1. Introduction
The gender productivity gap in academia, well known as the ‘productivity puzzle’—that on average men publish more papers than women—still persists across the countries, but the trend is ramping down over time and the gap considerably varies between different subfields of science [
1,
2]. Many theories explain the gender productivity difference ranging from family responsibilities [
3,
4] to career absences [
5], to mention a few.
In contrast to the OECD finding that there are more female than male undergraduate and graduate students in most countries [
6], there are relatively few female than male full professors in many countries. According to the US National Science Foundation, female scientists earn almost half the PhDs in science and engineering (STEM fields) in the US, but comprise only 21% of full science professors and surprisingly only 5% of full professors in engineering [
7]. There is persisting gender inequality in hiring [
8], female scientists continue to face discrimination in earnings [
7], funding [
9], patenting [
10], prizes and awards [
11], and grant applications [
12]. Moreover, female scientists publish significantly fewer papers in areas in which research is expensive, and it is much less likely that females are listed as either first or last author on a paper [
2]. Holman, Stuart-Fox, and Hauser estimate that men are invited by journals to submit papers at approximately double the rate of women [
13]. Analyses accomplished in the Quebec region reported that after women have passed the age of about 38, they receive less funding for research than men, and are at a slight disadvantage in terms of the scientific impact (measured by citations) of their publications [
14]. However, a few papers reported that the gender gap is diminishing [
1,
15], e.g., Xie and Shauman reported that the gender differences in research productivity declined overtime with the female-to-male ratio increasing from about 60 percent in the late 1960s to 75 to 80 percent in the late 1980s and early 1990s [
15]. Le Moine shows that the concentration of women among researchers who publish a single article is greater than for men, while their representation among ‘star’ scientists is less [
16].
How children and marriage affect men and women is not conclusive. Most studies report the positive effect of marriage on scientific productivity, but Prpic [
17] shows that men more than women experience benefitsdue to the presence of a spouse. Fox [
3] reports that unmarried men are the least productive of all. Married women, particularly those married for the second or third time, exhibit a higher level of productivity. Stack shows that women with preschool-aged children publish less than other women [
18] . For example, the divorce rate among tenured females is 50% higher than that of tenured men [
19]. Tenured female scientists are almost three times more likely than male colleagues to be single without children [
20]. Obviously, the time, energy, and money devoted to raising children can reduce time devoted to science. Due to thinking that raising kids is more a female responsibility, men with children make them more productive than women with children [
17]. Moreover, female postdocs who plan or become parents decide to abandon research careers up to twice as often as men [
21], confirming that children and marriages do affect carriers in science, but not equally males and females.
In terms of citations, it was revealed that the smaller number of citations received by females is first because on average, men like to cite their own papers 56% more than women [
22]. Second, females generally publish less [
1]. However, on average, papers written by females receive more citations than papers written by males [
23], implying that women often have a higher impact (citations) per publication [
24,
25,
26]. Duch et al. hypothesized and confirmed that the higher the resource requirements on research, the greater is the difference in the publication rates between females and males in favor of males. However, the gender differences in publication rate and citations are discipline-specific [
26].
To stress gender differences across different research areas, for the Italian academia the authors showed that females are more productive than male colleagues in medical sciences, agriculture, veterinary sciences, and earth sciences, while men dominate in industrial and information engineering, chemical sciences, physical sciences, and mathematics and information sciences [
27]. Abramo et al. concluded that there is a significant number of scientific fields where women’s performance cannot be considered to be inferior compared to men’s.
2. Materials and Methods
To test whether the male and female researchers comprise two distinct subgroups and not a unique, in our paper we apply the Mann–Whitney U test that is intended to measure the difference between two populations, in our analyses the difference in ranks of male and female citations. Generally, when the test is applied in practice, the total citations data (males and females collected together) must be first sorted in ascending order. Combining all citations values in a single array, but keeping information of which sample each observation comes from, the U test first ranks all scientists according to their citations from smallest to largest and then separately sums up all female and male ranks, thus, taking the gender into account. We denote these sums by
and
, whereby
and
we denote the respective sample sizes, in our case representing the numbers of male and female researchers. Since the the test is conceived to incorporate both citations and the sample size which is the number of professors of a particular gender, the test is actually designed to evaluate citations per capita of a given gender. The test statistic quantifying the difference between the rank sums is defined as:
or
When the two-tail test is applied, the two-tail test statistic
U is taken to be the smaller of
and
.
The distribution of U test is symmetrical where a mean and variance equal
If
and
are at least equal to 8, the distribution of
U is approximately Gaussian so that
is standardized Gaussian distributed with mean zero and variance 1.
Here, the null hypothesis is that the distributions of citations per capita of the two subgroups (men and women) are the same. The null hypothesis is rejected for values of the test statistic falling into either tail of its sampling distribution. If a one-tailed test is performed, the alternative hypothesis suggests that the variable of one group is larger than the other group.
4. Discussion and Conclusions
At the largest Croatian University, applying the Mann–Whitney test, we demonstrate no gender difference in number of citations except for seven faculties, where males are significantly better than females on six faculties. We report that female STEM full professors are significantly more cited than their male colleagues, while male non-STEM assistant professors are significantly more cited than their female colleagues. In
Table 11 as a comparison between male and female researchers, we report the average citations across different University members. In contrast to the US and many Western nations, at the largest Croatian University there are several faculties where the most cited researcher is woman and these faculties are the following, both STEM and non-STEM: Faculty of Chemical Engineering and Technology, Faculty of Economics and Business, Faculty of Food Technology and Biotechnology, Faculty of Geotechnical Engineering, Faculty of Graphic Arts, Faculty of Kinesiology, Faculty of Organization and Informatics, Faculty of Political science, Faculty of Science-Biology, Faculty of Science-Geography, and Faculty of Teacher Education. Thus, at individual level, at some particular faculties and scientific fields, the most cited female is better off than the most cited male. Moreover, at collective level, there are ten faculties where females have the larger average citations than their male colleagues: Faculty of Chemical Engineering, Faculty of Economics, Faculty of Food Technology, Faculty of Forestry, Faculty of Geotechnical Engineering, Faculty of Graphical Arts, Faculty of Organization and Informatics, Faculty of Political science, Faculty of Science-Physics, and Faculty of Dental Medicine.
Wealthy countries such as Japan, Germany, and Switzerland has fewer women authors than poorer ones [
13]. Besides this asymmetry, there is a significant difference in gender productivity between the US and the poorer countries. However, would it not be reasonable to assume that the more developed a country, the smaller the gender differences in their performance? Partially the gender difference in the US academia appears because in the US, in contrast to the EU, the large majority of universities are private and the private educational system recognizes only quality and does not care much about gender or even racial equality. Comparing the US and developing countries, due to globalization where the US is the attractor of best students all over the world, the level of competition in the US academia is much higher than in any other country. Therefore, females to compete with males in the US academia must compete not only with the best US males, but with the most brilliant world candidates. Since raising a family is even in the US still considered to be more female than male responsibility, being a female makes their position much harder. Clearly, if a female is a genius like Nobel Prize Laureate Marie Curie, she can achieve excellent academic results in parallel with being an excellent mother and even raise her daughter to become another Nobel Prize Laureate. However, there are not so many geniuses of that kind. In large educational systems such as the US, fluctuations are substantially smaller than in small countries such as Croatia, and so events much different than expectations are more likely.
Briefly, we expect that in a limit where children are equally educated regardless of gender and where family obligations are equally distributed among both parents, it is meaningful that men and women should perform equal results, not only in science. The closer a society to this limit, the larger the similarity in their performance. It is a challenge to test this obvious hypothesis. To this end, in academia, the gender research productivity gap will not diminish without substantial reforms in education, mentoring, and academic publishing.
For a country to be competitive in both science and business, in agreement with Cobb-Douglas production function extended for human capital, maximization of its intellectual capital particularly its females’ part is a top country’s priority. In the short run, policymakers should launch new programs required to stimulate international collaboration for female researchers, because international collaboration is one of the pillars for excellence in science. We suggest that the best female researchers should generally receive more grants and funding than their best male colleagues as compensation for spending more time in raising a family. The pay gap in science should become an illegal practice. In the grant review process, gender equality among reviewers should be always respected, and if not, the call should be canceled. We even suggest that kindergartens should be open at least within large Universities and institutes. In long run, we need to rapidly boost the interest in science and particularly STEM disciplines among the girls.