Next Article in Journal
A Stable Large-Scale Multiobjective Optimization Algorithm with Two Alternative Optimization Methods
Previous Article in Journal
Variational Hybrid Monte Carlo for Efficient Multi-Modal Data Sampling
Previous Article in Special Issue
Design of DNA Storage Coding with Enhanced Constraints
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Entropy and Fractal Techniques for Monitoring Fish Behaviour and Welfare in Aquacultural Precision Fish Farming—A Review

by
Harkaitz Eguiraun
1,2,* and
Iciar Martinez
2,3,4
1
Department of Graphic Design & Engineering Projects, Faculty of Engineering in Bilbao, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, 48013 Bilbao, Bizkaia, Spain
2
Research Center for Experimental Marine Biology and Biotechnology—Plentziako Itsas Estazioa (PiE-UPV/EHU), University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), 48620 Plentzia, Bizkaia, Spain
3
Department of Zoology and Animal Cell Biology, Faculty of Science and Technology, University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), 48940 Leioa, Bizkaia, Spain
4
IKERBASQUE, Basque Foundation for Science, 48009 Bilbao, Bizkaia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Entropy 2023, 25(4), 559; https://doi.org/10.3390/e25040559
Submission received: 27 January 2023 / Revised: 19 March 2023 / Accepted: 22 March 2023 / Published: 24 March 2023

Abstract

:
In a non-linear system, such as a biological system, the change of the output (e.g., behaviour) is not proportional to the change of the input (e.g., exposure to stressors). In addition, biological systems also change over time, i.e., they are dynamic. Non-linear dynamical analyses of biological systems have revealed hidden structures and patterns of behaviour that are not discernible by classical methods. Entropy analyses can quantify their degree of predictability and the directionality of individual interactions, while fractal dimension (FD) analyses can expose patterns of behaviour within apparently random ones. The incorporation of these techniques into the architecture of precision fish farming (PFF) and intelligent aquaculture (IA) is becoming increasingly necessary to understand and predict the evolution of the status of farmed fish. This review summarizes recent works on the application of entropy and FD techniques to selected individual and collective fish behaviours influenced by the number of fish, tagging, pain, preying/feed search, fear/anxiety (and its modulation) and positive emotional contagion (the social contagion of positive emotions). Furthermore, it presents an investigation of collective and individual interactions in shoals, an exposure of the dynamics of inter-individual relationships and hierarchies, and the identification of individuals in groups. While most of the works have been carried out using model species, we believe that they have clear applications in PFF. The review ends by describing some of the major challenges in the field, two of which are, unsurprisingly, the acquisition of high-quality, reliable raw data and the construction of large, reliable databases of non-linear behavioural data for different species and farming conditions.

1. Introduction

Aquaculture is expected to play a key role in supplying high-value protein and micronutrients [1] to a human population estimated to reach about 9700 million by 2050 (United Nations World Population Prospects https://population.un.org/wpp/, accessed on 21 March 2023). Indeed, in Asia and some African countries, seafood already makes up over 50% of the animal protein in their diets [1]. Although most seafood is expected to originate from aquaculture [1], the fish farming industry faces some serious challenges, including climate change, environmental contaminants, and the need to ensure fish health and welfare, which will require the development of alternative disease treatments and the identification of novel sources of feed and nutrients [2]. In response to these challenges, a paradigm shift is mandatory [3]; thus, the European aquaculture industry [2] and European policy [4] are targeting green and resilient production systems and emerging technologies such as Environmental Intelligence and Monitoring Systems, both of which include Precision Farming as a key area [5]. Føre et al. [6] introduced the concept of Precision Fish Farming ((PF) which is comparable to Intelligent Aquaculture (IA)) to apply control-engineering principles to improve farmers’ ability to monitor, control, and document farm production. PFF consists of several cyclical operational processes performed in four phases [6]: Phases 1 and 2 constitute the observation and interpretation, respectively, of fishes’ responses, which form the basis for Phase 3, decision making, which, in turn, governs the actions implemented in Phase 4.
Observation requires the deployment of sensors and the implementation of monitoring methods, which were recently reviewed by [6,7]. The most common monitoring methods are based on the analysis of video images, while newer ones target sound/acoustic signals [8,9,10]. However, the sensors themselves may influence an animal’s behaviour; for instance, acoustic telemetry requires tagging [9,11], sonar affects a fish’s hearing [12], and the presence of robots or divers’ videorecording also alter fishes’ behaviour [13,14]. Fixed sensors interfere the least, but they can only record fish within a certain region. Phase 2, which is addressed in the present review, deals with the handling and interpretation of the large amount of data generated by automatic monitoring. However, the automatic interpretation of the data demands the application of machine learning, artificial intelligence (AI) algorithms, and decision support systems, whose addressal is outside the scope of the present work (see reviews by [7,15,16,17,18,19]). Phase 3 also benefits from AI algorithms and decision support systems, and both Phases 3 and 4 (implementation) will most likely ultimately rest on the farmer’s experience, and it will, in all probability, be the farmer, and not an automated system, who will make the ultimate decisions.
Understanding the status of fish and detecting the presence of stressors is important to improve animal welfare and optimize production yield and quality. Intensive fish farming includes all life stages from the broodstock to the marketable fish. The hatchery phase usually takes place indoors (and under completely controlled conditions), while the ongrowing phase usually occurs in recirculating aquaculture systems (RAS) or outdoor ponds or cages. Outdoor rearing exposes fish to predators, variable environmental conditions, contaminants (heavy metals, pesticides, drugs, etc.), diseases and infections (including parasites, bacterial and viral infections), micro- and nanoplastics, and anthropogenic noise pollution, which have all been shown to alter fish behaviour [20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32]. Changes in aquatic organisms’ behaviour have been proposed to serve as Biological Early Warning Systems (BEWS) to monitor both the presence of environmental contaminants in water resources [31,33,34] and the fish production in aquaculture [3,6,23,35]. We have recently reviewed the potential of entropy and fractal dimension (FD) analyses of individual and collective fish behaviours to establish Biological Early Warning Systems for the presence of environmental contaminants (Eguiraun and Martinez, submitted); therefore, it will not be addressed in the present work.
Unfortunately, there are numerous additional stressors that remain unknown or that are not easily measurable, such as social interactions. The existence of collective behaviour and potential social stressors must also be considered in both experimental settings and on farms. Fish have been shown to be truly social creatures with social structures and dominance hierarchies whose very complex and largely unknown formation [36] needs to be mapped on real-life farms to achieve optimal production. The fact that social structures and hierarchies under real farming conditions are largely unknown due to the difficulty of their documentation does not mean that they are irrelevant or that they do not influence the health, welfare, and other phenotypical/quality aspects of production. Methodologies for the fast detection of abnormal fish behaviours, which may be critical for the early detection of deficiencies during farming, have already been successfully tested applying a convolution 3D deep (C3D) network model optimized by cross-entropy loss to analyse real-life aquaculture video surveillance [37].
Laboratory studies with zebrafish have shown that the organizational patterns of dominance hierarchies are heavily influenced by individual interactions, indicating that fish systems are both self-structuring and self-organizing and that the evolution of behaviour in the formation of a dominance hierarchy is influenced by networks of individuals rather than independent interacting pairs of individuals ([38]; see [39] for a more general discussion on swarm behaviours). The formation of hierarchies is particularly interesting because it has been shown that too few fish in such a system (under about 15 individuals) will alter the normal behaviour of naturally gregarious species as shown by the system’s Shannon Entropy (SE) [35]. Studying the connectiveness of swarm behaviour, which can also be applied to fish shoals, Komareji et al. [39] identified a relationship between the size of the swarm/shoal (the number of individuals) and the number of topological nearest neighbours influencing any individual’s behaviour and dynamics. Importantly, the connectedness of the swarm and the structural properties of the swarm network are generally not constant, which is relevant if we wish to monitor the collective behaviour of fish on farms. A second important point is that the probability of the entire swarm being connected is a function of both the number of individuals in the swarm and the number of interacting nearest neighbours; thus, the larger the number of individuals, the larger the number of interacting nearest neighbours needed for the swarm to display a dynamic collective behaviour [39]. This means that to obtain information about production based on the collective behaviour of the fish system, the farming conditions must allow for the interaction (albeit not the necessarily direct interaction) of all the fish in the cage. In addition, the available space for the fish to swim must be proportional to the number of fish; for example, the interactions that occur when there are too few fish in a large cage may hinder the development of collective behaviour.
Anthropogenic noise pollution is a less-studied source of stress for fish that negatively affects the physiology and behaviour of individuals as it induces temporary or permanent hearing loss, stress and behavioural reactions to noise [32], and changes in shoaling behaviour (discussed in the study by [25] and references therein). In addition, anthropogenic noise seems to increase the number of fish killed by predation [40]. The identification of stressful noises requires the use of passive acoustic monitoring devices to first identify the normal noises produced by the monitored species, followed by the noises precipitating the stress and, finally, the noises the fish produce in response to the stress. This approach is still in its infancy.

1.1. Applications of Entropy and Fractal Analyses to Fish Behaviour Studies

The interactions between individuals, including knowledge regarding who leads, i.e., “who follows whom”, is important in social species and requires the quantification of the direction of information flow between individuals. Transfer entropy (TE) is a method that is commonly used to identify the flow of information and thus detect leaders both in interactions between pairs of individuals and in groups. Zebrafish have often been used as a model system in studies using either real fish, robotic replicas, and/or mathematical models based on its behaviour [41]. However, it must be borne in mind that many of these studies are performed within a narrow time-window and assume that the leaders are consistent over time. Yet, this may not be the case since individuals in interacting pairs have been shown to swap roles occasionally and adjust their responses to one another as they exchange roles [42].
Non-linear dynamical analyses are becoming increasingly relevant for understanding biological systems. For instance, apparently random social and behavioural patterns in several species’ activities, including the swimming patterns of fish [43,44,45], have been shown to contain highly non-random components of a fractal nature (see the research conducted by [46] and references therein), i.e., they display self-similarity [47]. Likewise, the predictability of a system can be described by, for instance, Rényi, Shannon or Kolmogorov entropy measurements [48], while the interactions among its components have been described mostly by TE [49], which has been successfully used to identify leaders in interactions [50]. Mann and Garnett [51] postulated the causal entropic principle, according to which individuals’ behaviours in a group tend to maximize the entropy of the entire system. The causal entropic principle was indeed able to predict many social interactions in animal groups, including humans. According to these works, optimally functional biological systems should maximize the entropy values, and it follows that stressors should diminish them.
However, the reader must understand that non-linear analyses of fish behaviours are not substitutes for classical methods. Rather, they must be considered providers of valuable additional information that, as mentioned above, classical analyses do not always readily expose.

1.2. Aim of the Work

By applying mathematical models and novel linear and non-linear algorithms [52,53], it is possible to extract simplified, practical information from complex biological systems (such as fish in aquaculture and in the wild), which inherently contain a large number of stochastic and deterministic components [54]. Two often used such measurements are the entropy of the system, i.e., the degree of predictability/chaoticity of the system [48,51,55], and its FD, which is a characteristic of fractal structures describing their complexity [43,44,45,56,57,58,59]. Indeed, Alados et al. [60] postulated that some exploratory biological structures and behavioural patterns have been naturally selected towards increasing complexity. Since stress increases an organism’s metabolic rate and energy consumption, it should provoke a consequent reduction in the complexity of exploratory behaviour even though humans may not be able to perceive it by eye. However, the FD of the structures and behavioural patterns, as a measure of their complexity, may serve as stress indicators and allow for the quantification of changes in behaviour. The authors demonstrated that the FD values of some behavioural complexity patterns in goats (Sarcoptes scabieis) (e.g., head lifts, feeding gaps, and vigilance behaviour) decreased with the two kinds of stress tested: pregnancy and parasitic infection [60].
There is also an increasing wealth of research devoted to the development of deep learning methods and neural network classification techniques using sophisticated mathematical models that will undoubtably find applications in IA and PFF (for further details, see a recent review by [7]). Many of these approaches use the entropy of the raw data (including that of images [61]) to improve the accuracy of a model. Feeding status is often selected as a targeted variable of clear interest for farmers [62,63]. As mentioned above, these applications have not been included in the present review.
The purpose of this work is to provide an overview of recent publications indicating the potential of entropy and FD analyses of fish behaviours to provide relevant information to farmers when integrated into an PFF/IA architecture, particularly with respect to the early detection of stress and critical behaviours (Figure 1).

2. Methodology Followed for the Review

Initially, the Web of Science (WoS) and Scopus databases were used for the bibliographic searches, but the WoS rendered a very large number of results, most of which were not relevant for our purposes. Therefore, the searches were limited to Scopus. The search strings and number of documents found (updated on the 26 January 2023) are listed in Table 1. Reviewing all the documents, we found only 37 dealing with the exact subject of our study, namely, relevant individual or collective behaviours whose FD and/or entropy analyses have the potential to provide relevant information applicable in a PFF/IA framework. A summary of the contents of those publications is presented in Appendix A.

3. Targeted Applications

Appendix A displays a summary of the contents of the 37 selected publications, including the most relevant data and results. The works have been classified according to the investigated subject and the targeted applications addressing the following topics: individual identification in groups; preying/search behaviours in larvae and fish; feeding status; collective behaviour, including the effect of the number of fish, individual interactions, hierarchies, and collective behaviour in shoals of mixed species; effect of tagging and pain; fear/anxiety responses to predators; modulation of fear/anxiety and positive emotional contagion.

3.1. Individual Identification in Groups

Neumeister et al. [46] were able to distinguish between the swimming trajectories of individual goldfish within groups using the discriminant analysis of six variables, consisting of the mean velocity and five nonlinear measures, namely, the characteristic FD, the Richardson dimension (DR), the Lempel–Ziv complexity, the Hurst exponent (HE) and the relative dispersion (the reader should refer to [46] and references to the original works describing the parameters for further information). In addition, although not apparently distinguishable, the swimming patterns were rather complex, and each fish displayed highly individual and disparate swimming profiles. No single measure was “the most effective”, although the nonlinear measures were more effective than the mean velocity, and the most effective measures were HE and DR.

3.2. Preying/Feeding Search Behaviour

3.2.1. Larvae

Coughlin et al. [64] published the first work applying fractal analysis to the swimming and preying behavioural patterns of clownfish larvae, which was conducted because traditional methods did not differentiate foraging modes appropriately. Both the age of the larvae and the amount of available prey influenced the swimming and search patterns: the larvae showed highly variable, complex swimming paths during the first two days after hatching as reflected in their higher FD, which decreased by the third day after the start of active feeding, thus denoting a trend toward simpler, more linear paths. Different search patterns were followed by the larvae after the onset of feeding: straighter (with the lowest FD) and intricate (higher FD) paths in low- and high-prey-density media, respectively, were observed.
Mahjoub et al. [65] also studied the prey search behaviour of malabar grouper, Epinephelus malabaricus, larvae via the 3D recording of their swimming behaviour in the absence and presence of prey. FD analyses of the swimming projections in the three axes (XZ, XY, and YZ) revealed anisotropy in the changes of the search pattern induced by the addition of prey. Without prey, the FD analyses indicated increased activity on the vertical axis but, in the presence of prey, the complexities in the vertical and horizontal axes were similar. This indicates an optimization of the search volume by the larvae and, therefore, the need to consider 3D search behaviour in further studies. It is important to remark that the authors emphasized that the FD of the trajectories was used for comparative purposes and not to determine the exact FD of a given path.

3.2.2. Fish

The fractal properties of fish school trajectories were examined by Tikhonov et al. in two papers [44,45] using the computer modelling of four components of the trophic chain: nutrients, phytoplankton, zooplankton, and fish. Their model was able to describe a wide range of fish school motions. For example, it revealed that the groups swim to the areas with the highest zooplankton density and that the complex motion of the school is basically dependent on the predation rate. It also indicated that a decrease in the predation rate induced a transition in the type of trajectory from one with frequent changes of direction (with fractal properties for all temporal scales,) to another, straighter one (with pronounced multifractal properties for large-scale displacements). These results agree with the above-mentioned work regarding larvae after the onset of feeding [64] that displayed straighter swimming paths (with lowest FD) in a low-prey-density environment. Although not considered in their model, other works had indicated that the natural variation in environmental parameters can cause the school–prey system to adopt quasi-periodic behaviour and display chaotic oscillations ([44,45] and references therein).
Examining in the wild the prey-searching methods of tagged marine organisms from several taxa, Sims et al. [66] concluded that they adopt Lévy-like moving behaviour, albeit not continuously, to optimize their chances of encountering patches of prey. It must be noted that prey (and feed on a farm) do not usually have a random distribution. This behaviour will be particularly advantageous in environments with shifting resources due, for example, to the exploitation of their prey by other species (which may resemble an open, multitrophic aquacultural production system), climate change, or other modifications, and its study may contribute to the optimization of feeding strategies in conventional aquaculture and in PFF.
Zhang et al. [67] developed a procedure for classifying small fish groups according to their shoaling/feeding status. Processed 2D video images of zebrafish were used to create a database of different behaviours that constituted the core information used to train a VGG-16 network (a 16-layer deep convolutional neural network). Changes of the status of fish during feeding were described by two-dimensional image entropy (calculated according to SE). The procedure was able to discriminate between two group behaviours that the authors termed “Normal” when shoaling unmolested and “Abnormal” when responding to a feeding stimulus, where the response resembles the schooling of the fish. It must be noted, however, that “abnormal” is an unfortunate selection of name given that the response examined is the normal response of the fish to the feed. In any case, the authors indicate the need to develop their model further by including images of different quality and different types of behaviour. This work supports the previously mentioned study by [35] who found that not only the SE of the fish system increased with the number of fish but also that the behaviour corresponding to schooling (in response to a stochastic event, similar to the “Abnormal” behaviour described in the study by [67]) had a higher SE value than that of the “Normal” shoaling system (termed “basal” in the study by [35]), for which the actual values are a function of the number of fish in the group, a parameter whose value was not taken into consideration by [67]. For 25 fish and depending on the tank and the number of days after the beginning of the experiment, the SE in the study by [35] increased from 4.3–4.8 (basal, normal shoaling) to 4.4–5.5 (schooling, “abnormal” response), while in the study by [67] the entropy of the zebrafish system (which seems to contain over 25 individuals) in their “normal” state is recorded to be under 0.25, increasing to 0.45 upon human intervention or during feeding.

3.3. Feeding Status

A relevant piece of information for farmers is the feeding status of their fish, i.e., whether they are feeding and how much they consume. While there are works devoted to the discrimination of feeding/non feeding fish, there is a lack of information on how to classify feeding status according to the amount of food a fish feeds, particularly under real-life production settings. This subject was addressed by Chen et al. ([61] where the abstract is in English and the original paper in Chinese) who developed a fine-grained classification algorithm of fish-feeding status. The raw data used to build the database were 752 videos (3 s each, constituting 90 frames) labelled as non-eating, weakly eating, or strongly eating, that had been recorded in an RAS production facility (although, unfortunately, the authors do not mention the species). An optical flow algorithm was applied to convert the videos into many inter-frame motion feature samples upon which a five-layer (comprising one input layer, one output layer, and three hidden layers) classification neural network (CNN) was built, which had three output categories corresponding to the three different selected feeding states (non-eating, weakly eating, and strongly eating). The CNN had been optimized by a cross-entropy loss function. Two issues can be raised regarding the work: One is that the CNN does not include the ability to identify (and classify) “non-feeding” fish, which is very relevant. The second is that, at least in the English summary, there is no information on whether the model was tested and/or validated with video recordings different from those used to develop the CNN. In any case, the approach is interesting and deserves further attention.

3.4. Collective Behaviour

Nonlinear approaches have helped to understand how collective decisions are better than individual ones in social groups [68], including fish. Mann and Garnett [51] successfully applied the causal entropic framework designed by [69] to determine the origin of collective behaviour from a purely entropic point of view. The authors were able to predict the fundamental form of social interactions and showed that the causal entropic principle could provide a purely statistical prediction for many of the emergent properties of collective behaviour, including cases where the mechanisms for inter-individual interactions were not fully understood. Entropy has also found applications in the detection of special behaviours of relevance for the welfare of Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) in a RAS system. Zhao et al. [70] detected gastrointestinal evacuation using a modified kinetic energy model (KEM) that employed the dispersion, velocity, and turning angle of the shoal as parameters, for which dispersion was estimated by optical flow, entropy, and statistical parameters. The proposed KEM model showed a good performance in detecting emergent gathering and scattering behaviours.
It is known that collective changes from shoaling to schooling in a group of fish may happen suddenly as a consequence of a stochastic event [71], and this is reflected in changes in the SE of the system [35]. This phenomenon prompted Crosato et al. [72] to study the flow of information in a school of fish during collective U-turn swimming changes by using TE. The authors identified two different flows of information: an informative flow (positive TE) from fish that have already turned to fish that are turning, and a misinformative flow (negative TE) from fish that have not turned yet to fish that are turning. Local behavioural changes of a single individual could lead to large transitions between the collective state of a school, such as between schooling and milling (an ordered state in which individuals constantly rotate around an empty core [73]). Considering that a stochastic event may be anything unexpected that will startle a fish (such as noises or the sudden appearance of a predator, feed, a diver, or a robotic camera, all of which occur under normal farming conditions), it follows that these kind of studies are of relevance when attempting to understand the dynamics of fish motion, behaviour, and communication within cages, particularly in large offshore cages.

3.5. Effect of the Number of Fish

The number of fish in a group influences both the FD [74] and the SE [35] of some behaviours, particularly for small numbers of fish of shoaling species. Kushida et al. [74] calculated, among other properties, the FD of Japanese horse mackerel (Trachurus japonicus) from a HD video recording with a camera mounted on top of an experimental tank. Their aim was to build small virtual robots generating actions resembling the behaviour of actual fish. Their work showed that as the number of fish increased from one to twenty-four, the FD of the real fish tended to decrease, and the fish tended to move toward smaller regions of the tank. They postulated that this movement toward smaller regions should result in lower average swimming speeds that would, in turn, produce the measured lower FD values.
Unlike in the research conducted by [74], where it was shown that the FD tended to decrease with an increasing number of fish, [35] measured an increase in the SE in fish systems consisting of between one and fifty fish in both their shoaling and schooling responses for which a power relationship was maintained with the number of fish and whose coefficient of variation, which was the largest for the one-fish systems, decreased concomitantly with the number of fish. Compared to the shoaling, basal state, SE was usually higher (i.e., the unpredictability increased) for the schooling response to a stochastic event for all fish groups, but the difference was particularly noticeable for individual fish and for groups with only two to five fish. These results may indicate the influence of stress that occurs when shoaling fish find themselves isolated or in small groups, indicating that the results of experiments performed using one or a small number of fish may not be directly applicable to real-life fish-farming settings, and even less so to the larger PFF operations with hundreds of thousands of fish.

3.6. Collective Behaviour and Individual Interactions

Hiramatsu et al. [75] modelled the schooling behaviour of a group of medaka based on how an individual fish interacted with its neighbours in a tank with no flow, measuring the distance between individuals, their spatial distribution, and their communication processes. The structure and characteristics of the real fish school were quantified by the nearest neighbour distance (which also yielded an estimate of the cohesiveness of the school), the level of polarization (the intensity of the parallel orientation of a fish school), the expanse (an estimate of the distance of every fish to the mass centre of the school), and its FD (as a measurement of the straightness/tortuousness of the trajectory of the school’s centre of mass). The simulations were able to satisfactorily reproduce the behaviour of the medaka school using a simple genetic algorithm with fitness defined by those four variables.
The coexistence of order and flexibility within fish schools was examined by Inada and Kawachi [43] using a simple numerical model and a computer simulation. According to their results, both the number of neighbours interacting and the randomness of individual motion influence the order of the school and its flexibility. For high flexibility, there were optimal and low (two to three) numbers of interacting neighbours. A slightly larger number of interacting neighbours (four to five) was necessary when the fish paid attention to more conspecifics in the school. School order was established for three to four neighbours, indicating that schooling fish have evolved a specialized ability to establish both school order and flexibility when the maximum number of individuals interacting is relatively small, i.e., higher than three.
Suzuki et al. [76] designed a model after quantifying the behaviour of chicken grunt (Parapristipoma trilineatum) schools of different sizes in a tank containing a column. The FD analyses of the time series [77] quantifying their behaviour indicated that it was random, but the attractive and repulsive forces of the walls were larger, while the magnitudes of the propulsive force were smaller, in small schools (one to five fish) than in larger schools (ten to twenty-five fish). However, the attractive and repulsive forces of the structure were significant only for the larger school (n = 25). Although the model seemed to be useful, the authors indicate the need to collect more data on different patterns of use of space and for different species.
In a comprehensive and very interesting work, Wark et al. [78] examined the existence of population-specific shoaling behaviours in 13 wild populations of three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Standard measures of shoaling behaviour failed to distinguish among the different populations, but when their swimming patterns were analysed using population-level probability distributions and their SE was quantified, significant differences between populations were identified. Unfortunately, there were not enough data to estimate the entropy for individual fish and, consequently, it was not possible to elucidate whether the differences between populations were due to consistent individual behavioural patterns or to differences among individuals within the populations. However, a log-likelihood analysis showed that an individual’s behaviour was more akin to that of its own population, thus supporting the hypothesis that the SE of a population most likely reflects that of its individuals.
Fish are exposed to different kinds of stimuli in farms. Obviously, one of them is the feed, but there are also undesirable stimuli that may alarm fish and provoke stress and changes in their behaviour, such as noises and the presence of predators, particularly when the latter are actively feeding upon their conspecifics. Schaerf et al. [79] studied the changes in individual and group behaviour as well as the rules of interaction among fish before and after being submitted to two different stimuli: an alarm cue (the filtered, macerated bodies of their conspecifics) and a food cue. The controls were exposed to the conditioned water only. The behavioural responses were measured by linear and nonlinear parameters. Compared to the controls, the fish stimulated by food displayed a reduced frequency of observing group mates at close proximities and they travelled at higher speeds. Unsurprisingly, the strongest responses were elicited by the alarm cue, including a tendency to travel at lower speeds. Changes in their conditional entropy, mutual information, and entropy rate indicated an increase in the unpredictability of their movements upon exposure to the alarm. The alarmed fish also moved in more tightly clustered shoals with smaller distances to their neighbours than in either the control groups or the groups exposed to food. An increase in the entropy in the alarmed shoals was also reported by [35], and the difference between the SE of basal and the alarmed states was more pronounced when the fish were alone or in small groups (two to five individuals) than in larger groups of up to fifty individuals, wherein the fish may have felt safer.
Cross-entropy loss was one of the parameters used by Heras et al. [80] to develop a deep attention network to understand, and ultimately predict, the behaviour of a fish in its group, with the group being variable in size. The example selected was the estimation of the probability of a fish within a group to turn right or left. The network was developed and trained based on the behaviour of groups of 60, 80, and 100 juvenile zebrafish that were 2D-video-recorded and whose 2D position, velocity, and acceleration values were tracked by Idtracker.ai (https://idtrackerai.readthedocs.io/, accessed on 21 March 2023). The network performed satisfactorily and, in agreement with the abovementioned works on the number of fish and their interactions, the number of interacting individuals was found to be variable, typically somewhere between 8–22 fish, with 1–10 more important cases, particularly if some moved at higher speed in front or to the sides or if they were very close or on a collision path. Their results indicate that each fish decides the turning size by actively selecting information from the group.
The work of Wilson et al. [81] presented some practical implications when a shoal of fish consists of hungry and sated fish and indicates that even optimizing the feeding conditions for all fish may decrease the expense of energy that hungry fish and groups consisting of mostly hungry fish will use to swim faster, possibly, as indicated by the authors, to optimize their detection of prey/feed. Their work examined how the number of hungry fish in groups of up to eight individuals of X-ray tetras (Pristella maxillaris) influences the behaviour of the individuals and the group. The authors used five different kinds of groups with various ratios of hungry/satiated fish (8/0, 6/2, 4/4, 2/6, and 0/8). They calculated, at the individual level, the mean speed and mean nearest neighbour distance and, at the group level, the median polarization (as a measure of group coherence), while information flow within each group was calculated by the mean pairwise TE, as described by [72]. Groups with a greater proportion of hungry fish swam faster and exhibited greater nearest neighbour distances, but there was no difference in the swimming speeds of hungry versus well-fed fish within the groups. Thus, the nutritional status of individuals within the group seems to impact both individual and group behaviours. In addition, one very interesting result was that the flow of information was greater in the groups with a majority of hungry individuals, but there was not a linear relationship between the pairwise TE and the relative number of hungry/fed fish. Rather, there seemed to be a difference, which was also underlined by the authors, between the three groups consisting of mostly hungry fish and the one with up to 50–50% hungry/fed fish on the one hand and the two other groups consisting of a majority of fed fish (six fed/two hungry and eight fed fish) on the other. The existence of such a breaking point, which deserves further study and characterization, may find a practical application when optimizing feeding schedules to identify the point in time when most of the fish on a farm are hungry and need to be fed.

3.7. Collective Behaviour and Hierarchies

Collective movement requires each individual animal to decide to move, a process that is usually initiated by a single individual or by a small group and that may be based on a variety of factors (e.g., age, sex, position in the hierarchy, etc.) [82,83]. Therefore, the identification of leaders in groups and the mechanisms of leadership are important, particularly (but not only) in the wild, where they may need to escape predators, find food, and avoid polluted and stressful locations. Collignon et al. [38] used the idTracking software [84] to generate the individual trajectories of each individual in groups of n = 2–10 zebrafish. The distribution of leadership was quantified by calculating the entropy associated with the time series of the identity of all the leaders. The results indicated that any fish had the potential to lead the collective movement of the group and that all shoal members had the same success rate. The predictor of a fish’s tendency to initiate collective movement mainly seemed to be its mobility, regardless of its position in the hierarchy, i.e., an informed individual moving in a specific direction is more likely to be followed by a group of naive individuals by just moving faster than the rest. However, it was also observed that some individuals lead collective movements more often than others, which may, over time, result in the development of specialized roles, for example, if this initiation is linked to personality traits such as boldness.
Niizato et al. [85] used ayus (Plecoglossus altivelis) and boids (artificial life simulation) models to assess the integrity in small groups of fish (n = 2 to 5). They compared three parameters to analyse the dynamics within the fish groups: Mutual information (MI), TE, and integrated information theory (IIT 3.0). Unlike most parameters concerning the acquirement of information about “what the (fish) system does”, integrated information theory (IIT) measures the “degree of consciousness” of a system. IIT postulates that the amount of information loss caused by a minimum information partition is equivalent to the degree of information integration in the system, i.e., IIT measures “what the (fish) system is” [85]. IIT 3.0 identifies intrinsic differences in the integrity of schools of n = 2–5 fish, and it detects the existence of a discontinuity in the integrity of the system between groups of n = 2–3 and those n = 4–5 individuals. Such differences in group integrity (i.e., an estimation of leadership in the group, albeit different from the classical leadership concept as used in TE analyses) is due to the fact that group integration in systems of n < 4 fish requires an intact visual field, but for groups of n > 4 fish, group integration has tolerance for some blind spots. No such discontinuity was detected by the SE of seabass groups of n = 1 to 5 fish mentioned above by [35]. This may be due to differences in the parameters used but also to the time scale of the analyses (3 min in the study by [35] and 0.05 s in the study by [85]) and to the likely differences in the behaviour of the two species. It is important to stress that the results obtained by [85] are only applicable within the very limited timescale used (the reaction time of a fish, which is about 0.05 s) and to the number of fish used (n = 2–5). The behaviour of systems over longer time periods and incorporating a larger number of fish must be examined independently. Nevertheless, IIT has emerged as an interesting parameter with respect to understanding fish systems’ behaviour and may shed new light on the understanding of the behaviour of larger systems, such as those used in PFF.

3.8. Collective Behaviour and Mixed Shoal Species

Despite the emphasis placed on multitrophic aquaculture, there are very few published works on fish behaviour in mixed-species settings. It must be noted that the species mixed on multitrophic farms do not predate on each other; thus, the well-studied predator–prey interactions should not apply to those cases. Ward et al. [86] studied the behaviour of three species that form mixed-species shoals in the wild: three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), nine-spined sticklebacks (Pungitius pungitius), and roach (Rutilus rutilus). The authors set up different experiments in which the individuals from the three species were mixed and analysed their behaviour through classical (the mean of the median speeds of each fish, mean distance between all fish, and the mean polarization of the group during each trial) and nonlinear (TE) methods. The single-species groups were more polarized than the mixed-species groups, and there were differences between treatments in terms of the mean pairwise TE. Species-specific differences were noted with respect to (1) the use of information within the mixed-species groups and (2) the responses to conspecifics and heterospecifics in the mixed-species groups. The TE-based results indicated that information flows both between and within species in all treatments. Interestingly, the results from the TE analysis did not always agree with the results of the classical parameters uncovering features of the inter-species interactions that traditional measurement methods did not. This study stresses the relevance of examining the inter-species interactions in multitrophic settings and the need to use non-linear methods to monitor their interactions.

3.9. Tagging and Pain

The tagging of fish is a common practice used to perform experiments and, potentially, in farming settings to identify fish and monitor their production [6]. The Visual Implant Elastomer (VIE) tags developed by Northwest Marine Technology are considered to avoid significantly influencing the behaviour of fish [87]. Corroborating the work of [87], Eguiraun et al. [23] also found a negligible effect of VIE-tagging on the SE of the schooling response of a group of 81 European seabass (a 0.25% decrease) and a very small effect on the permutation entropy of the same response (increased by less than 1%). On the other hand, the Katz–Castiglioni FD of the same experiment [23] decreased in tagged fish somewhere between 4–15% (for sliding window lengths of between 320–1280 points from 24 frames/sec video images). It must be noted that the Katz–Castiglioni FD suffered an increase upon MeHg treatment between 3–15%, which led the authors to propose a non-relevant effect of the tag compared to that of a documented chemical stressor [23].
Using zebrafish, Ruberto et al. [88] found somewhat different results: tagging barely impacted individual behaviour and shoaling and schooling tendencies, but the procedure did increase the speed of the individual fish and that of the group. Interestingly, TE indicated a significant effect of tagging on inter-individual interactions, providing indications that they might be tag-colour-dependent. For example, yellow-tagged fish were less likely to influence others, although the other colours did not seem to provoke similar responses [88]. The influence of tagging on the social behaviour of fish had already been documented by Frommen et al. [89], who showed that test fish spent significantly more time near the tagged shoal than near the sham-tagged shoal; however, contrary to [88], they did not find any significant effect attributable to the colour of the tag. However, in the study by [89], the test zebrafish that were not tagged and had grown with non-tagged fish did show a preference to swim closer to tagged individuals. Taken together, it seems that tagging may have a colour- and species-specific effect on fish behaviour, and caution should be adopted when experiments require the mixing of tagged and non-tagged fish.
Tagging most likely also causes pain, which inherently brings about reduced welfare and increased stress. While VIE tags are expected to induce some form of pain in a fish when injected, this tagging procedure is usually performed under anaesthesia, which may exert an effect on its own (see below), but other forms of tagging may be performed quickly and without aesthetic treatment, such as fin clipping. When the tagging and/or the tag induce pain, the pain itself may change the behaviour of the injected fish, as demonstrated by Deakin et al. [90]. These authors examined the effects of two alternative tagging methods, namely, fin clipping (performed with and without lidocaine anaesthesia) and PIT tagging (the implantation of Passive Integrated Transponders for individual fish identification). They assessed the effects of the treatments on the complexity of individual fishes’ behaviour by measuring the FD of their 3D swimming trajectories and showed that, indeed, both treatments decreased the complexity of their behaviours, which was reflected by a decrease in their FD values. Interestingly, an injection of acetic acid, a standard pain test, also decreased the FD in a dose-dependent manner. On the other hand, when the fins were clipped after lidocaine treatment, the FD of the fish was not altered, resembling that of the control and sham-treated fish. This work indicates the usefulness of the FD of swimming trajectories for estimating changes in fish welfare due to pain and the beneficial effects of the use of anaesthesia to avoid painful procedures.
Fin amputation and pain in zebrafish were also the subject of a very interesting study by Audira et al. [91]. Fin tagging is a common procedure used to distinguish fish groups under experimental conditions where different fins may be targeted for different experimental groups in the same experiment. The study also addressed whether the selection of the fin (dorsal, caudal, anal, pelvic, or pectoral) might have implications on the behaviour of the amputated fish. The authors aimed to assess the complexity of 3D swimming behaviour (by idTracker) by measuring the subjects’ locomotor activity (by four end-point parameters), movement orientation (two end points), exploratory behaviour (six end points) and through FD and entropy analyses of their swimming trajectories. Additional experiments were performed to verify whether potential changes might be attributed to pain (by using lidocaine) or whether they were simply due to mechanical effects caused by the lack of a fin. The subjects’ behaviour during the regeneration period was also examined. The observations during the first 2 days after fin amputation were made for isolated fish and groups (n = 6). Upon identification of caudal amputation as the intervention exerting the strongest (negative) effect on fish behaviour, the caudal-fin-amputated fish were further investigated, but only in groups (n = 6), for 10 days for the lidocaine-treated fish and a further 30 days to perform follow-up assessments of their behaviour during caudal fin regeneration. The FD values of fish behaviour for the fish with amputated caudal, pelvic, or pectoral fins were significantly lower than those of the control fish but only when the fish were tested in groups and not when they were tested individually. Similarly, only the entropy of the fish groups whose caudal fin (but not other fins) had been amputated was significantly different, albeit higher, than the entropy of the control groups, but the significance was lost when the fish were individually tested. According to the behavioural parameters, recovery started 1 day post-amputation, was almost complete after 5 days, and was fully completed by the 10th day. It was paralleled by a gradual recovery in the values of FD and entropy, which, 3 days post-amputation, resembled the controls. The regeneration of the caudal fin had already started on the 5th day, while almost full and full regeneration had occurred by days 20–25 and day 30 day, respectively. However, only 5 days post-amputation, with only about 20% of the fin regenerated, the fish were able to display relatively normal behaviour, indicating that mechanical hindrance may not be the main reason for their altered behaviours. Lidocaine treatment provoked diminished activity, which is an expected side-effect consistent with its sedative effects. The diminished activity of amputated fish may also be a response adopted to avoid the aggressive responses of conspecifics known to occur in species that form dominance hierarches, such as zebrafish, and whose intensity may be dependent on the size of the group (see the research conducted by [91] for a complete discussion and additional references). As the authors indicate, this would explain why the fish tested individually (i.e., in the absence of threatening conspecifics) displayed higher activity.
Although the study by [91] was performed on zebrafish and to test pain, it casts some very interesting light on the application of entropy and FD techniques for identifying the behaviour of farmed fish that have been subjected to aggression from conspecifics, given that such aggression is often enacted in the form of bites to the fins. Moreover, it can help to quantify how practical implementations to reduce stress, e.g., through environmental enrichment, may decrease aggression [92,93]. It may also explain why wounded fish would prefer to be still and alone (thereby avoiding potential aggression) and why healthy fish might prefer to be in groups (safety in numbers).

3.10. Fear/Anxiety Responses to Predators

Regarding the study of fishes’ responses to predators, robots have been introduced in research studies as predators in order to prevent focal fish from exhibiting the inconsistent responses that are often observed when real predators are introduced in the system. This is because unlike actual predatory fish, robots can be used under controlled, custom-designed experimental conditions that produce more repeatable results.
Butail et al. [94] used zebrafish in an experimental set up with a known information flow and applied TE as a measure of directional information in two systems: (1) fish–fish and (2) fish–robot (a life-sized zebrafish replica) interactions. The behaviours in the two systems were 2D-video-recorded by a camera above the tank, and the SE was used to calculate the TE of the interactions between the fish–conspecific and fish–robot. As expected, the authors found that when a live fish interacts with a replica moving along a predetermined trajectory, the dominant flow of information is from the replica to the fish, and the information flow is reduced significantly if the motion of the replica is randomly delayed. On the other hand, the TE from a living focal fish to a living conspecific and vice versa were not significantly different [94]. In a related experiment, Bartolini et al. [95] used robotic replicas of different sizes and found that the TE indicated preferences and an adjustment in fish behaviour consisting of an avoidance of larger replicas and attraction towards smaller ones, by which the fish were, in turn, influenced. In contrast to the study conducted by [94], Bartolini et al. [95] found that similar-sized replicas did not elicit significant responses, i.e., no information transfer was observed between the fish and surrogate data generated by the hypothetical motion of either a support or a shoal of replicas.
TE was also applied by Neri et al. [96] to identify causal relationships in the behaviour of fish using a prey–predator system where the predatory fish was located outside the space occupied by the prey. This set up imitated the conditions of a fish swimming in a cage and visually interacting with predators attracted to the net and surrounding it. They initially used a robotic predator and, as is common when one of the interacting partners is a robot, the information flowed unidirectionally from the robot (predator) to the fish, i.e., the zebrafish reacted to the behaviour of the robot. However, when the robot was replaced by a real predatory fish (the red tiger oscar fish, Astronotus ocellatus), the one-directional information flow was substituted by a reciprocal one: upon visual interaction, a positive feedback loop was established by which the predator watches and shows an increased responsiveness to the prey’s movement, whose response is avoidance. This can be of relevance for fish swimming in the outer parts of the net where predators may also swim looking for potential prey. Interestingly, these results contrast with those related by Hu et al. [97], who reported a net information flow from a living predator (the northern snakehead, Channa argus) to the prey fish (rosy bitterling, Rhodeus ocellatus) in a similar circular arena. However, Hu’s work had several methodological differences, including the use of different species and the fact that each fish could feel the ripples in the water produced when the other fish swam. In this case, the flow of information moved from from the predator to the prey, including a critical and sensitive region where the prey is highly vigilant of the predator’s behaviour and did not allow it to draw near.
To evaluate a zebrafish’s fear response to a 3D replica of the above-mentioned allopatric predator (Astronotus ocellatus), Spinello et al. [98] used geotaxis and two avoidance-related parameters: the average distance between the replica and the fish and the time spent by the focal fish in the half of the water column opposite to that occupied by the replica. A finite-state Markov chain switching between “stationary”, “swimming”, and “attacking” states was used to control the motion of the replica. As expected, exposure to the replica increased the fear response of the fish, shown by an increased rate of geotaxis (i.e., the fish swam to the bottom), and they spent different times in the vertical axis opposite to that occupied by the replica, but there were no differences regarding the average distance from the replica. TE revealed a rapid adjustment of the fishes’ behaviour to avoid the predator’s attacks.

3.11. Modulation of Fear/Anxiety Responses

The parameter most used as an indicator of fear/anxiety is geotaxis, i.e., the tendency to swim to the bottom of a tank. To characterize and model this behaviour, Burbano and Porfiri [99] used data from zebrafish treated with citalopram and ethanol [100], two commonly used anxiolytic drugs, and what they defined as spatial entropy (calculated as its SE) to identify the extent of the volume of water occupied by the fish and in which part of the tank its activity is more concentrated. The same group of authors have extensively used zebrafish models to study the effect of fear/anxiety and anxiolytic drugs on this species’ behaviour and on the positive emotional contagion conferred by the treated toward the untreated fish to relieve the anxiety responses provoked in the latter by a predator (see references below).
Examining the effect of psychoactive substances on zebrafish behaviour, Ladu et al. [101] applied TE to quantify the interaction between individual fish and a shoal-replica of four zebrafish treated with different doses of caffeine (0 (control), 5, 25, and 50 mg/L). TE did not detect any significant flow of information from fish to replica and vice versa for either the control or the group with the lowest caffeine dose; however, for the fish treated with 25 and 50 mg/L, the TE from fish to replica was lower than that from the replica to the fish.
Macrì et al. [102] documented the modulation of fear conditioning in zebrafish provoked by ethanol (a 0% control group, and two test groups with 0.25% and 1.00% concentrations of ethanol/water). Individual 3D swimming trajectories of the fish were generated by two video cameras (placed on top and in front of the experimental tank) and analysed using the authors’ own software. The fear response was elicited by three zebrafish replicas manoeuvred along 3D trajectories by a robotic platform. The following parameters were tested: the avoidance index, geotaxis, freezing, spatial entropy (tendency of the fish to explore the tank, which was computed as the SE), and average speed, acceleration and angular speed. The spatial avoidance values indicated that ethanol, particularly the 0.25% concentration, decreased the degree of aversion to the fear-related compartment. Geotaxis was also modulated by low ethanol concentrations: the 0.25%-ethanol-treated fish displayed no preference, while both the control and 1.00%-ethanol-treated fish preferred the bottom of the tank. Over the course of the experiment, the evolution of the values of spatial entropy and average speed in the control and 0.25%-ethanol-treated fish were comparable, but those of the 1.00%-treated individuals decreased with time. Thus, according to the authors’ hypothesis, the zebrafish were indeed fear-conditioned, and the response was modulated by ethanol, particularly for anxiety-related behaviours (i.e., spatial avoidance and geotaxis). However, only the highest dose (1.00%) exerted effects on the other parameters (i.e., average speed, average acceleration, and spatial entropy), yet these were small and mostly consisted of a decrease in the values during the test. These results confirmed the ethanol-dependent reduction in general locomotion observed in previous studies ([102] and references therein).
In a related study, Clément et al. [103] 3D-tracked zebrafish behaviour to estimate fear-conditioned behaviour and how it is affected by the treatment with two anxiolytic substances: citalopram and ethanol. As described in the study by [104], fear was elicited by a programmed robot simulating a zebrafish-sympatric predator (an Indian pond heron, Ardeola grayii), which hit the water surface of a lateral compartment approximately every 30 s to induce, as a conditioned response, the avoidance of the upper part of the water column. The success of the conditioning procedure was indicated by the values of spatial entropy (calculated using the SE). The experiment was performed in the presence and absence of three different concentrations of citalopram (30, 50, and 100 mg/L) and ethanol (0.25%, 0.50%, and 1.00%) and the drug-free control groups. The parameters measured were the avoidance index, geotaxis, freezing, spatial entropy, and average speed, acceleration and angular speed. The results of the spatial entropy values for citalopram and ethanol treatments were also similar and mirrored the freezing behaviour: the fish displayed an increasing tendency to explore during the later stages of the test that was not modulated by citalopram concentration, while the spatial entropy did not significantly vary according to the experimental group, and no time–concentration interactions were found. Geotaxis was the most informative parameter: a conditioned control fish showed a clear positional avoidance of the robot by exhibiting a robust preference for the lower portion of the tank. As hypothesized by the authors, the citalopram administration resulted in a linear dose–response curve with respect to anxiety, with the subjects treated with a 100 mg/L dose exhibiting a significant preference for the upper part of the tank. Similar results were obtained with the ethanol treatment but, in this case, the modulation followed a U-shaped dose–response curve: the fish treated with 0.25% ethanol preferred the upper part, while the fish treated with 0%, 0.50%, and 1.00% preferred the lower part of the tank.

3.12. Psychoactive Drugs for Anxiety Modulation

Macrì et al. [100] compared the behavioural information obtained from 2D and 3D video-tracking procedures. The subjects of the study were groups of zebrafish (n = 16, consisting of 8 males and 8 females), and the treatments were as follows: a control, three groups treated with citalopram (30, 50, and 100 mg/L), and three groups treated with ethanol (0.25%, 0.50%, and 1.00% ethanol/water vol/vol). One objective of the study was to assess the behavioural responses of the fish to the drugs, for which the authors found that both drugs influenced the fishes’ swimming patterns and anxiety-related profiles. In addition, the ethanol administration induced erratic movements, freezing, and the avoidance of the anxiety-eliciting areas. The second purpose of their work was to compare the quality of the information obtained from the 2D and from 3D video recordings. The results indicate that compared to the 3D views, the 2D views occasionally yielded false positive and false negative findings. Unsurprisingly, the 2D projections of 3D trajectories introduced a source of unwanted variation in zebrafish behavioural phenotyping and both 2D views underestimated the absolute levels of general locomotion. Given a choice regarding the positioning of a camera for a 2D recording only, the authors stated that the top of the experimental tank would be preferable, since the data thus obtained were more akin to those from the 3D reconstruction. On the other hand, they did not recommend to position the camera for a frontal view, since this produced negative findings [100]. Although the work does not use entropy or FD data treatment (and, therefore, is not listed in Appendix A), we consider that the subject is relevant both as a basis with which to recommend 3D image acquisition and to understand the other related works by the same group of authors mentioned in this review that do use non-linear methods.

3.13. Positive Emotional Contagion

Emotional contagion, a behaviour widely documented in numerous species, including fish, seems to be unconscious and governed by the amygdala [105]. Therefore, species with a proper amygdala or with a homologous organ (such as zebrafish) may respond in a similar manner (see the research conducted by [106] for a more detailed discussion). Until now, fear-contagion-based studies have mostly been the object of scientific work, but some recent studies (mentioned below) indicate that positive emotional contagion also occurs in fish groups. The study and implementation of positive emotional contagion in fish farming may improve the welfare of the entire production by adequately treating only a subset of fish. Alternatively, the selection and inclusion of “positive” fish, i.e., those with an innate tendency to display positive behaviours, may have a similar effect on the shoal, and it would eliminate the need to use undesirable and time-dependent treatments.
Burbano Lombana et al. [106] examined the response to citalopram of individually treated zebrafish both individually (where the fish were tested in isolation) and in groups (consisting of one treated and four untreated fish). The experimental set up was akin to that described in the study by [100], and the treatment consisted of a control, untreated group, and two groups treated with 30 and 100 mg citalopram/L. In agreement with previous studies, the citalopram treatment decreased the geotaxis of the treated fish when individually tested. Interestingly, the presence of a single treated fish in the group also decreased the anxiety-related behaviour of the entire group, which was demonstrated by their reduced geotaxis. Notably, while the behaviours of the fish treated with 30 and 100 mg/L were not significantly different from each other, the fish treated with the higher dose displayed a greater tendency to swim upwards when compared to the control. Group cohesion (nearest neighbour distance) and coordination (polarization) were not affected by the treatment. Interestingly, a TE analysis of the causal interactions within the group showed that the directionality of the emotional contagion was from the treated to the untreated fish, but not the other way around.

4. Prospects and Research Requirements

Most of the studies reviewed herein were performed under experimental conditions and on model fish systems (primarily zebrafish), with excellent water quality and light conditions and no external disturbances. Furthermore, the information in these studies was usually obtained through the processing of 2D video images (which [100] reported to be suboptimal) and the recording of only a few minutes of activity at most, even though the videos may have been recorded for longer times and the experiments themselves may have lasted many days. Therefore, even though the results are undoubtfully relevant, they must be confirmed under real farming conditions. One of the key issues is the acquisition of high-quality reliable data, preferably from information obtained from 3D swimming patterns [100]. In our opinion, sensors will be optimized and deployed in the near future to provide 3D information on fish behaviour, regardless of the quality of water. A second key issue is the need for complete, reliable databases of recorded and analysed behaviours from different fish species, under different conditions, and concerning longer periods of time. The addressal of this issue will enable the fast and successful classification of on-farm detected behaviours through the use of AI procedures such as 3D machine vision and machine learning for the adequate classification of information (including classification convolutional and neural networks, Bayesian networks, Hidden Markov models, and others). Thirdly, the on-farm information needs to be recorded with high frequency, promptly processed, and made available to farmers given that such information should aim to both facilitate the control of production and the detection, as quickly as possible, of deviations affecting the health and welfare of fish and the quality of production. It follows that implementation of a full IA system implies a large investment, both financially and in terms of the recruitment of expert personnel, that only powerful farmers are likely to be able to afford. Yet, their willingness to share valuable information in a competitive market will likely be very limited. Fortunately, some punctual applications are indeed finding their way into practical implementation, for instance, machine learning algorithms and techniques for identifying and classifying fish, evaluating biomass, performing behavioural analysis, and predicting water quality parameters [15].
A particularly interesting and novel study examined herein is the one addressing positive emotional contagion [106] since it opens the possibility of improving the welfare of the shoal by manipulating the status of selected individuals. It also opens interesting new research avenues concerning the identification and possible selection of fish strains with naturally low levels of anxiety to exert a positive effect on the welfare of the entire production.
In our opinion, and based on the above-mentioned studies and how the field is developing, it is clear that non-linear analyses, including entropy and fractal analyses of fish behaviour on farms, will find wide applications in (1) identifying the status of individual fish in a shoal; identifying (2) normal behaviours (in response to normal environmental variations, feed, and conspecifics) and (3) abnormal ones (in response to disease, parasites, aggression, and predators); (4) quantifying welfare; (5) maintaining health; (6) reducing disease and parasitism (potentially uncovered for instance by changes in their FD); and (7) reducing stress (positive emotional contagion) and, consequently, (8) improving the yield and quality of production. However, as already mentioned above, the practical application and successful implementation of these techniques still requires a wealth of additional experimental and practical data obtained in real farming settings, under different conditions, and for different species.

Author Contributions

The authors, contributed as follows to the present work: conceptualization, H.E. and I.M.; methodology, H.E. and I.M.; formal analysis of information, H.E. and I.M.; writing of the original draft preparation, subsequent revisions and editing, H.E. and I.M. Funding acquisition and project administration H.E. and I.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The work was supported by the Spanish MINECO (Grant RTC-2014–2837-2- “SELATUN: Minimización de la problemática del mercurio del atún y valorización del atún como alimento saludable, Programa Retos-Colaboración 2014”. The funding source had no involvement in the preparation of this manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study does not require ethical approval.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created in the preparation of this review. All publications cited are public.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the writing of the manuscript or in the decision to publish it.

Appendix A

Table A1. Summary of main data from publications on fractal and entropy-based methods analysing individual and collective behaviours of fish with potential for selected applications in PFF/IA.
Table A1. Summary of main data from publications on fractal and entropy-based methods analysing individual and collective behaviours of fish with potential for selected applications in PFF/IA.
ReferenceSpeciesMethods, FD, and/or EntropyIndividual/Collective BehaviourMain Findings
Individual identification in groups.
[46]Mature goldfish (Carassius auratus).The mean swimming velocity and five nonlinear parameters: two measures of the FD (characteristic FD and the Richardson dimension) the Lempel–Ziv complexity, the Hurst exponent, and the degree of relative dispersion.Individual swimming patterns in groups.Discriminant analysis of the six measures showed that each fish displayed a very different and highly individual swimming profile, which allowed the researchers to distinguish each individual fish within groups.
[101]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).2D video recording of the interaction between the fish and a robotic replica of 4 zebrafish by TE as affected by 3 concentrations of caffeine: 0 (control), 5, 25, and 50 mg/L.Individual response of individually placed fish to a replica of a shoal of conspecifics and its modulation by caffeine.The TE was always higher from the replica to the fish, but the difference was only significant in fish exposed to at least 25–50 mg caffeine/mL.
Preying/feeding search behaviours—Larvae
[64]Pink anemonefish (Amphiprion perideraion) larvae.Fractal analysis of prey search patterns.Individual swimming and searching behaviour in groups (up to 10 larva) prior to and after the start of feeding.FD studies show that the larvae forage using at least one type of search behaviour for ranging and another for local searches depending on the age of the larvae and the prey’s abundance
[65]Malabar grouper (Epinephelus malabaricus) larvae3D-recorded swimming behaviour of the larvae in presence of prey, including FD.Early feeding: individual larval prey-searching behaviour in groups of 4 larvae.Without prey, the FDs of the horizontal and vertical projections of larvae indicated increased activity on vertical axis. In the presence of prey, the FD showed similarly complex activity in both dimensions, indicating an optimization of the search volume and, therefore, the need to consider 3D search behaviour.
Preying/feeding search behaviours—Fish
[44,45]Computer simulation.Chaos and fractals.Modelling of fish schools’ chaotic movements in the presence of prey.The motion of the fish school and its fractal properties depend on the predation rate.
[66]Several—Pressure (depth)-sensitive-data-logging-tagged basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus), small spotted catshark (Scyliorhinus canicular), bigeye tuna (Thunnus obesus), Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua), leatherback turtles (Dermochelys coriacea), penguins (Spheniscus magellanicus), and southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonine).Simulation of searches was used to test the hypothesis that foraging success (biomass consumed per distance moved) by optimal Lévy walkers in fractal (natural) prey distributions exceeded prey acquisition rates within random prey fields.Different marine predators’ searching behaviour in the wild.Optimal search patterns (Lévy walks) seem to arise as a function of the underlying distribution of the prey field, i.e., the predator’s prey-searching patterns are a result of the prey distribution it encounters.
[67]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).The number of fish is not indicated. The behaviour of the groups was 2D-video-recorded, and the treated images were processed and used to create a database of different behaviours that was further used to train a VGG-16 network. Two-dimensional image entropy (calculated according to SE) was applied to describe the changes of the status of fish. Two statuses were examined: “Normal” (shoaling) and “Abnormal” (response to feeding).Collective behaviour in a group.The procedure can correctly classify fish into their real status. The image entropy (SE) increases when the fish transfer from a shoaling to a schooling-type of behaviour.
Feeding status
[61]. Abstract in English and original paper in Chinese.Not given.Real-life RAS farm settings. Use of a database of recoded videos to classify the fish into 3 categories: non-eating, weakly eating, and strongly eating using a 5-layer classification neural network (CNN) optimized by a cross-entropy loss function.Classification of fish according to three categories. It was not indicated if individual fish were classified.There was a lack of relevant information in the English abstract, including the species used, whether the classification system applies to individuals or groups, and whether the model was tested with data different from those used to develop the CNN.
Collective behaviour
[51]Species/organisms/social bodies displaying collective behaviour.Study of collective behaviours by applying the causal entropic (CE) framework of [69].To understand the origin of collective behaviour from a purely entropic point of view and make testable predictions about the fundamental form of social interactionsThe CE principle gives a purely statistical prediction for many of the emergent properties of collective behaviour even in the absence of a detailed understanding of the mechanisms of interaction between individuals.
[70]Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus).Video recording of a shoal and calculation of its dispersion (estimated by optical flow, entropy, and statistical parameters), velocity, and turning angle.Shoal behaviour and welfare: construction of an efficient method to recognize special shoal behaviours and provide reliable theoretical support for online smart supervision in aquaculture, especially in RAS, to ensure fish welfare.The proposed KEM model to detect a specific behaviour (evacuation of gastro-intestinal contents) showed good performance with respect to detecting emergent gathering and scattering behaviours of the shoal.
[72]Freshwater rummy-nose tetra (Hemigrammus rhodostomus).Video recording of the fish trajectories and assessment of the collective interactions by TE.Collective behaviour and interactions: flow in a fish school during collective U-turn swimming changes are exposed by TE.Two different information flows were identified: an informative flow (positive TE) from fish that have already turned to fish that are turning and a misinformative flow (negative TE) from fish that have not turned yet to fish that are turning.
Number of fish
[74]Japanese horse mackerel (Trachurus japonicas)HD video recording using a camera that is mounted
on top of a tank containing 1 to 24 fish and a simulation with robots under the same conditions.
Number of fish (1–24) and FD. Evaluation of a simulation of behaviour using robots and comparison of the fishes’ behaviour under the same conditions. Measures, among other parameters, the FD of the system.The trajectories of the virtual robot differed from those of real fish: while the value of FD in the real fish decreases with increasing number of fish, the opposite occurred when using robots.
[36]European seabass (Dicentrarchus labrax).2D video recording of the behaviour of fish estimated by the SE of their trajectory exposed to two variables: number of fish (n = 1 to 50) and a hit in the tank.Individual and collective (2–50) behaviour of groups in response to two different stressors: number of fish and a hit in the tank.The SE of the shoaling behaviour increased concomitantly with the number of fish (power function). In addition, the SE entropy increased after the hit in the tank (schooling response) for all fish groups but particularly so for individual fish and groups with only 2–5 fish.
Collective behaviour and individual interactions
[75]Medaka (Oryzias latipes).Simple genetic algorithm, the
nearest neighbour distance, polarization, the expanse, and the FD.
Individual schooling behaviour in groups of 5 fish and its mathematical modelling.Modelling the behaviour of the medaka school using a simple genetic algorithm with the fitness defined by 4 variables: the nearest neighbour distance, polarization, the expanse, and the FD of the school’s centre of mass.
[43]Computer simulation.Order and flexibility of the school based on attraction, repulsion, and parallel orientation
behaviour of individuals.
Motion of fish schools.School’s order and flexibility are affected by the number of neighbours interacting and by the randomness of individual motion. Schooling fish have evolved specialized ability to establish both school order and flexibility at low Nb,max (=3).
[76]Chicken grunt (Parapristipoma trilineatum).Higuchi’s FD analysis of digital video images time series.Effect of the number of fish and the presence of structures in the tank on the behaviour of the schools.There was a difference in behaviour of schools with 1–5 fish compared to those of 10–25 fish.
[78]Three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus).Video recording of the shoaling behaviour of fish from 13 different populations and estimation of the SE and log-likelihood analysis of the population distributions.Shoaling behaviour: Assessment of inter- and intra-populational variation in shoaling behaviour of 13 different populations of wild sticklebacks.Traditional behavioural measures did not reveal populational differences in shoaling behaviour but SE analyses identified population-specific clustering patterns consistent with individual-specific behavioural patterns.
[79]X-ray tetra (Pristella maxillaris).Recording and comparing, via linear and non-linear (entropy) measurements, the behaviour of fish groups exposed to an alarm cue (macerated conspecifics) and to food versus the controlsCollective behaviour of groups in responses to an alarm cue and food.Exposure to the alarm cue induced the strongest responses with wide ranging effects on the behaviour of the fish and changes in the entropy, mutual information, and entropy rate, indicating that their movements became more unpredictable after exposure to the alarm both in terms of changes in displacement and changes in velocity over short time periods.
[80]Juvenile zebrafish (Danio rerio)Groups of 60, 80, or 100 juvenile zebrafish were 2D-video-recorded and their individual position, velocity, and acceleration values were tracked (Idtracker.ai). The statistics calculated from the videos included the following parameters: distance to centre of arena, local polarization, inter-individual distance, and probability of finding another animal around a focal one. Cross-entropy loss was one of the parameters used to train the deep network when estimating the probability of a fish turning right or left. The purpose of the work was to develop a model (using deep attention networks) to understand the rules of collective behaviour and predict the sides a fish will turn toward. Modelling individual behaviour of the collective according to interaction with conspecifics.The network indicated that the number of interacting individuals is between 8–22, with 1–10 more important ones, particularly if some move at higher speed in front or to the sides or if they are very close or on a collision path.
[81]X-ray tetras (Pristella maxillaris).2D video recording of the behaviour of groups of fish with different ratios of hungry/satiated fish (8/0, 6/2, 4/4, 2/6, and 0/8). Analysis of individual and group speed, group cohesion, and polarization, as well as pair-wise TE between individuals in each group.Individual and collective behaviours in groups of 8 fish and their interactions according to the hunger status of their individual members.The nutritional status of individuals within the groups impacts both individual and group behaviour, and members of heterogeneous groups adapt their behaviour to facilitate collective motion.
Collective behaviour and hierarchies
[38]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).Individual trajectories of each fish in groups of 2–10 zebrafish were established by idTracking [84] and the distribution of leadership was quantified by calculating the entropy associated with the time series of all the leaders.Collective behaviour: identification of leadership in groups of zebrafish.Any fish could potentially lead the collective movements in the shoal. The predictor of a fish’s tendency to lead seemed to be mostly its mobility, regardless of its position in the hierarchy, although some individuals led collective movements more often than others, which, over time, may result in the development of specialized roles.
[85]Ayus (Plecoglossus altivelis) and Boids (artificial life simulation) models.2D video recording of groups of n = 2–5 fish and analysis of Boids models to analyse the integrity of fish groups via three parameters: Mutual information (MI), TE, and integrated information theory (IIT 3.0). Use of individual interactions and dynamics of the collective behaviour in the school to assess the integrity of the system.IIT 3.0 identifies intrinsic differences in the behaviour of schools of 2–5 fish and a discontinuity in the group integration between systems of n = 2–3 fish and n = 4–5. For n < 4 groups, integrity requires an intact visual field. For n > 4 groups, integrity has tolerance for some blind spots.
Collective behaviour in mixed-species shoals
[86]Three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), nine-spined sticklebacks (Pungitius pungitius), and roach (Rutilus rutilus)Video recording of the behaviour of individuals and shoals composed of individuals from the same and different species using linear (mean of the median speeds of each fish, mean distance between all fish, and mean polarization of the group during each trial) and nonlinear (TE calculated on heading updates and differences for each pair of individuals within each group across all relevant samples) methods.Collective behaviour and interactions in environments with mixed-species shoals.Single-species groups were more polarized than mixed-species groups, for which there were differences between treatments in mean pairwise TE. Species-specific differences were noted in: (1) the use of information within the mixed-species groups and in (2) differences in the responses to conspecifics and heterospecifics in groups of mixed species.
Tagging and pain
[23]European seabass (Dicentrarchus labrax).The FD (Higuchi, Katz, and Katz–Castiglioni) and Shannon and Permutation entropy of the schooling response to a percussive force applied to the tank) of groups (n = 81) of VIE-tagged and non-tagged fish.Collective schooling behaviours of tagged/non-tagged fish.Negligible effect of VIE-tagging on the SE and very small effect on the permutation entropy. On the other hand, the Katz–Castiglioni FD of tagged fish decreased somewhere between 4–15% depending on the window length.
[88]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).Videorecording of the
individual and group behaviours
exhibited by groups of 3 individuals: two non-tagged and one (focal subject) that was
either non-tagged (control condition) or sham-, purple-, blue-, or yellow-tagged using traditional behavioural parameters of shoaling and schooling activities and information theoreticl measure of social interaction by TE
Effect of tagging on the individual and collective behaviour and interactions in groups of 3 fish.Tagging did not affect the shoaling and schooling tendencies of the fish, but it significantly increased individual speed of the tagged subjects and of the group. TE. however, showed altered levels of interactions between individuals and that yellow-tagged fish were less likely to influence others.
[90]Female zebrafish (Danio rerio).Effect of fin clipping (with and without administration of the anaesthetic lidocaine), PIT tagging, and a standard pain test on the complexity of the 3D swimming patterns of individual fish estimated by their FD values. The results were compared to those of control and sham-treated zebrafish.Effect of the selected pain treatments on the FD of 3D swimming trajectories of individual fish that were individually tested.The FD in treated groups showed a reduced degree pf complexity in the fishes’ trajectories, while the FD of the control and sham-treated fishes did not change over time. Anesthetizing the fish with lidocaine prior to fin clipping restored the complexity of the fish behaviour to that seen in control fish. FD was useful for estimating lack of welfare/presence of pain.
[91]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).Effect of fin amputation, lidocaine treatment, and fin regeneration on the complexity of 3D swimming patterns (analysed by idTracker) of individual fish estimated via their FD and through meandering entropy analyses. In addition, principal component and hierarchical clustering analyses of collective behaviour performance were calculated. The results were compared to those of control and sham-treated zebrafish.Effect of the selected treatments on the individual behaviour of fish individually tested (fin amputation) and collective behaviour in groups (n = 6, for lidocaine treatment and regeneration experiments).Amputation of caudal fin resulted in more dramatic effects that fish recovered from even before full regeneration of the fin. Lidocaine treatment did not accelerate recuperation and induced minor (sedative) side-effects.
Fear/anxiety responses to predators
[94]Zebrafish (Danio rerio) and life-sized robot replica of the zebrafish.The behaviours of a fish–conspecific and a fish–robot system were recorded by a web camera placed above the surface of the water. The SE was used to calculate the TE between the fish–conspecific and the fish–robot systems.Testing of TE as a measure of directional information in fish–fish and fish–robot interactions.Validation of TE as an information-theoretical measure with which to compute directional information flow in social behaviour.
[95]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).TE was used to study the relationship between fish and robots imitating these fish but differing in size.Behavioural response of individual fish to replica of differing in size.TE showed that the fish adjust their behaviour in response to variations in the size of the fish replicas, avoiding larger replicas and being attracted to and influenced by smaller ones. Similar-sized replica did not elicit significant responses.
[96]Zebrafish (Danio rerio) as prey, robotic predators, and red tiger oscar fish (Astronotus ocellatus) as live predators.Video recording of prey fish and their interactions with a robotic predator and with a real life predatory fish surrounding it. Analysis of causal interactions by TEIndividual behaviour. Predator–prey interactions: Testing of TE as a tool to identify and quantify causal prey–predator behavioural interactions in real fish systems.When a living predator surrounds the prey (as in open fish farming settings), a two-way interaction is established, as shown by TE: the predator watches and responds to the behaviour of the prey, which displays an avoidance response.
[97] Prey (rosy bitterling, Rhodeus ocellatus) and its predator (northern snakehead, Channa argus).Videorecording of the behaviour of the prey and the predator as they interact when the prey is placed in a concentric tank surrounded by a larger tank containing the predator. The fish can interact with each other visually and by feeling the ripples in the water produced by the swimming movements of the other.Use of TE to acquire information on the interactions between the two individual fish.The prey’s TE was generally significantly greater than the predator’s (i.e., information flows from the predator to the prey) regardless of the set of coarse-grained parameters chosen, indicating that the prey was more vigilant with respect to the predator’s position than vice versa. The prey was also vigilant while the predator moved aimlessly.
[98]Male and female zebrafish (Danio rerio) acting as prey and a 3D-printed robotic replica of its allopatric predator the red tiger oscar fish (Astronotus ocellatus).Videorecording of the motion of the prey fish in the vertical and horizontal axes in response to the movements of the replica predator controlled under closed- and open-loop conditions. The fear response to the predator is computed by two avoidance-related parameters: (1) the average distance between the replica and the fish and (2) the time spent by the focal fish in the half of the water column opposite to that occupied by the replica.Individual behaviour of individual fish in response to a replica predator. TE quantifies the response of the prey in terms of “stationary”, “swimming”, and “attacking” motion patterns of the predator.The zebrafish quickly adjusted their behaviour to avoid the predator’s attacks. TE revealed that the state of the robot affected the vertical position of the fish only in the closed-loop control condition and not its position in the horizontal axis.
Modulation of fear/anxiety
[99]Zebrafish (Danio rerio). Data from [100].Data from [100].Modelling of geotaxis by using spatial entropy (SE) to identify the extent of the volume of water occupied by a fish and in which part of the tank most of the activity takes place.Mathematical model to quantify geotaxis.
[101]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).2D videorecording of the interaction between the fish and a robotic replica of 4 zebrafish according to TE as affected by 3 concentrations of caffeine: 5, 25, and 50 mg/L and compared to an untreated controlIndividual response of individually placed fish to a replica of a shoal of conspecifics and its modulation by caffeine.The TE was always higher from the replica to the fish but only in fish exposed to at least 25–50 mg caffeine/mL the difference was significant.
[102]Zebrafish (Danio rerio) and a fear-eliciting stimulus consisting of three zebrafish replicas with a synchronized and polarized motion moving in 3D trajectories via a robotic platform.3D tracking of zebrafish responses in the presence and absence of ethanol (0, control group, and two test groups with 0.25% and 1.00% concentrations of ethanol/water), which is known to interfere with fear conditioning in different species.Modulation of conditioned fear-responses. Individual responses. The parameters assessed were avoidance index, geotaxis, freezing, spatial entropy (SE), and average speed, acceleration, and angular speed.Ethanol significantly lowered the intensity of anxiety-related behaviours (i.e., spatial avoidance and geotaxis). The effects on spatial entropy (SE) and average speed and acceleration were marginal, time-dependent, and only present at the highest dose (1.00%). There was no effect on freezing or average angular speed.
[103]Zebrafish (Danio rerio) and a robotic stimulus mimicking a sympatric predator of zebrafish (Indian pond heron, Ardeola grayii) programmed via a microcontroller that regularly struck the water’s surface in a lateral compartment.3D tracking of zebrafish behaviour in the presence and absence of two anxiolytic compounds. The experimental groups included: drug-free control groups and those treated with three different concentrations of citalopram (30, 50, and 100 mg/L) and ethanol (0.25%, 0.50%, and 1.00%).Modulation of fear/anxiety. Individual responses. The parameters assessed were avoidance index, geotaxis, freezing, spatial entropy (SE), and average speed, acceleration, and angular speed.In the absence of the conditioning stimulus, zebrafish displayed a conditioned geotaxis that was reduced in a linear dose–response-oriented manner by citalopram and in a U-shaped dose–response manner by ethanol.
Positive emotional contagion
[106]Zebrafish (Danio rerio).2D recording of zebrafish administered a solution of citalopram of 0 (control), 30, and 100 mg/L. The fish were tested individually and in groups of n = 5, where only 1 of them had been citalopram-treated.Testing of positive emotional contagion by analysing geotaxis, group cohesion, coordination, and causal interactions (by TE) for individuals (individually assessed) and for groups.Group cohesion and coordination were not affected by the treatment. Changes in geotaxis were consistent with alleviation of anxiety in citalopram-treated individuals and in groups where only one member was treated. TE indicated that emotional contagion was directional: the treated individual influenced untreated fish, but not vice versa
Recognition of abnormal behaviour
[37]Species not declaredVideo recording under real farming conditions and development of a method to detect abnormal behaviours.Detection of abnormal behaviour in real-life situation on a farm.Softmax cross-entropy loss and weight attenuation L2 regularization were used to optimize a convolution 3D model, which, consequently, proved to be able to successfully analyse real-life aquaculture videos and detect abnormal behaviours.

References

  1. FAO. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture 2022. Towards Blue Transformation; FAO: Rome, Italy, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. EATiP. A Review of the Strategic Research and Innovation Agenda. In Our Vision for the Future of European Aquaculture; European Aquaculture Technology and Innovation Platform: Liege, Belgium, 2017; Available online: https://eatip.eu/wp-content/uploads/2018/02/EATIP-SRIA-2017.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2023).
  3. Eguiraun, H.; Izagirre, U.; Martinez, I. A Paradigm Shift in Safe Seafood Production: From Contaminant Detection to Fish Monitoring—Application of Biological Warning Systems to Aquaculture. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2015, 43, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. EC. A Sustainable Food System for the European Union; 2020; ISBN 978-3-9820301-3-5. Available online: https://www.sapea.info/wp-content/uploads/sustainable-food-system-report.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2023).
  5. EU. Identification of Emerging Technologies and Breakthrough; 2022; ISBN 9789294691224. Available online: https://eic.ec.europa.eu/system/files/2022-02/EIC-Emerging-Tech-and-Breakthrough-Innov-report-2022-1502-final.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2023).
  6. Føre, M.; Frank, K.; Norton, T.; Svendsen, E.; Alfredsen, J.A.; Dempster, T.; Eguiraun, H.; Watson, W.; Stahl, A.; Sunde, L.M.; et al. Precision Fish Farming: A New Framework to Improve Production in Aquaculture. Biosyst. Eng. 2018, 173, 176–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Li, D.; Wang, G.; Du, L.; Zheng, Y.; Wang, Z. Recent Advances in Intelligent Recognition Methods for Fish Stress Behavior. Aquac. Eng. 2022, 96, 102222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Marques, T.A.; Thomas, L.; Martin, S.W.; Mellinger, D.K.; Ward, J.A.; Moretti, D.J.; Harris, D.; Tyack, P.L. Estimating Animal Population Density Using Passive Acoustics. Biol. Rev. 2013, 88, 287–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Føre, M.; Svendsen, E.; Alfredsen, J.A.; Uglem, I.; Bloecher, N.; Sveier, H.; Sunde, L.M.; Frank, K. Using Acoustic Telemetry to Monitor the Effects of Crowding and Delousing Procedures on Farmed Atlantic Salmon (Salmo salar). Aquaculture 2018, 495, 757–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rose, C.S.; Stoner, A.W.; Matteson, K. Use of High-Frequency Imaging Sonar to Observe Fish Behaviour near Baited Fishing Gears. Fish Res. 2005, 76, 291–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gesto, M.; Zupa, W.; Alfonso, S.; Spedicato, M.T.; Lembo, G.; Carbonara, P. Using Acoustic Telemetry to Assess Behavioral Responses to Acute Hypoxia and Ammonia Exposure in Farmed Rainbow Trout of Different Competitive Ability. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 230, 105084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Halvorsen, M.B.; Zeddies, D.G.; Ellison, W.T.; Chicoine, D.R.; Popper, A.N. Effects of Mid-Frequency Active Sonar on Hearing in Fish. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 599–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kruusmaa, M.; Gkliva, R.; Tuhtan, J.A.; Tuvikene, A.; Alfredsen, J.A. Salmon Behavioural Response to Robots in an Aquaculture Sea Cage. R Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 191220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Kruusmaa, M.; Rieucau, G.; Montoya, J.C.C.; Markna, R.; Handegard, N.O. Collective Responses of a Large Mackerel School Depend on the Size and Speed of a Robotic Fish but Not on Tail Motion. Bioinspir. Biomim. 2016, 11, 56020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Zhao, S.; Zhang, S.; Liu, J.; Wang, H.; Zhu, J.; Li, D.; Zhao, R. Application of Machine Learning in Intelligent Fish Aquaculture: A Review. Aquaculture 2021, 540, 736724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gladju, J.; Kamalam, B.S.; Kanagaraj, A. Applications of Data Mining and Machine Learning Framework in Aquaculture and Fisheries: A Review. Smart Agric. Technol. 2022, 2, 100061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Saberioon, M.; Gholizadeh, A.; Cisar, P.; Pautsina, A.; Urban, J. Application of Machine Vision Systems in Aquaculture with Emphasis on Fish: State-of-the-Art and Key Issues. Rev. Aquac. 2017, 9, 369–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Yang, L.; Liu, Y.; Yu, H.; Fang, X.; Song, L.; Li, D.; Chen, Y. Computer Vision Models in Intelligent Aquaculture with Emphasis on Fish Detection and Behavior Analysis: A Review. Arch. Comput. Methods Eng. 2021, 28, 2785–2816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wei, Y.; Wei, Q.; An, D. Intelligent Monitoring and Control Technologies of Open Sea Cage Culture: A Review. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2020, 169, 105119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Andrady, A.L. Microplastics in the Marine Environment. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 2011, 62, 1596–1605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Brausch, J.M.; Connors, K.A.; Brooks, B.W.; Rand, G.M. Human Pharmaceuticals in the Aquatic Environment: A Review of Recent Toxicological Studies and Considerations for Toxicity Testing. Rev. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 2012, 218, 1–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Brodin, T.; Piovano, S.; Fick, J.; Klaminder, J.; Heynen, M.; Jonsson, M. Ecological Effects of Pharmaceuticals in Aquatic Systems—Impacts through Behavioural Alterations. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Eguiraun, H.; López-de-Ipiña, K.; Martinez, I. Application of Entropy and Fractal Dimension Analyses to the Pattern Recognition of Contaminated Fish Responses in Aquaculture. Entropy 2014, 16, 6133–6151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Gavrilescu, M.; Demnerová, K.; Aamand, J.; Agathos, S.; Fava, F. Emerging Pollutants in the Environment: Present and Future Challenges in Biomonitoring, Ecological Risks and Bioremediation. N. Biotechnol. 2015, 32, 147–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Herbert-Read, J.E.; Kremer, L.; Bruintjes, R.; Radford, A.N.; Ioannou, C.C. Anthropogenic Noise Pollution from Pile-Driving Disrupts the Structure and Dynamics of Fish Shoals; 2017; Volume 284, ISBN 0000000154703. Available online: https://research-information.bris.ac.uk/ws/portalfiles/portal/134650896/20171627.full.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2023).
  26. Jacquin, L.; Petitjean, Q.; Côte, J.; Laffaille, P.; Jean, S. Effects of Pollution on Fish Behavior, Personality, and Cognition: Some Research Perspectives. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 8, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Llorca, M.; Farré, M. Current Insights into Potential Effects of Micro-Nanoplastics on Human Health by in-Vitro Tests. Front. Toxicol. 2021, 3, 752140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Mattsson, K.; Johnson, E.V.; Malmendal, A.; Linse, S.; Hansson, L.A.; Cedervall, T. Brain Damage and Behavioural Disorders in Fish Induced by Plastic Nanoparticles Delivered through the Food Chain. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 11452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Okoye, C.O.; Addey, C.I.; Oderinde, O.; Okoro, J.O.; Uwamungu, J.Y.; Ikechukwu, C.K.; Okeke, E.S.; Ejeromedoghene, O.; Odii, E.C. Toxic Chemicals and Persistent Organic Pollutants Associated with Micro-and Nanoplastics Pollution. Chem. Eng. J. Adv. 2022, 11, 100310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Suryanto, M.E.; Yang, C.-C.; Audira, G.; Vasquez, R.D.; Roldan, M.J.M.; Ger, T.; Hsiao, C. Evaluation of Locomotion Complexity in Zebrafish after Exposure to Twenty Antibiotics by Fractal Dimension and Entropy Analysis. Antibiotics 2022, 11, 1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Wang, B.; Zhu, J.; Wang, A.; Wang, J.; Wu, Y.; Yao, W. Early Detection of Cyanide, Organophosphate and Rodenticide Pollution Based on Locomotor Activity of Zebrafish Larvae. PeerJ 2021, 9, e12703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Weilgart, L.S. The Impacts of Anthropogenic Ocean Noise on Cetaceans and Implications for Management. Can. J. Zool. 2007, 85, 1091–1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bae, M.J.; Park, Y.S. Biological Early Warning System Based on the Responses of Aquatic Organisms to Disturbances: A Review. Sci. Total Environ. 2014, 466–467, 635–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Brodin, T.; Fick, J.; Jonsson, M.; Klaminder, J. Dilute Concentrations of a Psychiatric Drug Alter Behavior of Fish from Natural Populations. Science (1979) 2013, 339, 814–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Eguiraun, H.; Casquero, O.; Sørensen, A.J.; Martinez, I. Reducing the Number of Individuals to Monitor Shoaling Fish Systems—Application of the Shannon Entropy to Construct a Biological Warning System Model. Front. Physiol. 2018, 9, 493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Chase, I.D.; Tovey, C.; Spangler-Martin, D.; Manfredonia, M. Individual Differences versus Social Dynamics in the Formation of Animal Dominance Hierarchies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 5744–5749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Hu, Z.; Li, X.H.; Xie, X.Y.; Zhao, Y.C. Abnormal Behavior Recognition of Underwater Fish Body Based on C3D Model. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Machine Learning and Soft Computing, Haikou, China, 15–17 January 2022; pp. 92–97. Available online: https://dl.acm.org/doi/pdf/10.1145/3523150.3523165 (accessed on 21 March 2023). [CrossRef]
  38. Collignon, B.; Séguret, A.; Chemtob, Y.; Cazenille, L.; Halloy, J. Collective Departures and Leadership in Zebrafish. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0216798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Komareji, M.; Bouffanais, R. Resilience and Controllability of Dynamic Collective Behaviors. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e82578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Simpson, S.D.; Radford, A.N.; Nedelec, S.L.; Ferrari, M.C.O.; Chivers, D.P.; McCormick, M.I.; Meekan, M.G. Anthropogenic Noise Increases Fish Mortality by Predation. Nat. Commun. 2016, 7, 10544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Mwaffo, V.; Butail, S.; Porfiri, M. Analysis of Pairwise Interactions in a Maximum Likelihood Sense to Identify Leaders in a Group. Front. Robot. AI 2017, 4, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Nakayama, S.; Harcourt, J.L.; Johnstone, R.A.; Manica, A. Initiative, Personality and Leadership in Pairs of Foraging Fish. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e36606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Inada, Y.; Kawachi, K. Order and Flexibility in the Motion of Fish Schools. J. Theor. Biol. 2002, 214, 371–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Tikhonov, D.A.; Enderlein, J.; Malchow, H.; Medvinsky, A.B. Chaos and Fractals in Fish School Motion. Chaos Solitons Fractals 2001, 12, 277–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tikhonov, D.A.; Malchow, H. Chaos and Fractals in Fish School Motion, II. Chaos Solitons Fractals 2003, 16, 287–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Neumeister, H.; Cellucci, C.J.; Rapp, P.E.; Korn, H.; Faber, D.S. Dynamical Analysis Reveals Individuality of Locomotion in Goldfish. J. Exp. Biol. 2004, 207, 697–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Mandelbrot, B.B. The Fractal Geometry of Nature; W.H. Freeman: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1982; ISBN 978-0716711865. [Google Scholar]
  48. Zmeskal, O.; Dzik, P.; Vesely, M. Entropy of Fractal Systems. Comput. Math. Appl. 2013, 66, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Schreiber, T. Measuring Information Transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2000, 85, 461–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Butail, S.; Mwaffo, V.; Porfiri, M. Model-Free Information-Theoretic Approach to Infer Leadership in Pairs of Zebrafish. Phys. Rev. E 2016, 93, 042411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Mann, R.P.; Garnett, R. The Entropic Basis of Collective Behaviour. J. R. Soc. Interface 2015, 12, 20150037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kitano, H. Computational Systems Biology. Nature 2002, 420, 206–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Spasic, S.; Savic, A.; Nikolic, L.; Budimir, S.; Janosevic, D.; Mitrovic, A. Applications of Higuchi’s Fractal Dimension in the Analysis of Biological Signals. In Proceedings of the 2012 20th Telecommunications Forum, TELFOR 2012—Proceedings, Belgrade, Serbia, 20–22 November 2012; pp. 639–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Costa, M.; Goldberger, A.L.; Peng, C.K. Multiscale Entropy Analysis of Biological Signals. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter. Phys. 2005, 71, 021906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Shannon, C.E. A Mathematical Theory of Communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 1948, 27, 379–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Mandelbrot, B. How Long Is the Coast of Britain? Statistical Self-Similarity and Fractional Dimension. Science (1979) 1967, 156, 636–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Kith, K.; Sourina, O.; Kulish, V.; Khoa, N.M. An Algorithm for Fractal Dimension Calculation Based on Renyi Entropy for Short Time Signal Analysis. In Proceedings of the ICICS 2009—Conference Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Information, Communications and Signal Processing, Macau, China, 8–10 December 2009; pp. 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ezeiza, A.; López de Ipiña, K.; Hernández, C.; Barroso, N. Enhancing the Feature Extraction Process for Automatic Speech Recognition with Fractal Dimensions. Cognit. Comput. 2013, 5, 545–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sekine, M.; Tamura, T.; Akay, M.; Fujimoto, T.; Togawa, T.; Fukui, Y. Discrimination of Walking Patterns Using Wavelet-Based Fractal Analysis. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2002, 10, 188–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Alados, C.L.; Escos, J.M.; Emlen, J.M. Fractal Structure of Sequential Behaviour Patterns: An Indicator of Stress. Anim. Behav. 1996, 51, 437–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Chen, T.; Lihong, X.; Shijing, L. Fine-Grained Classification Algorithm of Fish Feeding State Based on Optical Flow Method. Nongye Gongcheng Xuebao/Trans. Chin. Soc. Agric. Eng. 2021, 37, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Iqbal, U.; Li, D.; Akhter, M. Intelligent Diagnosis of Fish Behavior Using Deep Learning Method. Fishes 2022, 7, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kong, Q.; Du, R.; Duan, Q.; Zhang, Y.; Chen, Y.; Li, D.; Xu, C.; Li, W.; Liu, C. A Recurrent Network Based on Active Learning for the Assessment of Fish Feeding Status. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2022, 198, 106979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Coughlin, D.J.; Strickler, J.R.; Sanderson, B. Swimming and Search Behaviour in Clownfish, Amphiprion perideraion, Larvae. Anim. Behav. 1992, 44, 427–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Mahjoub, M.S.; Souissi, S.; Schmitt, F.G.; Nan, F.H.; Hwang, J.S. Anisotropy and Shift of Search Behavior in Malabar Grouper (Epinephelus malabaricus) Larvae in Response to Prey Availability. Hydrobiologia 2011, 666, 215–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Sims, D.W.; Southall, E.J.; Humphries, N.E.; Hays, G.C.; Bradshaw, C.J.A.; Pitchford, J.W.; James, A.; Ahmed, M.Z.; Brierley, A.S.; Hindell, M.A.; et al. Scaling Laws of Marine Predator Search Behaviour. Nature 2008, 451, 1098–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Zhang, B.; Xie, F.; Han, F. Fish Population Status Detection Based on Deep Learning System. In Proceedings of the 2019 IEEE International Conference on Mechatronics and Automation, ICMA 2019, Tianjin, China, 4–7 August 2019; pp. 81–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Surowiecki, J. The Wisdom Of Crowds: Why the Many Are Smarter than the Few and How Collective Wisdom Shapes Business, Economics, Society and Nations; 1st Abacus Edition; Little Brown: Boston, MA, USA, 2004; ISBN 978-0349116051. [Google Scholar]
  69. Wissner-Gross, A.D.; Freer, C.E. Causal Entropic Forces. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2013, 110, 168702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Zhao, J.; Gu, Z.; Shi, M.; Lu, H.; Li, J.; Shen, M.; Ye, Z.; Zhu, S. Spatial Behavioral Characteristics and Statistics-Based Kinetic Energy Modeling in Special Behaviors Detection of a Shoal of Fish in a Recirculating Aquaculture System. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2016, 127, 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tunstrøm, K.; Katz, Y.; Ioannou, C.C.; Huepe, C.; Lutz, M.J.; Couzin, I.D. Collective States, Multistability and Transitional Behavior in Schooling Fish. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2013, 9, e1002915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Crosato, E.; Jiang, L.; Lecheval, V.; Lizier, J.T.; Wang, X.R.; Tichit, P.; Theraulaz, G.; Prokopenko, M. Informative and Misinformative Interactions in a School of Fish. Swarm Intell. 2018, 12, 283–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Calovi, D.S.; Lopez, U.; Schuhmacher, P.; Chaté, H.; Sire, C.; Theraulaz, G. Collective Response to Perturbations in a Data-Driven Fish School Model. J. R. Soc. Interface 2015, 12, 20141362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Kushida, K.; Fukuda, K.; Sugino, R.; Morizumi, N. Computational Algorithm for Fish Schooling Using Bitmap Image. In Proceedings of the SICE Annual Conference, Nagoya, Japan, 14–17 September 2013; pp. 937–942. [Google Scholar]
  75. Hiramatsu, K.; Shikasho, S.; Mori, K. Mathematical Modeling of Fish Schooling of Japanese Medaka Using Basic Behavioral Patterns. J. Fac. Agric. Kyushu Univ. 2000, 45, 237–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Suzuki, K.; Takagi, T.; Hiraishi, T. Video Analysis of Fish Schooling Behavior in Finite Space Using a Mathematical Model. Fish Res. 2003, 60, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Higuchi, T. Approach to an Irregular Time Series on the Basis of the Fractal Theory. Physica D 1988, 31, 277–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wark, A.R.; Wark, B.J.; Lageson, T.J.; Peichel, C.L. Novel Methods for Discriminating Behavioral Differences between Stickleback Individuals and Populations in a Laboratory Shoaling Assay. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011, 65, 1147–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Schaerf, T.M.; Dillingham, P.W.; Ward, A.J.W. The Effects of External Cues on Individual and Collective Behavior of Shoaling Fish. Sci. Adv. 2017, 7, e1603201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Heras, F.J.H.; Romero-Ferrero, F.; Hinz, R.C.; De Polavieja, G.G. Deep Attention Networks Reveal the Rules of Collective Motion in Zebrafish. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2019, 15, e1007354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Wilson, A.D.M.; Burns, A.L.J.; Crosato, E.; Lizier, J.; Prokopenko, M.; Schaerf, T.M.; Ward, A.J.W. Conformity in the Collective: Differences in Hunger Affect Individual and Group Behavior in a Shoaling Fish. Behav. Ecol. 2019, 30, 968–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. King, A.J.; Johnson, D.D.P.; Van Vugt, M. The Origins and Evolution of Leadership. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, R911–R916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Petit, O.; Bon, R. Decision-Making Processes: The Case of Collective Movements. Behav. Process. 2010, 84, 635–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Pérez-Escudero, A.; Vicente-Page, J.; Hinz, R.C.; Arganda, S.; De Polavieja, G.G. IdTracker: Tracking Individuals in a Group by Automatic Identification of Unmarked Animals. Nat. Methods 2014, 11, 743–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Niizato, T.; Sakamoto, K.; Mototake, Y.I.; Murakami, H.; Tomaru, T.; Hoshika, T.; Fukushima, T. Finding Continuity and Discontinuity in Fish Schools via Integrated Information Theory. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0229573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Ward, A.J.W.; Schaerf, T.M.; Burns, A.L.J.; Lizier, J.T.; Crosato, E.; Prokopenko, M.; Webster, M.M. Cohesion, Order and Information Flow in the Collective Motion of Mixed-Species Shoals. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2018, 5, 181132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Brennan, N.P.; Leber, K.M.; Blankenship, H.L.; Ransier, J.M.; DeBruler, R. An Evaluation of Coded Wire and Elastomer Tag Performance in Juvenile Common Snook under Field and Laboratory Conditions. N. Am. J. Fish Manag. 2005, 25, 437–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Ruberto, T.; Clément, R.J.G.; Spinello, C.; Neri, D.; MacRì, S.; Porfiri, M. The Tagging Procedure of Visible Implant Elastomers Influences Zebrafish Individual and Social Behavior. Zebrafish 2018, 15, 433–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Frommen, J.G.; Hanak, S.; Schmidl, C.A.; Thünken, T. Visible Implant Elastomer Tagging Influences Social Preferences of Zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behaviour 2015, 152, 1765–1777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Deakin, A.G.; Spencer, J.W.; Cossins, A.R.; Young, I.S.; Sneddon, L.U. Welfare Challenges Influence the Complexity of Movement: Fractal Analysis of Behaviour in Zebrafish. Fishes 2019, 4, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Audira, G.; Suryanto, M.E.; Chen, K.H.C.; Vasquez, R.D.; Roldan, M.J.M.; Yang, C.C.; Der Hsiao, C.; Huang, J.C. Acute and Chronic Effects of Fin Amputation on Behavior Performance of Adult Zebrafish in 3D Locomotion Test Assessed with Fractal Dimension and Entropy Analyses and Their Relationship to Fin Regeneration. Biology 2022, 11, 969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Zhang, Z.; Fu, Y.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, X.; Chen, S. A Comparative Study on Two Territorial Fishes: The Influence of Physical Enrichment on Aggressive Behavior. Animals 2021, 11, 1868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Näslund, J.; Rosengren, M.; Del Villar, D.; Gansel, L.; Norrgård, J.R.; Persson, L.; Winkowski, J.J.; Kvingedal, E. Hatchery Tank Enrichment Affects Cortisol Levels and Shelter-Seeking in Atlantic Salmon (Salmo salar). Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 2013, 70, 585–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Butail, S.; Ladu, F.; Spinello, D.; Porfiri, M. Information Flow in Animal-Robot Interactions. Entropy 2014, 16, 1315–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Bartolini, T.; Mwaffo, V.; Showler, A.; Macrì, S.; Butail, S.; Porfiri, M. Zebrafish Response to 3D Printed Shoals of Conspecifics: The Effect of Body Size. Bioinspir. Biomim. 2016, 11, 026003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Neri, D.; Ruberto, T.; Cord-Cruz, G.; Porfiri, M. Information Theory and Robotics Meet to Study Predator-Prey Interactions. Chaos Interdiscip. J. Nonlinear Sci. 2017, 27, 073111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Hu, F.; Nie, L.J.; Fu, S.J. Information Dynamics in the Interaction between a Prey and a Predator Fish. Entropy 2015, 17, 7230–7241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Spinello, C.; Yang, Y.; Macrì, S.; Porfiri, M. Zebrafish Adjust Their Behavior in Response to an Interactive Robotic Predator. Front. Robot. AI 2019, 6, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Burbano, D.A.L.; Porfiri, M. Modeling Zebrafish Geotaxis as a Feedback Control Process. In Proceedings of the American Control Conference 2021, New Orleans, LA, USA, 25–28 May 2021; pp. 660–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Macrì, S.; Clément, R.J.G.; Spinello, C.; Porfiri, M. Comparison between Two- and Three-Dimensional Scoring of Zebrafish Response to Psychoactive Drugs: Identifying When Three-Dimensional Analysis Is Needed. PeerJ 2019, 7, e7893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Ladu, F.; Mwaffo, V.; Li, J.; Macrì, S.; Porfiri, M. Acute Caffeine Administration Affects Zebrafish Response to a Robotic Stimulus. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 289, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Macrì, S.; Karakaya, M.; Spinello, C.; Porfiri, M. Zebrafish Exhibit Associative Learning for an Aversive Robotic Stimulus. Lab. Anim. 2020, 49, 259–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Clément, R.J.G.; Macrì, S.; Porfiri, M. Design and Development of a Robotic Predator as a Stimulus in Conditioned Place Aversion for the Study of the Effect of Ethanol and Citalopram in Zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 2020, 378, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Cianca, V.; Bartolini, T.; Porfiri, M.; Macrì, S. A Robotics-Based Behavioral Paradigm to Measure Anxiety-Related Responses in Zebrafish. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e69661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  105. Nakahashi, W.; Ohtsuki, H. When Is Emotional Contagion Adaptive? J. Theor. Biol. 2015, 380, 480–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Burbano Lombana, D.A.; Macrì, S.; Porfiri, M. Collective Emotional Contagion in Zebrafish. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 730372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Schematic diagram indicating the subjects targeted by the review and how they would fit into a PFF framework.
Figure 1. Schematic diagram indicating the subjects targeted by the review and how they would fit into a PFF framework.
Entropy 25 00559 g001
Table 1. Search strings used in the Scopus database and number of results obtained on 26 January 2023.
Table 1. Search strings used in the Scopus database and number of results obtained on 26 January 2023.
QueriesDocuments
(TITLE-ABS-KEY (fish AND behav*) AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (fractal* OR entropy)) 143
(TITLE-ABS-KEY (aquacult*) AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (fractal* OR entropy)) 87
(TITLE-ABS-KEY ("Fish behavio*") AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (entropy)) 11
(TITLE-ABS-KEY ("Fish behavio*") AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (fractal)) 9
(TITLE-ABS-KEY ("collective behaviour" OR "collective behavior") AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (fish) AND TITLE-ABS-KEY (welfare OR stress* OR health OR disease)) 23
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Eguiraun, H.; Martinez, I. Entropy and Fractal Techniques for Monitoring Fish Behaviour and Welfare in Aquacultural Precision Fish Farming—A Review. Entropy 2023, 25, 559. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25040559

AMA Style

Eguiraun H, Martinez I. Entropy and Fractal Techniques for Monitoring Fish Behaviour and Welfare in Aquacultural Precision Fish Farming—A Review. Entropy. 2023; 25(4):559. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25040559

Chicago/Turabian Style

Eguiraun, Harkaitz, and Iciar Martinez. 2023. "Entropy and Fractal Techniques for Monitoring Fish Behaviour and Welfare in Aquacultural Precision Fish Farming—A Review" Entropy 25, no. 4: 559. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25040559

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop