Working Memory Training: Assessing the Efficiency of Mnemonic Strategies
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- We carefully chose WM tasks which require the participants to perform an operation (as by definition of WM tasks), but explicitly looking for an operation that requires as little computational power as possible. It is in fact hard to assess how many resources the participant needs to employ in order to perform a simple computation, as required for a complex span task. We then decided for a perturbative approach, where the requirement of executive control processing is devoted to the random retrieval order: the participant is asked to be able to navigate across the formed memories.
- The training of the experimental group consists of “classic” mnemonic strategies. These strategies did not wait, of course, for the formulation of the concept of WM: they are much more ancient. Among their most prominent promoters, in fact, we find the Roman orator Cicero (106 b.C.–43 b.C.), we find them mentioned even in older Greek treatises and, within the more recent literature, they represent the main topic of “Moonwalking with Einstein” [17], a book describing Joshua Foer’s journey from being a journalist to winning the international memory competition, confirming that their efficacy is well demonstrated in several contexts. These techniques include, in particular, the “method of loci” and the “memory palace”. Even though the transmission of these strategies throughout the centuries confirms per se their value, to the best of our knowledge they have never been tested in a lab setting with naïve participants. One of us, VdL, is a professional mnemonist who relies on these strategies for his own on-stage performance, and he participated in this study by offering 2 h of mnemonic strategy training to our experimental subject group.
- The training is kept short and the possibility to practice the freshly learnt strategy is very limited. Obviously, professional mnemonists dedicate a lot of time to the practice of these techniques. This is confirmed by Foer’s description of his experience [17] and by VdL as well. In their case, in fact, the outstanding performances achieved are to be imputed both to the use of successful strategies and to the amount of practice that they devote to improving a specific skill set. In our setup, the training being limited to one short session, possible differences between the control and the experimental group are likely to be due to the use of a better strategy.
- The WM span is usually considered largely domain-independent [6]. This was an important factor in our design: in our experiment, we included different types of material, in order to check whether the same training can benefit different tasks or, in other words, whether the newly learnt strategy could be generalized to be used on different items.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Experimental Procedure
2.3. N-Back
2.4. Seminar
2.5. Memory Tests
3. Results
3.1. N-Back
3.2. Afternoon Tests
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- arancia {orange}
- banana {banana}
- bicchiere {glass}
- birra {beer}
- bomba {bomb}
- bruco {caterpillar}
- canoa {canoe}
- carrello {shopping cart}
- cesto {basket}
- chiave {key}
- cocco {coconut}
- elefante {elephant}
- fiore {flower}
- foglie {leaves}
- fragola {strawberry}
- fungo {mushroom}
- fuoco {fire}
- gallina {hen}
- gatto {cat}
- gelato {ice cream}
- giornale {newspaper}
- guanti {gloves}
- limone {lemon}
- mappa {map}
- martello {hammer}
- mattoni {bricks}
- miele {honey}
- mucca {cow}
- palla {ball}
- palma {palm tree}
- panda {panda}
- panino {sandwich}
- pasta {pasta}
- patata {potato}
- pennello {paint brush}
- pesce {fish}
- pianta {plant}
- pizza {pizza}
- ragno {spider}
- sale {salt}
- sapone {soap}
- scarpa {shoe}
- serpente {snake}
- stella {star}
- torta {cake}
- trofeo {cup}
- valigia {luggage}
- vino {wine}
References
- Baddeley, A.D.; Hitch, G. Working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. Adv. Res. Theory 1974, 8, 47–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, N. An Embedded-Processes Model of Working Memory. In Models Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control; Miyake, A., Shah, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 62–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Shah, P. Models of Working Memory: An Introduction. In Models Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control; Miyake, A., Shah, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daneman, M.; Carpenter, P.A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980, 19, 450–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shipstead, Z.; Redick, T.S.; Engle, R.W. Is working memory training effective? Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 628–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kane, M.; Tuholski, S.; Hambrick, D.; Wilhelm, O.; Payne, T.; Engle, R. The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. J. Exp. Psychol. 2004, 133, 189–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bühner, M.; König, C.J.; Pick, M.; Krumm, S. Working memory dimensions as differential predictors of the speed and error aspect of multitasking performance. Hum. Perform. 2006, 19, 253–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salminen, T.; Strobach, T.; Schubert, T. On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ishkhanyan, B.; Boye, K.; Mogensen, J. The Meeting Point: Where Language Production and Working Memory Share Resources. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2019, 48, 61–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Daneman, M.; Merikle, P.M. Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1996, 3, 422–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jaeggi, S.M.; Buschkuehl, M.; Jonides, J.; Perrig, W.J. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 6829–6833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jaeggi, S.M.; Studer-Luethi, B.; Buschkuehl, M.; Su, Y.F.; Jonides, J.; Perrig, W.J. The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning—Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence 2010, 38, 625–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinussen, R.; Hayden, J.; Hogg-Johnson, S.; Tannock, R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2005, 44, 377–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alloway, T.P. Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2009, 25, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Bastian, C.C.; Oberauer, K. Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychol. Res. 2014, 78, 803–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Donolato, E.; Giofrè, D.; Mammarella, I.C. Differences in verbal and visuospatial forward and backward order recall: A review of the literature. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Foer, J. Moonwalking with Einstein. The Art and Science of Remembering Everything; Penguin Books: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 1956, 63, 343–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Crepaldi, D.; Amenta, S.; Mandera, P.; Keuleers, E.; Brysbaert, M. SUBTLEX-IT. Subtitle-based word frequency estimates for Italian. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Italian Association for Experimental Psychology, Rovereto, Italy, 10–12 September 2015; Available online: https://lrlac.sissa.it/publications/frequency-estimates-different-registers-explain-different-aspects-visual-word (accessed on 15 July 2019).
- Joypixels. Available online: www.joypixels.com (accessed on 1 August 2019).
- Murdoch, B.B.; Lissner, E.; Marvin, C. The serial position effect of free recall. J. Exp. Psychol. 1962, 64, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Di Santo, S.; De Luca, V.; Isaja, A.; Andreetta, S. Working Memory Training: Assessing the Efficiency of Mnemonic Strategies. Entropy 2020, 22, 577. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22050577
Di Santo S, De Luca V, Isaja A, Andreetta S. Working Memory Training: Assessing the Efficiency of Mnemonic Strategies. Entropy. 2020; 22(5):577. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22050577
Chicago/Turabian StyleDi Santo, Serena, Vanni De Luca, Alessio Isaja, and Sara Andreetta. 2020. "Working Memory Training: Assessing the Efficiency of Mnemonic Strategies" Entropy 22, no. 5: 577. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22050577
APA StyleDi Santo, S., De Luca, V., Isaja, A., & Andreetta, S. (2020). Working Memory Training: Assessing the Efficiency of Mnemonic Strategies. Entropy, 22(5), 577. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22050577