Horselenberg, R.; van Koppen, P.J. Law and order effects: On cognitive dissonance and belief perseverance. Psychiatry Psychol. Law 2022, 29, 33–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garas, S.R.R.; Mahran, A.F.A.; Mohamed, H.M.H. Do you consider buying a counterfeit? New evidence from the theory of planned behavior and cognitive dissonance theory. J. Prod. Brand Manag. 2022; in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xi, W.; Baymuminova, N.; Zhang, Y.-W.; Xu, S.-N. Cognitive dissonance and public compliance, and their impact on business performance in hotel industry. Sustainability 2022, 14, 14907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wason, P.C. On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1960, 12, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wason, P.C. Reasoning about a rule. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1968, 20, 273–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwald, A.G.; Pratkanis, A.R.; Leippe, M.R.; Baumgardner, M.H. Under what conditions does theory obstruct research progress? Psychol. Rev. 1986, 93, 216–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, M.B.; Smallman, H.S. Human factors of the confirmation bias in intelligence analysis: Decision support from graphical evidence landscapes. Hum. Factors 2008, 50, 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palminteri, S.; Lebreton, M. The computational roots of positivity and confirmation biases in reinforcement learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 607–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pan, M.; Ryan, A. Investigating confirmation bias in transportation: An analysis of perceived health risk on public transit during the pandemic. J. Transp. Health 2022, 26, 101486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ask, K.; Granhag, P.A. Motivational sources of confirmation bias in criminal investigations: The need for cognitive closure. J. Investig. Psychol. Offender Profiling 2005, 2, 43–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonas, E.; Schulz-Hardt, S.; Frey, D.; Thelen, N. Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions: An expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 80, 557–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehner, P.E.; Adelman, L.; Cheikes, B.A.; Brown, M.J. Confirmation bias in complex analyses. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern.-Part A Syst. Hum. 2008, 38, 584–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKenzie, C.R.M. Increased sensitivity to differentially diagnostic answers using familiar materials: Implications for confirmation bias. Mem. Cogn. 2006, 34, 577–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Baddeley, A.; Eysenck, M.W.; Anderson, M.C. Memory; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, W.; Mazar, A.; Neal, D.T. Habits and goals in human behavior: Separate but interacting systems. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 17, 590–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arthur, W.B. Competing technologies, increasing returns, and lock-in by historical events. Econ. J. 1989, 99, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, W.M.; Levinthal, D.A. Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on learning and innovation. Adm. Sci. Q. 1990, 35, 128–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roedenbeck, M. Individual path dependency and social inertia: Facing the crudeness of sociology. J. Future Stud. 2011, 15, 25–44. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, K.; Kraemer, K.L.; Gurbaxani, V.; Xu, S.X. Migration to open-standard interorganizational systems: Network effects, switching costs, and path dependency. MIS Q. 2006, 30, 515–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bali, A.S.; He, A.J.; Ramesh, M. Health policy and COVID-19: Path dependency and trajectory. Policy Soc. 2022, 41, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goumagias, N.; Fernandes, K.J.; Nucciarelli, A.; Li, F. How to overcome path dependency through resource reconfiguration. J. Bus. Res. 2022, 145, 78–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McConnell, A.R.; Rydell, R.J.; Leibold, J.M. Expectations of consistency about the self: Consequences for self-concept formation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 38, 569–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Susskind, J.; Maurer, K.; Thakkar, V.; Hamilton, D.L.; Sherman, J.W. Perceiving individuals and groups: Expectancies, dispositional inferences, and causal attributions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bikhchandani, S.; Hirshleifer, D.; Welch, I. Learning from the behavior from others: Conformity, fads, and informational cascades. J. Econ. Perspect. 1998, 12, 151–170. Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=1288968 (accessed on 2 February 2023). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davern, M.; Shaft, T.; Te’eni, D. Cognition matters: Enduring questions in cognitive IS research. J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 2012, 13, 273–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kirs, P.J.; Pflughoeft, K.; Kroeck, G. A process model cognitive biasing effects in information systems development and usage. Inf. Manag. 2001, 38, 153–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Z.; Wang, X.; Wu, R. Is the devil in the details? Construal-level effects on perceived usefulness of online reviews for experience services. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2021, 36, 101033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guo, B.; Zhou, S. What makes population perception of review helpfulness: An information processing perspective. Electron. Commer. Res. 2017, 17, 585–608. Available online: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10660-016-9234-7 (accessed on 2 February 2023). [CrossRef]
- Paulus, D.; Fathi, R.; Fiedrich, F.; de Walle, B.V.; Comes, T. On the interplay of data and cognitive bias in crisis information management. Inf. Syst. Front. 2022; in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Choi, H.S.; Leon, S. An empirical investigation of online review helpfulness: A big data perspective. Decis. Support Syst. 2020, 139, 113403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, D.; Bond, S.D.; Zhang, H. Anxiety or angry? Effects of discrete emotions on the perceived helpfulness of online reviews. MIS Q. 2014, 38, 539–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ghose, A.; Ipeirotis, P.G. Estimating the helpfulness and economic impact of product reviews: Mining text and reviewer characteristics. IEEE Trans. Knowl. Data Eng. 2011, 23, 1498–1512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Samuelson, W.; Zeckhauser, R. Status quo bias in decision making. J. Risk Uncertain. 1988, 1, 7–59. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/41760530 (accessed on 2 February 2023). [CrossRef]
- Petty, R.E.; Cacioppo, J.T. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Berkowitz, L., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986; Volume 19, pp. 123–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynch, J.G., Jr.; Srull, T.K. Memory and attentional factors in consumer choice: Concepts and research methods. J. Consum. Res. 1982, 9, 18–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klayman, J.; Ha, Y.-W. Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychol. Rev. 1987, 94, 211–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Tariq, S.; Alvi, H. How primary and supplementary reviews affect consumer decision making? Roles of psychological and managerial mechanisms. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2021, 46, 101032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sussman, S.W.; Zhang, W. Capitalizing on content: Information adoption in two online communities. J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 2008, 9, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jin, H.; Yan, D.; Mehl, C.E.; Cui, W. An empirical grounded framework that evaluates argument quality in scientific and social contexts. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2021, 19, 681–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, M.; Huang, P. Assessing the product review helpfulness: Affective-cognitive evaluation and the moderating effect of feedback mechanism. Inf. Manag. 2020, 57, 103359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dibbets, P.; Meesters, C. Disconfirmation of confirmation bias: The influence of counter-attitudinal information. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 41, 2327–2333. Available online: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12144-020-00744-x (accessed on 2 February 2023). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cheung, M.Y.; Sia, C.L.; Kuan, K.Y. Is this review believable? A study of factors affecting the credibility of online consumer reviews from an ELM perspective. J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 2012, 13, 618–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Abedin, E.; Mendoza, A.; Karunasekera, S. Exploring the moderating role of readers’ perspective in evaluations of online consumer reviews. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 2021, 16, 3406–3424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anastasiei, B.; Dospinescu, N.; Dospinescu, O. Understanding the adoption of incentivized word-of-mouth in the online environment. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 2021, 16, 992–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pozharliev, R.; Rossi, D.; Angelis, M.D. Consumers’ self-reported and brain responses to advertising post on Instagram: The effect of number of followers and argument quality. Eur. J. Mark. 2022, 56, 922–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarz, N.; Bless, H.; Strack, F.; Klumpp, G.; Rittenauer-Schatka, H.; Simons, A. Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1991, 61, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bagozzi, R.P.; Dholakia, U.M. Intentional social action in virtual communities. J. Interact. Mark. 2002, 16, 2–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biswas, B.; Sengupta, P.; Ganguly, B. Your reviews or mine? Exploring the determinants of “perceived helpfulness” of online reviews: A cross-cultural study. Electron. Mark. 2022, 32, 1083–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, P.; Kuwabara, K.; Zeckhauser, R.; Friedman, E. Reputation systems. Commun. ACM 2000, 43, 45–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, J.Q.; Craciun, G.; Shin, D. When does electronic word-of-mouth matter? A study of consumer product reviews. J. Bus. Res. 2010, 63, 1336–1341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, N.; Pavlou, P.A.; Zhang, J. Overcoming the J-shaped distribution of product reviews. Commun. ACM 2009, 52, 144–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adaval, R. Sometimes it just feels right: The differential weighting of affect-consistent and affect-inconsistent product information. J. Consum. Res. 2001, 28, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Purnawirawan, N.; Dens, N.; De Pelsmacker, P. Balance and sequence in online reviews: The wrap effect. Int. J. Electron. Commer. 2012, 17, 71–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Babić Rosario, A.; de Valck, K.; Sotgiu, F. Conceptualizing the electronic word-of-mouth process: What we know and need to know about eWOM creation, exposure, and evaluation. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2020, 48, 422–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koc, E.; Yurur, S.; Ozsahin, M. Problem-solving abilities of managers: Inflated self-efficacy beliefs. J. Hosp. Tour. Insights, 2022; in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nunnally, J. Psychometric Theory, 2nd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogerson, P.A. Statistical Methods for Geography; Sage: London, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Chau, P.Y.K. An empirical assessment of a modified technology acceptance model. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 1996, 13, 185–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y. On the evaluation of structural equations models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1988, 16, 74–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheaton, B.; Muthen, B.; Alwin, D.F.; Summers, G.F. Assessing reliability and stability in panel models. Sociol. Methodol. 1977, 8, 84–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E. Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed.; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Tang, M.-C.; Wu, P.-M. Reconciling the effects of positive and negative electronic word of mouth: Roles of confirmation bias and involvement. Online Inf. Rev. 2022, 46, 114–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langer, E. The illusion of control. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 32, 311–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moloi, M.; Quaye, E.S.; Saini, Y.K. Evaluating key antecedents and consequences of the perceived helpfulness of online consumer reviews: A South African study. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2022, 54, 101172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weathers, D.; Swain, S.D.; Grover, V. Can online product reviews be more helpful? Examining characteristics of information content by product type. Decis. Support Syst. 2015, 79, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chou, Y.-C.; Chuang, H.-C.; Liang, T.-P. Elaboration likelihood model, endogenous quality indicators, and online review helpfulness. Decis. Support Syst. 2022, 153, 113683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emont, J.; Bürge, C. How scammers in China manipulate Amazon. Wall Str. J. Tech. 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Heinzman, A. How Fake Reviews Are Manipulating You Online. How-To Geek. 2019. Available online: https://www.howtogeek.com/407521/how-fake-reviews-are-manipulating-you-online/ (accessed on 2 February 2023).
- Nguyen, N. Fake reviews and inflated ratings are still a problem for Amazon. Wall Str. J. 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Wong, G. Fake orders brush up online sales in China. Wall Str. J. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Luo, L.; Duan, S.; Shang, S.; Pan, Y. What makes a helpful online review? Empirical evidence on the effects of review and reviewer characteristics. Online Inf. Rev. 2021, 45, 614–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lappas, T.; Sabnis, G.; Valkanas, G. The impact of fake reviews on online visibility: A vulnerability assessment of the hotel industry. Inf. Syst. Res. 2016, 27, 940–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Luca, M.; Zervas, G. Fake it till you make it: Reputation, competition, and Yelp review fraud. Manag. Sci. 2016, 62, 3412–3427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Birim, S.Q.; Kazancoglu, I.; Mangla, S.K.; Kahraman, A.; Kumar, S.; Kazancoglu, Y. Detecting fake reviews through topic modeling. J. Bus. Res. 2022, 149, 884–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayzlin, D.; Dover, Y.; Chevalier, J. Promotional reviews: An empirical investigation of online review manipulation. Am. Econ. Rev. 2014, 104, 2421–2455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moon, S.; Kim, M.-Y.; Iacobucci, D. Content analysis of fake consumer reviews by survey-based text categorization. Int. J. Res. Mark. 2021, 38, 343–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, S.; Lee, S.; Baek, H. Does the dispersion of online review ratings affect review helpfulness? Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 117, 106670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Podsakoff, P.M.; Organ, D.W. Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and prospects. J. Manag. 1986, 12, 531–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y.; Phillips, L. Assessing construct validity in organizational research. Adm. Sci. Q. 1991, 36, 421–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindell, M.K.; Whitney, D.J. Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research design. J. Appl. Psychol. 2001, 86, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Item | AQ | E | NC | PC | BC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
AQ1 | 0.817 | 0.112 | 0.238 | 0.237 | 0.101 |
AQ2 | 0.827 | 0.078 | 0.194 | 0.204 | 0.146 |
AQ3 | 0.827 | 0.073 | 0.202 | 0.209 | 0.227 |
AQ4 | 0.766 | 0.086 | 0.139 | 0.216 | 0.207 |
E1 | 0.140 | 0.874 | 0.005 | 0.121 | 0.119 |
E2 | 0.037 | 0.933 | 0.027 | 0.141 | 0.034 |
E3 | 0.094 | 0.933 | 0.027 | 0.127 | 0.028 |
NC1 | 0.220 | 0.036 | 0.890 | 0.109 | 0.066 |
NC2 | 0.181 | 0.020 | 0.895 | 0.106 | 0.143 |
NC3 | 0.203 | 0.007 | 0.889 | 0.119 | 0.130 |
PC1 | 0.311 | 0.119 | 0.121 | 0.839 | 0.153 |
PC2 | 0.208 | 0.154 | 0.143 | 0.884 | 0.094 |
PC3 | 0.246 | 0.183 | 0.100 | 0.871 | 0.142 |
BC2 | 0.449 | 0.267 | 0.182 | 0.226 | 0.621 |
BC3 | 0.329 | 0.033 | 0.221 | 0.203 | 0.823 |
Eigenvalue | 3.286 | 2.682 | 2.667 | 2.612 | 1.298 |
Cumulative Variance (%) | 21.906 | 39.787 | 57.564 | 74.978 | 83.633 |
Construct | Mean | SD | AQ | E | NC | PC | BC | Cronbach Alpha | Composite Reliability |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AQ | 3.32 | 0.77 | 0.810 | 0.90 | 0.93 | ||||
E | 3.16 | 0.88 | 0.327 | 0.914 | 0.92 | 0.93 | |||
NC | 3.41 | 0.80 | 0.409 | 0.134 | 0.891 | 0.92 | 0.94 | ||
PC | 3.49 | 0.78 | 0.532 | 0.368 | 0.299 | 0.865 | 0.92 | 0.94 | |
BC | 3.49 | 0.66 | 0.617 | 0.373 | 0.387 | 0.460 | 0.729 | 0.74 | 0.84 |
RH | 6.88 | 1.84 | 0.467 | 0.228 | 0.234 | 0.383 | 0.367 | none | none |
Goodness-of-Fit Indexes | Recommended Threshold | Measurement Model | Structural Model | Moderation (Expertise) |
---|---|---|---|---|
χ2/df | ≤3.00 | 1.781 | 2.259 | 1.296 |
GFI | ≥0.90 | 0.948 | 0.995 | 0.992 |
AGFI | ≥0.80 | 0.923 | 0.960 | 0.969 |
NFI | ≥0.90 | 0.964 | 0.992 | 0.986 |
CFI | ≥0.95 | 0.984 | 0.995 | 0.997 |
SRMR | ≥0.05 | 0.033 | 0.025 | 0.023 |
RMSEA | ≥0.08 | 0.048 | 0.061 | 0.030 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Shih, H.-P.; Lai, K.-h.; Cheng, T.C.E. Complied by Belief Consistency: The Cognitive-Information Lens of User-Generated Persuasion. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 2023, 18, 372-393. https://doi.org/10.3390/jtaer18010020
Shih H-P, Lai K-h, Cheng TCE. Complied by Belief Consistency: The Cognitive-Information Lens of User-Generated Persuasion. Journal of Theoretical and Applied Electronic Commerce Research. 2023; 18(1):372-393. https://doi.org/10.3390/jtaer18010020
Chicago/Turabian StyleShih, Hung-Pin, Kee-hung Lai, and T. C. E. Cheng. 2023. "Complied by Belief Consistency: The Cognitive-Information Lens of User-Generated Persuasion" Journal of Theoretical and Applied Electronic Commerce Research 18, no. 1: 372-393. https://doi.org/10.3390/jtaer18010020
APA StyleShih, H. -P., Lai, K. -h., & Cheng, T. C. E. (2023). Complied by Belief Consistency: The Cognitive-Information Lens of User-Generated Persuasion. Journal of Theoretical and Applied Electronic Commerce Research, 18(1), 372-393. https://doi.org/10.3390/jtaer18010020