Next Article in Journal
Constructing Direct Z-Scheme Y2TmSbO7/GdYBiNbO7 Heterojunction Photocatalyst with Enhanced Photocatalytic Degradation of Acetochlor under Visible Light Irradiation
Next Article in Special Issue
Steroidomics in Men with Schizophrenia
Previous Article in Journal
Ultrastructural Characterization of PBMCs and Extracellular Vesicles in Multiple Sclerosis: A Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Disgust Sensitivity during Pregnancy and Endogenous Steroids: A Longitudinal Study

1
Department of Philosophy and History of Science, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Viničná 7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic
2
Department of Steroids and Proteofactors, Institute of Endocrinology, Národní 8, 116 94 Prague, Czech Republic
3
Department of Molecular Endocrinology, Institute of Endocrinology, Národní 8, 116 94 Prague, Czech Republic
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25(13), 6857; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25136857
Submission received: 7 April 2024 / Revised: 18 June 2024 / Accepted: 20 June 2024 / Published: 22 June 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Steroids and Neuropsychiatric Disorders)

Abstract

:
The emotion of disgust protects individuals against pathogens, and it has been found to be elevated during pregnancy. Physiological mechanisms discussed in relation to these changes include immune markers and progesterone levels. This study aimed to assess the association between steroids and disgust sensitivity in pregnancy. Using a prospective longitudinal design, we analyzed blood serum steroid concentrations and measured disgust sensitivity via text-based questionnaires in a sample of 179 pregnant women during their first and third trimesters. We found positive correlations between disgust sensitivity and the levels of C19 steroids (including testosterone) and its precursors in the Δ5 pathway (androstenediol, DHEA, and their sulfates) and the Δ4 pathway (androstenedione). Additionally, positive correlations were observed with 5α/β-reduced C19 steroid metabolites in both trimesters. In the first trimester, disgust sensitivity was positively associated with 17-hydroxypregnanolone and with some estrogens. In the third trimester, positive associations were observed with cortisol and immunoprotective Δ5 C19 7α/β-hydroxy-steroids. Our findings show that disgust sensitivity is positively correlated with immunomodulatory steroids, and in the third trimester, with steroids which may be related to potential maternal-anxiety-related symptoms. This study highlights the complex relationship between hormonal changes and disgust sensitivity during pregnancy.

1. Introduction

In recent decades, there has been a growing interest in a concept at the intersection of psychology and immunology: the behavioral immune system. This system comprises a network of psychological mechanisms that serve as the first line of defense against potential pathogens from the environment. It refers to the series of adaptive behaviors that humans exhibit in response to various infectious threats. The affective part of the behavioral immune system consists of the emotion of disgust [1], which is linked to the avoidance of potentially harmful stimuli [2].
Regarding disgust sensitivity, one can observe both interindividual and intraindividual differences. It has been shown that disgust sensitivity changes during ontogenesis [3,4], and differences in disgust sensitivity have also been observed between men and women [2,5]. Moreover, disgust sensitivity can be influenced by numerous factors, including activity of the immune system [6,7], the presence of pathogens in the environment [8], reproductive status [9,10], and psychosocial influences [11,12]. Based on the premise of this variability in disgust sensitivity, the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis has been proposed [13], assuming that the individual level of sensitivity to disgust is modulated depending on the current degree of immunosuppression.
Originally, the hypothesis was developed within the context of fluctuations in the levels of progesterone, a hormone believed to have immunosuppressive effects, and the consequent impact on immunosuppression during the menstrual cycle [14]. During the luteal phase, when progesterone levels peak, increased disgust sensitivity is believed to compensate for progesterone-associated immunosuppression. In subsequent years, numerous studies have tested the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis in the context of the menstrual cycle and in relation to changes in the levels of progesterone (and other hormones). The reported findings did not, however, form a consistent pattern.
Some studies that investigated this hypothesis focused on changes in disgust sensitivity during the menstrual cycle, especially between the different phases of the cycle. The first cross-sectional study to investigate this hypothesis found no differences between the follicular and luteal phases of the cycle [13]. Similarly, subsequent cross-sectional studies found no support for the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. One of these studies used video stimuli to trigger disgust responses [15], while the other employed two different text-based questionnaires to measure disgust [16]. Additionally, two longitudinal studies, one of which again used video stimuli [17] and the other used a text-based questionnaire [18], also failed to find evidence supporting this hypothesis. The results of a cross-sectional study conducted by Rafiee et al. [19], which observed the relationship between estimated levels of progesterone and estradiol and pathogen-related disgust, also found no supporting evidence. On the other hand, the results of two different longitudinal studies do support the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis: they found that women had higher disgust sensitivity during the luteal phase than during menstruation [20] or the follicular phase [21]. A cross-sectional study also found elevated disgust sensitivity during the luteal phase in a subsample of women who recently had an infection [22]. In a recent study, women in the luteal phase displayed a more negative attitude and higher sensitivity to disgust-related phrases compared to women tested during the follicular phase or menstruation [23].
Changes in disgust sensitivity during the menstrual cycle have also been observed in direct relation to hormone levels. The currently leading hypothesis claims that higher levels of progesterone are associated with higher disgust sensitivity. The first cross-sectional study found a positive association between the levels of salivary progesterone and disgust sensitivity to visual stimuli [24]. In a longitudinal study, progesterone levels in the serum positively correlated with disgust sensitivity but only during the mid-luteal phase [20]. While not directly measuring disgust sensitivity, two studies focused on the processing of disgusted facial expressions. Authors of the first study found a positive association between the levels of salivary progesterone and a higher tendency to perceive disgusted faces with averted gaze as more intense, which could signify an increased sensitivity to facial cues signaling a nearby presence of a pathogenic threat [25]. Another study found a negative association between salivary estradiol levels and the overall processing of disgusted faces with direct gaze, which is thought to express direct communication of disgust over violation of moral norms [26]. The authors suggest that progesterone and estradiol could modulate the perception of disgusted faces differently depending on the direction of gaze. There is some evidence for a link between progesterone and disgust sensitivity in animal models as well: a reappraisal of data from a recent study on mice by Kavaliers et al. [27] has shown that an injection of progesterone given to females increased disgust towards infected males (as measured via the frequency of females avoiding the odor of infected males) [28].
On the other hand, multiple studies did not find the expected association, including Timmers et al. [17], who found no association between changes in the levels of salivary progesterone and changes in self-reported disgust between the follicular and luteal phases of the cycle. A longitudinal study that measured salivary progesterone, estradiol, testosterone, and cortisol found no association between either of the hormones and disgust sensitivity [29]. Another recent longitudinal study likewise measured the levels of salivary progesterone, estradiol, testosterone, and cortisol and found no association with disgust sensitivity, neither in within-subject nor in between-subject analyses [18].
Simultaneously with the aforementioned research, Fessler et al. [9] extended the investigation of the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis to pregnant women. They hypothesized that disgust sensitivity would be elevated during the first trimester of pregnancy; it would be consistent with older theories which assumed that women’s immune system is suppressed during this phase of pregnancy. While their assumptions were confirmed in both their cross-sectional study [9] and in another longitudinal study [30], it is essential to recognize how our understanding of immunosuppression in early pregnancy has evolved. Recent findings indicate that during early pregnancy, the maternal immune system undergoes complex immunomodulation, with some processes being suppressed and others (such as inflammatory processes) being elevated [31,32,33]. Despite that—and in accordance with the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis—increased susceptibility to disgust was recently observed in early pregnancy in women with lower levels of certain cytokines, in women whose immune system is probably insufficiently activated [7], and in those who had lower maternal serum levels of free β-human chorionic gonadotropin (associated with pregnancy-induced immunotolerance) [34]. During the first trimester, a higher disgust sensitivity was also observed in women who reported recent health problems [10] or in association with increased concentrations of pathogens in the environment, such as during the COVID-19 pandemic [8]. Moreover, a recent study by Dlouhá et al. [16] showed a significantly higher disgust sensitivity in women during early pregnancy compared with non-pregnant childless controls, which indicates that higher disgust sensitivity in pregnancy may provide protection during a period that is sensitive to fetal neurodevelopmental disruptions.
Although two studies [9,30] found higher disgust sensitivity during the first trimester than in later pregnancy, Dlouhá et al. [10], in a recent longitudinal study, found increasing disgust sensitivity throughout pregnancy and even after birth. The authors of the study discussed a possible association with increasing progesterone during pregnancy [14], but progesterone levels decrease after childbirth, which is not consistent with the reported further increase in disgust sensitivity during the postpartum period. The increase might be attributed to a more intense need for protection against infections towards the end of pregnancy due to the approaching childbirth and subsequent care for the newborn. Another potential interpretation of these findings revolves around a positive correlation between disgust sensitivity and negative affectivity. Existing research indicates that disgust is linked to affective states such as phobias [35,36], depression [11], or anxiety [37] and that alterations in disgust levels coincide with changes in the symptoms associated with contamination-based obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) [12]. It is known that late pregnancy is associated with increased anxiety symptoms [38], and elevated fear of death has been observed in the third trimester [39] and during labor [40]. It has been shown that these factors, along with anxiety, are related to postpartum anxiety [41].
It is apparent from the above that the relation between hormone levels and changes in disgust sensitivity, especially in regularly cycling women, has been repeatedly discussed and that the results are inconsistent. On the other hand, in the context of pregnancy, although it is a period of significant hormonal and immunological changes, only one study to date has explored the influence of hormones on disgust sensitivity [34]. To better understand the association between hormonal changes and changes in disgust sensitivity and to shed more light on the proximal mechanisms of disgust regulation during pregnancy, the main aim of this study was to test a broad range of steroid hormones in the first trimester and then again in the third trimester of pregnancy and to analyze their relationships with simultaneously measured disgust sensitivity. We focused on associations between disgust sensitivity and the levels of hormones with immunomodulatory effects, such as progesterone, testosterone, cortisol, estradiol, or 7-oxygenated (7α-hydroxy, 7β-hydroxy, and 7-oxo) and 16α-hydroxy-derivatives of adrenal androgens dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and 5-androstene-3β,17β-diol [42,43]. We have also carried out an explorative analysis regarding possible associations between disgust and the levels of other endogenous steroids. Previous research that investigated the circulating steroids accompanying various mental disturbances [43] suggests that the relationship between disgust levels, immunity, and psychological status may also be modulated by, for instance, estrogen levels, reduced sulfoconjugation of steroids, 7α-, 7β-, and 16α-hydroxy-metabolites of C19 Δ5 steroids, or 5α/β-reduced pregnane steroids.

2. Results

2.1. Descriptive Statistics

The final sample consisted of 179 women aged between 21 and 44 (Mean = 31.5, SD = 4.27), out of whom 109 (60.9%) were primiparous and 75 (41.9%) were pregnant with a male fetus. Most women had a university degree (78.8%) and were married (54.7%). A total of 8 (5%) women had hypertension, and 19 (11.9%) women had gestational diabetes mellitus. Out of the sample, 16 women (8.9%) reported that they smoked regularly before pregnancy and 18 women (10.1%) only occasionally. The levels of steroids quantified in the circulation of pregnant women in the first and third trimesters are reported in Table 1.
Disgust sensitivity was assessed via the Disgust Scale-Revised (DS-R) [44] and the Pathogen domain of the Three Domains of Disgust Scale (TDDS) [45] (for more details, see the Material and Methods section). The mean DS-R/TDDS scores and internal consistencies are shown in Table 2.

2.2. Association between Disgust Sensitivity and Steroid Levels in the First Trimester

We assessed the association between disgust sensitivity and steroid levels in the first trimester of pregnancy. In the orthogonal projections to latent structures (OPLS) model for the overall DS-R score, disgust was significantly predicted by a broad spectrum of steroids, such that higher levels of androstenediol, 17-hydroxypregnanolone, 5β-pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol, 5α-androstane-3,17-dione, androsterone, androsterone sulfate, epiandrosterone, epiandrosterone sulfate, 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol, 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol, 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and 3α,5β-tetrahydrocorticosterone and lower levels of estrone predicted higher overall DS-R score. This model explained 9.8% (6.8% after cross-validation) of the overall DS-R score variability (Table 3).
The model for the Core disgust subscale revealed that higher levels of androstenediol, testosterone, 5α-dihydrotestosterone, 5α-androstane-3,17-dione, androsterone, androsterone sulfate, epiandrosterone sulfate, 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol and lower levels of conjugated pregnanolone predicted higher scores of Core disgust. This model explained 11.4% (9.7% after cross-validation) of the Core disgust score variability (Table 3, Figure 1).
The model for the Contamination disgust subscale revealed that higher subscale scores were predicted by higher levels of estrone, androsterone sulfate, epiandrosterone sulfate, etiocholanolone sulfate, epietiocholanolone sulfate, and conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol. This model explained 8.2% (5.2% after cross-validation) of the Contamination disgust score variability (Table 3, Figure 1).
The model for the Animal reminder disgust subscale showed that the scores of this subscale were positively associated with the following steroids: testosterone, estrone, estradiol, 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, conjugated 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and 5α-androstane-3,17-dione. This model explained 9.2% (7.4% after cross-validation) of the Animal reminder disgust score variability (Table 3, Figure 1).
In the OPLS model for the Pathogen disgust score of the TDDS, higher disgust was significantly predicted by higher levels of 5α-dihydrotestosterone, estradiol, 5β-pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol, conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol, and 5α-androstane-3,17-dione and lower levels of 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone. This model explained 16.6% (13.5% after cross-validation) of the Pathogen disgust score variability (Table 4, Figure 1).
Regarding the covariates, no covariates (maternal age, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, pregnancy length, maternal weight gain, parity, fetal sex, maternal diabetes and hypertension, maternal pre-pregnancy smoking) contributed to the explanation of the variability of the disgust scores measured by both the DS-R and the Pathogen domain of the TDDS.

2.3. Association between Disgust Sensitivity and Steroid Levels in the Third Trimester

We also assessed the association between disgust sensitivity and steroid levels in the third trimester of pregnancy. In the OPLS model for the overall DS-R score, disgust was significantly predicted by higher levels of 7α-hydroxy-DHEA, androstenediol, 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and cortisol. This model explained 11.7% (8.8% after cross-validation) of the overall DS-R score variability (Table 5).
The model for the Core disgust subscale revealed that higher levels of pregnenolone, 17-hydroxypregnenolone, DHEA, 7α-hydroxy-DHEA, 7β-hydroxy-DHEA, androstenediol, 5α-androstane-3α,7α,17β-triol, 5α-dihydrotestosterone, epiandrosterone, and 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol predicted higher scores of Core disgust. This model explained 11.7% (8.8% after cross-validation) of the Core disgust score variability (Table 5, Figure 2).
The model for the Contamination disgust subscale revealed that higher subscale scores were predicted by higher levels of pregnenolone, conjugated pregnanolone, conjugated epipregnanolone, conjugated 5α,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, conjugated 5α-pregnane-3α,20α-diol, conjugated 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, conjugated 5β-pregnane-3β,20α-diol, and lower levels of testosterone and estradiol sulfate. This model explained 32.5% (23.1% after cross-validation) of the Contamination disgust score variability (Table 5, Figure 2).
The model for the Animal reminder disgust subscale showed that the scores of this subscale were positively associated with the following steroids: epiandrosterone, 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol, 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol, conjugated 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol. This model explained 10% (7.8% after cross-validation) of the Animal reminder disgust score variability (Table 5, Figure 2).
In the OPLS model for the Pathogen disgust score of the TDDS, higher disgust was significantly predicted by higher levels of DHEA, DHEA sulfate, androstenediol, androsterone sulfate, epiandrosterone, epiandrosterone sulfate, and 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol. This model explained 13% (11.1% after cross-validation) of the Pathogen disgust score variability (Table 6, Figure 2).
As for the covariates, only maternal age and parity contributed to the explanation of the variability of the Contamination disgust subscale score, such that older women, as well as multiparous women, reported higher levels of Contamination disgust.

2.4. The Effect of Changes in Steroid Levels during Pregnancy on Changes in Disgust Sensitivity

Finally, we assessed the association between the delta scores of disgust sensitivity (Δdisgust) and delta steroids (Δsteroids) calculated as the levels measured in the third trimester minus the levels measured in the first trimester. In the input models, the Δdisgust sensitivity was represented by vector Y. Matrix X was constituted by Δsteroids, steroid levels, and relevant disgust sensitivity measured in the first trimester of pregnancy (representing the baseline), as well as the related variables of maternal age, maternal BMI before pregnancy and ΔBMI, pregnancy length (both in the first trimester and Δ), maternal weight gain (both in the first trimester and Δ), parity, fetal sex, maternal diabetes and hypertension, and maternal pre-pregnancy smoking.
In the OPLS model for Δoverall DS-R score, higher Δdisgust was significantly predicted by higher values of ΔDHEA sulfate, Δandrostenediol, Δ5α-dihydrotestosterone, Δandrosterone, and Δ5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol, and also by lower levels of the overall DS-R score and by a broad spectrum of steroids, both measured in the first trimester (Table 7). As for the role of the covariates, older women and those who had a longer pregnancy at the time of the first trimester measurement were significantly positively associated with higher values of Δoverall DS-R score. This model explained 23.7% (19.3% after cross-validation) of Δoverall DS-R disgust score variability.
ΔCore disgust was significantly predicted by lower levels of overall DS-R score in the first trimester and by lower levels of steroids measured in the first trimester: 17-hydroxyprogesterone, 16α-hydroxyprogesterone, androstenedione, testosterone, 17-hydroxypregnanolone, 5α-androstane-3,17-dione, androsterone, androsterone sulfate, epiandrosterone sulfate, epietiocholanolone sulfate, 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol. This model explained 12.4% (9.9% after cross-validation) of ΔCore disgust score variability (Table 7).
The model for ΔContamination disgust revealed that higher values of ΔContamination disgust were negatively associated with Δandrostenediol sulfate and also with many steroids measured in the first trimester (Table 7). Moreover, older women and those who had longer Δpregnancy length had higher ΔContamination disgust. The model for ΔContamination disgust explained 29.7% (24.9% after cross-validation) of ΔContamination disgust score variability.
In the model for ΔAnimal reminder disgust, higher Δdisgust was significantly predicted by higher values of Δ5α-dihydrotestosterone, Δ5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol, and Δpregnancy length, and again by lower levels of many steroids (e.g., testosterone, estradiol, androsterone) measured in the first trimester and also by lower levels of the Animal reminder disgust in the first trimester (Table 7). Moreover, women who had a longer pregnancy at the time of the first trimester measurement had significantly higher values of ΔAnimal reminder disgust. This model explained 19.2% (15.3% after cross-validation) of ΔAnimal reminder score variability.
In the OPLS model for ΔPathogen disgust score of the TDDS, higher Δdisgust was significantly predicted by higher values of pregnenolone (measured in the first trimester), Δestrone sulfate, Δestradiol sulfate, and Δestriol sulfate. Moreover, lower levels of Pathogen disgust measured in the first trimester significantly predicted higher ΔPathogen disgust. This model explained 14% (11.2% after cross-validation) of ΔPathogen disgust score variability (Table 8). No covariate was associated with ΔPathogen disgust.

3. Discussion

In order to better understand the physiological mechanisms involved in the changes in disgust sensitivity during pregnancy, we have investigated associations between a broad spectrum of steroids and disgust sensitivity in the first and third trimesters of pregnancy. In both trimesters, we found mainly positive correlations between disgust sensitivity and C19 steroids (androgens reflecting the activity of maternal adrenal zona reticularis), including the active androgens testosterone and 5α-dihydrotestosterone (both C19 Δ4 steroids). We have also observed associations between disgust sensitivity and androstenediol and DHEA (or their sulfates) as the precursors of active androgens in the Δ5 pathway, as well as androstenedione as a precursor in the Δ4 pathway. Although these positive associations were observed in both trimesters, they were significantly more pronounced in the first trimester for the C19 Δ4 steroids and in the third trimester for the C19 Δ5 steroids. In both trimesters, we have also observed positive correlations between disgust sensitivity and 5α/β-reduced metabolites of the C19 steroids.
In the first trimester, there was a positive association between disgust sensitivity, 17-hydroxypregnanolone, and estrogens such as estradiol and estrone (with the exception of the DS-R overall score, where we found a negative correlation with estrone). In addition to the substances mentioned above, we also found positive correlations between disgust sensitivity in the first trimester and the levels of certain C21 5α/β-reduced steroids, which are primarily derived from placental progesterone during pregnancy. In the third trimester, we found a positive association between disgust sensitivity and cortisol.
In line with our hypotheses, we therefore found that disgust sensitivity positively correlated with the levels of steroids that have immunomodulatory effects, such as testosterone, cortisol, estradiol, or 7α/β-hydroxy-, 7-oxo-derivatives of adrenal androgens, DHEA, and androstenediol (e.g., [42,43]). We have also confirmed the predicted association between disgust sensitivity and steroids such as estrogens, testosterone, cortisol, 7α-, 7β-, and 16α-hydroxy-metabolites of C19 Δ5 steroids, and 5α/β-reduced pregnane steroids, which were observed to be associated with mental wellbeing and certain mental disorders (e.g., [43,46]).

3.1. Immunomodulatory Steroids and Disgust Sensitivity in the First Trimester

Recent studies have reported that the first trimester is a time of significant immunomodulation [31,32]. Additionally, in line with the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis [9,13], it has been demonstrated that disgust sensitivity negatively correlates with certain cytokines [7] and, similarly, that disgust sensitivity is elevated in the first trimester in women who were recently ill [10]. We therefore expected to observe significant relationships between disgust sensitivity and steroids with immunomodulatory function during this period of pregnancy as well. However, we did not find any significant correlations between disgust sensitivity and the most active immunomodulatory 7α/β-hydroxy-, 7-oxo-, and 16α-hydroxy-metabolites (e.g., 5-androstene-3β,16α, and17β-triol and its sulfate) of C19 Δ5 steroids in the first trimester. On the other hand, we observed a positive relationship between disgust sensitivity and the levels of androstenediol, a C19 Δ5 steroid, during this stage of pregnancy. Androstenediol, an adrenal androgen produced by the adrenal glands, serves as a precursor to key sex hormones such as testosterone and estrogen (both discussed separately below). Furthermore, androstenediol also plays a significant role in immunomodulatory processes [42,43].
We have also found positive correlations between disgust sensitivity in the first trimester and the levels of some C21 5α/β-reduced steroids, such as conjugated pregnanolone, 17-hydroxypregnanolone, 5α,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, 5β-pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol, and 5α-androstane-3,17-dione. It is known that during pregnancy, the C21 steroids derive mainly from placental progesterone, which may be a product of LDL cholesterol penetrating from the maternal compartment into the placenta, but also from pregnenolone sulfate, which is formed in the fetal adrenal gland and then metabolized to progesterone in the placenta. These results thus suggest an important role of maternal and placental steroidogenesis in mother’s susceptibility to disgust.
In connection with the C21 5α/β-reduced steroids, 5α-androstane-3,17-dione deserves a separate mention. The levels of this steroid consistently positively correlated with disgust levels measured via the DS-R and TDDS questionnaires in the first trimester, as well as with disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R in the third trimester of pregnancy. While not bioactive itself, 5α-androstane-3,17-dione is a precursor of 5α-androstanediols. Specific 5α-androstanols are known to act as positive GABAergic (neuroinhibitory) modulators, such as androsterone, etiocholanolone, and 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol. Moreover, the biologically inactive 5α-androstane-3,17-dione is a direct 5α-reduced metabolite of the likewise biologically inactive androstenedione, which is a direct precursor of both the biologically active androgen testosterone and the biologically inactive estrone. Estrone, however, is further metabolized to create the active estrogen estradiol. In addition, the 5α-androstane-3,17-dione can be readily converted to the most active androgen 5α-dihydrotestosterone in a single metabolic step (via reduction of the oxo-group to 17beta-hydroxy-group), and this conversion works in both directions [47,48,49]. The presence of higher levels of 5α-androstane-3,17-dione can therefore indicate either a higher production of active sex steroids or, conversely, their higher catabolism. Considering that the results of our study also show a significant positive association between active sex steroids and disgust sensitivity, it would seem that in this case, the elevated levels of 5α-androstane-3,17-dione are more likely to be related to a higher production of these steroids and are thus in line with our prediction of the positive association between disgust sensitivity and sex steroids.

3.2. Immunomodulatory Steroids and Disgust Sensitivity in the Third Trimester

The associations between immunomodulatory steroids and disgust sensitivity were found not only in the first trimester but also in the third trimester. There we found positive correlations between cortisol, a steroid with well-known immunosuppressive effects [50], some steroids occurring in the metabolic pathway of cortisol synthesis, such as pregnenolone and 17-hydroxypregnenolone, and disgust sensitivity (measured via the DS-R).
Similarly, we also found positive correlations between Contamination disgust scores in the third trimester and multiple progesterone metabolites, such as conjugated pregnanolone, conjugated epipregnanolone, conjugated 5α,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, conjugated 5α-pregnane-3α,20α-diol, conjugated 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone, and conjugated 5β-pregnane-3β,20α-diol. These relationships may be related to the placental production of progesterone, although we observed no correlation with the hormone itself. Given that progesterone can be rapidly metabolized, these catabolites may be more stable markers of its presence, which could explain the absence of progesterone in these relationships. If this is indeed the case, it would be in line with the assumption of the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis, according to which the immunosuppressive function of progesterone is compensated by elevated disgust sensitivity. This has been previously shown in two studies on non-pregnant, naturally cycling women [20,24] and in an animal model [28]. A positive association was also observed between progesterone and increased sensitivity to disgusted faces with averted gaze, which may signal a pathogen threat in the environment [25].
In addition to the progesterone metabolites and cortisol, our findings also showed a positive association between immunoreactive metabolites of C19 Δ5 androstanes and disgust sensitivity (measured via both the DS-R and TDDS). These associations were noted for DHEA, 7α-hydroxy-DHEA, 7β-hydroxy-DHEA, 5-Androstene-3β,7α,17β-triol, and again for androstenediol. These substances are known to not only stimulate the immune response but also to help suppress autoimmunity. While C19 Δ5 steroids and their metabolites can reduce the severity of autoimmune diseases [51,52,53,54,55,56], autoimmune diseases can, in turn, impair the production of adrenal C19 Δ5 steroids [51,57]. Additionally, some of these steroids may also counteract the suppression of the primary immune response by glucocorticoids [58]. It has also been reported that DHEA regulates the Th1/Th2 balance by either promoting the Th1 component or reducing the production of both components [55,59]. The C19 Δ5 steroids also suppress cell-mediated immunity and autoantibody formation [53,54,55,56,60], and they may induce restoration of the Th1-dominated cytokine profile.
The autoimmune response can also be triggered by estradiol through its interaction with estrogen receptors. This represents another mechanism of action for the C19 Δ5 steroids, which involves the catabolism of C19 estrogen precursors such as DHEA, androstenediol, and 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol. These precursors, which are also estrogenic, are converted to their 7-oxygenated and 16α-hydroxylated catabolites. These metabolites cannot be further transformed into bioactive estrogens [61]. Interestingly, estradiol can stimulate catalytic steroid 7α-hydroxylase (CYP7B1) activity, mRNA, and human CYP7B1 reporter gene in human embryonic kidney cells HEK293. In turn, the stimulated catalytic CYP7B1 activity may control the DHEA and androstenediol levels in human tissues. These steroids serve as substrates for the synthesis of both, active androgens and estrogens [62]. The above mechanism could function as a negative feedback loop in the regulation of estrogen levels. Moreover, 5-androstene-3β,7β,17β-triol is immunoprotective despite its low concentrations and high clearance [63]. Synthetic anti-inflammatory derivatives of 5-androstene-3β,7β,17β-triol have been found to attenuate the production of inflammatory markers such as C-reactive protein, interleukin 17 (IL-17), TNFα, and interleukin 6 (IL-6), as well as to reduce the expression of mRNA for IL-6 and matrix metalloproteinase in inflamed tissues. Additionally, these steroids demonstrate suppressive effects on pro-inflammatory cytokines in the lungs and intensely stimulate splenic regulatory T-cells [64].
To summarize, the elevated levels of C19 Δ5 steroids and steroids such as cortisol, pregnenolone, or various progesterone metabolites during the third trimester of pregnancy could reflect an adaptive mechanism which would lead to increased protection against pathogens as childbirth approaches. After birth, the newborn is extremely vulnerable and since its own immune system is not yet developed, it relies primarily on maternal protection against infections.

3.3. Estrogens and Disgust Sensitivity

Focusing on results related to the first trimester of pregnancy, we have observed a positive correlation between disgust sensitivity (specifically the Contamination and Animal reminder subscales of the DS-R and the Pathogen domain of the TDDS) and estrogens (estrone and estradiol). The production of these estrogens during pregnancy relies on the production of C19 Δ5 steroid sulfates (DHEA sulfate, androstenediol sulfate) in the fetal zone of the adrenal gland [65]. This is also reflected in our results. In the first trimester, the levels of DHEA sulfate also positively correlated with the scores of the Contamination disgust subscale.
Similarly to the immunosuppressive effect of progesterone, which inspired the formulation of the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis [13], estrogens, too, play a role in immunomodulation. They are known to shift the immune response towards Th2 dominance [66]. Moreover, some studies have observed higher levels of disgust sensitivity during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, when estrogen levels are elevated, compared to the menstrual phase [20,23], aligning with our findings. Conversely, our results are not in line with two studies that found no association between salivary estradiol levels and disgust sensitivity measured via TDDS [18,29]. However, it should be noted that both of the aforementioned studies were conducted on a population of non-pregnant women, and they focused on the relationship between disgust and estradiol levels during the menstrual cycle. Moreover, the hormone levels were measured in saliva samples in both studies, as opposed to blood serum, which was used in our study.
Nevertheless, our results regarding estrone levels are also not entirely unambiguous. While higher estrone levels were associated with higher scores of the Contamination and Animal reminder disgust subscales and the Pathogen disgust domain, the overall DS-R score correlated negatively with estrone levels. It must be taken into consideration that aside from the Contamination and Animal reminder disgust subscales, the DS-R also contains a Core disgust subscale, focused on food and animal or bodily products, which also contributes to the overall score. This subscale may have been the cause of the observed reverse direction of correlation. A better understanding of the observed effects would, however, require further research.

3.4. Testosterone and Disgust Sensitivity

A significant positive correlation with testosterone levels was observed only in the first trimester, with the sole exception of the Contamination disgust subscale score, which increased in association with decreasing testosterone levels (this relationship is discussed below in connection with specific anxiety-related outcomes). In both trimesters, we have also found a significant positive correlation between disgust sensitivity and 5α-dihydrotestosterone, which is directly produced from testosterone via the enzyme 5α-reductase in peripheral tissue.
Since higher testosterone levels are associated with increased immunosuppression [67], our main findings are in line with the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis [13], suggesting that disgust sensitivity should be elevated during immunosuppression. Moreover, previous studies have observed higher disgust sensitivity in women pregnant with male compared to female fetus [10,30], possibly reflecting mechanisms that lead to elevated testosterone levels in women pregnant with a male fetus [68]. However, studies examining the relationships between salivary testosterone levels and disgust sensitivity in non-pregnant female population samples found no significant associations [18,29]. Additionally, our study found no evidence that the sex of the fetus influences disgust sensitivity during pregnancy, as fetus sex did not statistically significantly contribute to any of the analyses.

3.5. Steroids, Anxiety-Related Disorders, and Disgust Sensitivity in the Third Trimester

In the third trimester, we found a positive association between disgust sensitivity and cortisol and DHEA. This could be related to anxiety-related disorders, including OCD, as higher levels of cortisol and DHEA have been observed in female patients with OCD compared to a control group of women [46]. Moreover, this observation aligns with the positive association between disgust and anxiety-related disorders, including OCD [12,69,70], and could also help explain the specific results regarding the relationship between testosterone and the Contamination disgust subscale of the DS-R questionnaire.
In contrast to all other findings regarding disgust sensitivity and steroids, a lower level of testosterone in the third trimester predicted higher disgust scores in the Contamination subscale. It is also important to note that the testosterone levels had the strongest effect in this particular model: they explained 32.5% of variability. Similar to cortisol and DHEA, which were previously associated with anxiety disorders, lower salivary testosterone levels were measured in women with current depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, and agoraphobia without panic disorder [71].
That indicates that the increased disgust sensitivity during pregnancy might not only be an adaptive mechanism aimed at protecting the organism from pathogens. In the third trimester, it may also reflect the higher anxiety observed during this period [38,72], which may be associated with the approaching childbirth. The Contamination disgust subscale of the DS-R is centered around worries about the interpersonal transmission of pathogens and subsequent aversion. Contamination disgust is closely related to some types of OCD, whose symptoms include compulsive cleaning and handwashing. Our findings are also consistent with the study by Dlouhá et al. [10], where the authors observed increasing levels of disgust sensitivity during pregnancy that extended into the postpartum period.
Our findings underscore the importance of monitoring hormonal and psychological health in women in the third trimester of pregnancy to mitigate potential anxiety and OCD symptoms. Given that increased disgust sensitivity during pregnancy persists into the postpartum period, the observed associations may have long-term consequences for women’s mental health after childbirth.

3.6. The Effect of Changes in Steroid Levels during Pregnancy on Changes in Disgust Sensitivity

Regarding the correlations between changes in disgust sensitivity measured via DS-R and increasing pregnancy length, there was a general trend towards negative correlations between increasing disgust sensitivity and the overall activity of steroidogenesis during the first trimester of pregnancy. We have also observed positive correlations between disgust sensitivity and the increase in the C19 steroid levels between the first and third trimesters. This shows that on the one hand, lower steroidogenic activity in the first trimester is associated with a more significant increase in disgust sensitivity with increasing pregnancy length due to increased steroid production; on the other hand, a positive association between zona reticularis activity in the maternal adrenal gland and disgust sensitivity was also found. These data indicate that the role of the fetus is not decisive here because although the fetal zone of the fetal adrenal gland produces even more sulfated Δ5 androstanes than the zona reticularis in the maternal adrenal gland, these substances are rapidly metabolized to estrogens in the placenta, so while maternal blood estrogen levels increase exponentially with advancing gestational age, maternal adrenal androgen levels do not [65]. However, positive correlations between the increase in the scores of the Pathogen disgust domain of TDDS and the increase in estrogen sulfates also suggest an association with the activity of the fetal zone of the fetal adrenal gland. Interestingly, this zone is a fetal counterpart of maternal zona reticularis.

3.7. A Comparison of Questionnaires Measuring Disgust

The associations between specific steroids and the two different questionnaires, the DS-R and the Pathogen domain of TDDS, allow us to make some observations about the questionnaires themselves. The Pathogen domain of TDDS is often associated with similar steroids as the Core and Animal reminder subscales of the DS-R. During the first trimester, only a minimum of steroids is specifically associated only with the Pathogen domain, which suggests that the Pathogen domain reflects some part of the disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R. The DS-R has been previously criticized for not effectively reflecting the adaptive function of disgust. Based on this critique, the TDDS questionnaire was developed to focus specifically on the adaptiveness of disgust sensitivity [45]. The results obtained from the two different questionnaires would suggest that the DS-R actually reflects both the adaptive function of disgust and the maladaptive form of disgust associated with, for instance, anxiety disorders.

3.8. Strengths and Limitations

Our study has several strengths. Due to its longitudinal design, we were able to track the development of disgust levels in response to changes in steroid levels during pregnancy. Moreover, we had a relatively large sample of women, which is rather uncommon for this type of study. Another strength of the study is the simultaneous use of two of the most commonly used textual questionnaires (DS-R and TDDS) to measure disgust sensitivity. The inclusion of both questionnaires enabled us to better understand the various aspects of disgust and to detect overlaps and differences between the questionnaires.
The main limitation of this study is that disgust sensitivity was based on self-report textual questionnaires. Aside from textual questionnaires, there are other methods of measuring disgust sensitivity, such as those based on visual stimuli. Such methods can also measure the experienced emotion based on various physiological parameters that directly reflect the subject’s state. In the case of pregnant women, however, we could not use such methods for ethical reasons. Nevertheless, some studies used the textual and visual methods in parallel and found no differences in the results acquired by the two approaches [16].

4. Materials and Methods

In a prospective longitudinal study running between June 2019 and November 2022, we collected data from pregnant women in the first and third trimesters of pregnancy. This study was part of a larger project to explore longitudinal changes in pregnancy and their correlations with biological and psychological factors.

4.1. Procedure—Data Collection

In total, 228 adult women who conceived naturally and reported no severe chronic diseases or autoimmune disorders were recruited for the study in collaboration with three gynecological clinics in Prague, Czech Republic. Most women were from the ProfiGyn clinic (n = 133), 37 women from the GynFleur clinic, and only nine women were recruited in the Levret clinic. From this sample, 49 women were excluded from the study: 15 women miscarried, 19 women either did not provide blood samples in both trimesters or there was an insufficient amount of blood serum for steroid hormone testing, and 15 women left the study at their request. The final sample thus consisted of 179 women.
Participants were recruited for the study during their first antenatal medical checkup during which their pregnancy was confirmed by their gynecologist. At this time, between weeks 5 and 14 of pregnancy (mean ± SD = 7.7 ± 1.23), they completed a background questionnaire that included questions about age, physical parameters, parity, the method of conception, health status, and several demographic questions. During medical checkups in the first trimester, between weeks 9 and 14 of pregnancy (mean ± SD = 10.3 ± 0.91), and again during the third trimester, between weeks 30 and 38 pregnancy (mean ± SD = 33.0 ± 1.61), they completed questionnaires that measured disgust sensitivity and provided blood samples for determining the levels of steroid hormones. Information about the sex of the baby was obtained from both medical records and questionnaires.
All women participating in this study were part of a larger study focused on prenatal factors that affect the mother and child’s wellbeing and health. Participants answered all the disgust questions along with pregnancy nausea questions and were not explicitly informed that the study was about disgust sensitivity because that could influence their responses. All women signed an informed consent form and participated in all parts of the study under a pseudo-anonymous code. The project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Science, Charles University (Approval No. 2018/6 and 2019/10). All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

4.2. Questionnaires

All women completed a background questionnaire, including information about age, history of previous pregnancies, education level, size of residence area, and health data. Disgust sensitivity was assessed via the Disgust Scale-Revised (DS-R) [44] and via the Pathogen domain of the Three Domains of Disgust Scale (TDDS) [45].
The DS-R [44] is a self-report questionnaire containing 25 items, which are divided into three subscales: 12 items in the Core disgust subscale (disgust evoked by food and animal or bodily products); 8 items in the Animal reminder disgust subscale (disgust related to injuries, mortality, or violations of the body envelope); and 5 items in the Contamination disgust subscale (disgust associated with transmission of pathogens between people). The questionnaire has two parts: in the first part, participants rate to what extent they agree with presented statements on a scale from 0 = “Strongly disagree (very untrue about me)” to 4 = “Strongly agree (very true about me)”; in the second part, the participants rate how disgusting they would find situations described in the presented statements, again on a scale from 0 to 48 in the Core subscale, from 0 to 32 in the Animal reminder subscale, and from 0 to 20 in the Contamination subscale. The overall score can range from 0 to 100, with a higher score indicating higher disgust sensitivity. In cases where one-fifth or fewer questions were left unanswered within a subscale, we used the average score of that subscale to substitute the missing values (we substituted seven responses in the first trimester and five responses in the third trimester). In cases where more than one-fifth of answers were missing, the data from that participant were excluded from further analyses (ten in the first trimester and three in the third trimester).
The TDDS [45] is a 21-item self-report questionnaire containing 21 items, divided into three domains: Pathogen, Moral, and Sexual (each containing 7 items). For this study, only the Pathogen domain was used, as it was the most relevant to the aims of the study. In this questionnaire, the participants rate possibly disgusting statements on a scale from 0 (not disgusting at all) to 6 (extremely disgusting). The final score for this domain can range from 0 to 42. Once again, if less than one-fifth of the questions were left unanswered, the average score of the domain was used to substitute the missing values (one response in the first trimester and two responses in the third trimester). If more than one-fifth of the questions were missing, the data of that participant were excluded from further analyses (five in the first trimester and three in the third trimester).

4.3. Laboratory Measurement of Steroid Hormones

Blood samples of 179 women (the final sample) in their first and third trimester of pregnancy were analyzed for concentrations of steroid hormones in blood serum. Serum from blood was obtained after centrifugation (2 min at 3000× g at 21 °C) and stored at −20 °C until analyzed. We used the advanced gas chromatography trandem mass spectrometry (GC-MS/MS) platform for the multicomponent quantification of endogenous steroids: 56 unconjugated steroids and 35 polar conjugates of steroids (after hydrolysis). Unlike the current methods used for the quantification of circulating steroids on the GC-MS/MS platform, the present one was validated not only for the blood of men and non-pregnant women but also for the blood of pregnant women and for mixed umbilical cord blood [73]. The spectrum of analytes includes common hormones operating via nuclear receptors as well as other bioactive substances like immunomodulatory and neuroactive steroids. The present method was extended for corticoids and 17-hydroxylated 5α/β-reduced pregnanes, which are useful for the investigation of an alternative “backdoor” pathway. The testing was carried out in the Institute of Endocrinology under the direction of Martin Hill, PhD, DSc.

4.4. Statistics

At first, the data were both manually and automatically controlled. Using STATGRAPHICS Centurion 18 software (The Plains, VA, USA), all variables, with the exception for binary variables, were transformed towards symmetric distribution and constant variance before the final statistical analyses. Power transformations were used to transform the values of all analyzed variables, including steroids, to reach symmetric data distribution and homoscedasticity. Using the SIMCA software (v. 12.1.1.1), these transformed data were then automatically converted to z-scores for further analyses.
Multivariate regression with a reduction of dimensionality, known as orthogonal projections to latent structures (OPLS) [74], was conducted to assess the associations between hormonal changes and changes in disgust sensitivity during pregnancy. The OPLS model searches for the best linear combination of predictors for an optimum estimate of the dependent variable. Statistical software SIMCA v. 12.1.1.1. (Umetrics, Umea, Sweden) was used for the OPLS analysis.
In the input models, disgust sensitivity was represented by vector Y. Steroid levels and related variables, namely, maternal age, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (calculated based on a self-report of the participant’s pre-pregnancy weight and height), pregnancy length in the first and third trimester (in days), maternal weight gain (in kg), parity (0: primipara; 1: multipara), fetus sex (0: female; 1: male), maternal diabetes and hypertension (0: no; 1: yes), and maternal pre-pregnancy smoking (1: no; 2: only occasionally; 3: yes), constituted matrix X. Separate OPLS models were developed for the associations measured in the first trimester, in the third trimester, and also for the changes between the first and third trimester (association between Δ scores of disgust sensitivity and Δ(steroids) calculated as the levels of steroids/disgust scores in the third trimester minus the levels of steroids/disgust scores in the first trimester). In all cases, separate OPLS models were also developed for the Pathogen disgust measured via the TDDS, the overall DS-R score, and the three individual DS-R subscales. By using this approach, one predictive component was extracted for each model. Non-homogeneities were eliminated after checking the data homogeneity in predictors using Hotelling’s statistic.

5. Conclusions

While in the first trimester, disgust increases with rising maternal estrogen levels and C19 Δ5 steroids action, which are associated with catabolism of C19 estrogen precursors, in the third trimester, more positive correlations dominate between disgust levels and steroids, reflecting both maternal immune activity and potential symptoms accompanying anxiety or other psychiatric disorders. Specifically in the first trimester, disgust sensitivity was positively associated with the presence of some active sex steroids, such as testosterone and some estrogens, and the presence of some steroids indicating progesterone and sex steroid production. This has further implications for associations between disgust sensitivity and immune system activity as these steroids are known to have immunomodulatory effects. These results are in line with the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis, which states that disgust sensitivity is elevated when immunosuppression is higher. This adaptive mechanism is especially important during pregnancy as it is necessary to protect the mother and developing fetus from pathogens by enhancing aversion to potentially harmful substances and environments. In that regard, the results in the third trimester showed a positive association between disgust sensitivity and cortisol and progesterone metabolites, with cortisol and progesterone being known to be immunosuppressive, as well as metabolites of C19 Δ5 androstanes and estrogen, which are known for modulating the immune response and for the suppression of autoimmunity.
Moreover, results from the third trimester—specifically, positive associations between disgust sensitivity and cortisol and DHEA and negative associations between contamination-related disgust sensitivity and testosterone—suggest a maladaptive occurrence of elevated disgust in this period of pregnancy, possibly related to various mental disorders or their symptoms. Regarding changes in steroid levels and disgust sensitivity during pregnancy, our results showed that lower steroidogenic activity in the first trimester is associated with an increase in disgust sensitivity between the first and third trimesters. This increase is due to the heightened production of certain steroids such as androstenediol, androsterone, androstenediol sulfate, 5α-dihydrotestosterone, DHEA sulfate, and estrogen sulfates.
Understanding the neural, cognitive, and behavioral intricacies of the disgust system, together with their associations with physiological mechanisms, not only broadens our comprehension of human adaptive mechanisms but also has implications for a variety of fields, ranging from psychology and neuroscience to public health. Especially crucial is the understanding of these relationships during pregnancy, which represents a sensitive period for the mother and can significantly influence the future development of her child. Knowing how these relationships work can lead to better screening for issues related to immunity or mental health during pregnancy.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Š.K., D.D. and M.H.; methodology, Š.K., M.H., M.V., D.D. and J.U.; software, M.H.; validation, Š.K., M.H., M.V., D.D. and J.U.; formal analysis, Š.K., M.H. and J.U.; investigation, M.H., J.V. and M.V.; data curation, Š.K. and J.U.; writing—original draft preparation, Š.K., M.H. and D.D; writing—review and editing, all authors; visualization, M.H., Š.K. and J.U.; supervision, Š.K. and M.H.; project administration, J.U., D.D. and Š.K.; funding acquisition, Š.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Czech Science Foundation, project GAČR 20-16698S, “Disgust sensitivity in pregnancy: Individual differences and longitudinal changes”, and by the grant MH CZ-DRO (Institute of Endocrinology-EÚ, 00023761) from the Czech Ministry of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Science, Charles University (Approval No. 2018/6 and 2019/10).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jana Benešová, Jan Šeda, Natalie Kosinová, and Hana Hubová for their assistance with data collection. We would like to thank Anna Pilátová for proofreading the final text. We would also like to thank all participants for their willingness to participate in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Schaller, M.; Duncan, L.A. The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In Evolution and the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Social Cognition; Forgas, J.P., Haselton, M.G., von Hippel, W., Eds.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: Abingdon, UK, 2007; pp. 293–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Curtis, V.; Aunger, R.; Rabie, T. Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2004, 271 (Suppl. S4), S131–S133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Rozin, P.; Fallon, A.E. A perspective on disgust. Psychol. Rev. 1987, 94, 23–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Stevenson, R.J.; Oaten, M.J.; Case, T.I.; Repacholi, B.M.; Wagland, P. Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: Development and acquisition. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 165–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Al-Shawaf, L.; Lewis, D.M.G.; Buss, D.M. Sex Differences in Disgust: Why Are Women More Easily Disgusted Than Men? Emot. Rev. 2018, 10, 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Stevenson, R.J.; Case, T.I.; Oaten, M.J. Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2009, 30, 363–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kankova, S.; Takács, L.; Krulova, M.; Hlavacova, J.; Nouzova, K.; Hill, M.; Vcelak, J.; Monk, C. Disgust sensitivity is negatively associated with immune system activity in early pregnancy: Direct support for the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2022, 43, 234–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kankova, S.; Takacs, L.; Hlavacova, J.; Calda, P.; Monk, C.; Havlicek, J. Disgust sensitivity in early pregnancy as a response to high pathogen risk. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1015927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Fessler, D.M.; Navarrete, C.D. Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy-Evidence supporting prophylaxis the compensatory hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2005, 26, 344–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Dlouha, D.; Roberts, S.C.; Hlavacova, J.; Nouzova, K.; Kankova, S. Longitudinal changes in disgust sensitivity during pregnancy and the early postpartum period, and the role of recent health problems. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 4752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ille, R.; Schoggl, H.; Kapfhammer, H.P.; Arendasy, M.; Sommer, M.; Schienle, A. Self-disgust in mental disorders–symptom-related or disorder-specific? Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 938–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Olatunji, B.O. Changes in disgust correspond with changes in symptoms of contamination-based OCD: A prospective examination of specificity. J. Anxiety Disord. 2010, 24, 313–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fessler, D.M.; Navarrete, C.D. Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2003, 24, 406–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Miyaura, H.; Iwata, M. Direct and indirect inhibition of Th1 development by progesterone and glucocorticoids. J. Immunol. 2002, 168, 1087–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Olatunji, B.O.; Cox, R.C.; Li, I. Disgust regulation between menstrual cycle phases: Differential effects of emotional suppression and reappraisal. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2020, 68, 101543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dlouha, D.; Ullman, J.; Takacs, L.; Nouzova, K.; Hrbackova, H.; Seda, J.; Kankova, S. Comparing disgust sensitivity in women in early pregnancy and non-pregnant women in the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2024, 45, 164–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Timmers, A.D.; Bossio, J.A.; Chivers, M.L. Disgust, sexual cues, and the prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 4, 179–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stern, J.; Shiramizu, V. Hormones, ovulatory cycle phase and pathogen disgust: A longitudinal investigation of the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis. Horm. Behav. 2022, 138, 105103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Rafiee, Y.; Jones, B.C.; Shiramizu, V. Is pathogen disgust increased on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone is high? Evidence from a between-subjects study using estimated progesterone levels. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2023, 9, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zelazniewicz, A.; Borkowska, B.; Nowak, J.; Pawlowski, B. The progesterone level, leukocyte count and disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 161, 60–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Milkowska, K.; Galbarczyk, A.; Klimek, M.; Zablocka-Slowińska, K.; Jasienska, G. Pathogen disgust, but not moral disgust, changes across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2021, 42, 402–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Milkowska, K.; Galbarczyk, A.; Jasienska, G. Disgust sensitivity in relation to menstrual cycle phase in women with and without an infection. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2019, 31, e23233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Liu, M.; Zhang, X.; He, Z.; Liang, Y.; Zou, B.; Ma, X.; Gu, S.; Wang, F. Opposite effects of estradiol and progesterone on woman’s disgust processing. Front. Psychiatry 2023, 14, 1161488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fleischman, D.S.; Fessler, D.M. Progesterone’s effects on the psychology of disease avoidance: Support for the compensatory behavioral prophylaxis hypothesis. Horm. Behav. 2011, 59, 271–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Conway, C.A.; Jones, B.C.; DeBruine, L.M.; Welling, L.L.; Law Smith, M.J.; Perrett, D.I.; Sharp, M.A.; Al-Dujaili, E.A. Salience of emotional displays of danger and contagion in faces is enhanced when progesterone levels are raised. Horm. Behav. 2007, 51, 202–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Kamboj, S.K.; Krol, K.M.; Curran, H.V. A specific association between facial disgust recognition and estradiol levels in naturally cycling women. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0122311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kavaliers, M.; Bishnoi, I.R.; Ossenkopp, K.P.; Choleris, E. Differential effects of progesterone on social recognition and the avoidance of pathogen threat by female mice. Horm. Behav. 2021, 127, 104873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bressan, P.; Kramer, P. Progesterone does raise disgust. Horm. Behav. 2022, 137, 104937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Jones, B.; Hahn, A.; Fisher, C.; Wang, H.; Kandrik, M.; Lee, A.; Tybur, J.; DeBruine, L. Hormonal correlates of pathogen disgust: Testing the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2018, 39, 166–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Zelazniewicz, A.; Pawlowski, B. Disgust in pregnancy and fetus sex–longitudinal study. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 139, 177–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Racicot, K.; Kwon, J.Y.; Aldo, P.; Silasi, M.; Mor, G. Understanding the complexity of the immune system during pregnancy. Am. J. Reprod. Immunol. 2014, 72, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hove, C.; Trumble, B.C.; Anderson, A.S.; Stieglitz, J.; Kaplan, H.; Gurven, M.D.; Blackwell, A.D. Immune function during pregnancy varies between ecologically distinct populations. Evol. Med. Public Health 2020, 2020, 114–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Abu-Raya, B.; Michalski, C.; Sadarangani, M.; Lavoie, P.M. Maternal Immunological Adaptation During Normal Pregnancy. Front. Immunol. 2020, 11, 575197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kankova, S.; Hlavacova, J.; Roberts, K.; Benesova, J.; Havlicek, J.; Calda, P.; Dlouha, D.; Roberts, S.C. Associations between nausea and vomiting in pregnancy, disgust sensitivity, and first-trimester maternal serum free beta-hCG and PAPP-A. Horm. Behav. 2023, 152, 105360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Gerdes, A.B.; Uhl, G.; Alpers, G.W. Spiders are special: Fear and disgust evoked by pictures of arthropods. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2009, 30, 66–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cisler, J.M.; Olatunji, B.O.; Lohr, J.M. Disgust sensitivity and emotion regulation potentiate the effect of disgust propensity on spider fear, blood-injection-injury fear, and contamination fear. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2009, 40, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Marzillier, S.; Davey, G. Anxiety and disgust: Evidence for a unidirectional relationship. Cogn. Emot. 2005, 19, 729–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Silva, M.M.J.; Nogueira, D.A.; Clapis, M.J.; Leite, E. Anxiety in pregnancy: Prevalence and associated factors. Rev. Esc. Enferm. USP 2017, 51, e03253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Fava, G.A.; Grandi, S.; Michelacci, L.; Saviotti, F.; Conti, S.; Bovicelli, L.; Trombini, G.; Orlandi, C. Hypochondriacal fears and beliefs in pregnancy. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1990, 82, 70–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Odent, M. Fear of death during labour. Midwifery Today Int. Midwife 2003, 67, 20–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Shlomi Polachek, I.; Huller Harari, L.; Baum, M.; Strous, R.D. Postpartum anxiety in a cohort of women from the general population: Risk factors and association with depression during last week of pregnancy, postpartum depression and postpartum PTSD. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 2014, 51, 128–134. [Google Scholar]
  42. Sterzl, I.; Hill, M.; Starka, L.; Velikova, M.; Kanceva, R.; Jemelkova, J.; Czernekova, L.; Kosztyu, P.; ZadraZil, J.; Matousovic, K.; et al. Patients with IgA nephropathy have altered levels of immunomodulatory C19 steroids. Glucocorticoid therapy with addition of adrenal androgens may be the choice. Physiol. Res. 2017, 66, S433–S442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Honcu, P.; Hill, M.; Bicikova, M.; Jandova, D.; Velikova, M.; Kajzar, J.; Kolatorova, L.; Bestak, J.; Macova, L.; Kancheva, R.; et al. Activation of Adrenal Steroidogenesis and an Improvement of Mood Balance in Postmenopausal Females after Spa Treatment Based on Physical Activity. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2019, 20, 3687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Olatunji, B.O.; Williams, N.L.; Tolin, D.F.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Sawchuk, C.N.; Lohr, J.M.; Elwood, L.S. The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 19, 281–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Tybur, J.; Lieberman, D.; Griskevicius, V. Microbes, Mating, and Morality: Individual Differences in Three Functional Domains of Disgust. Psychiatry Investig. 2009, 12, 538–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Erbay, L.G.; Kartalci, S. Neurosteroid Levels in Patients with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2015, 12, 538–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Luu-The, V. Assessment of steroidogenesis and steroidogenic enzyme functions. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2013, 137, 176–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lephart, E.D. Brain 5alpha-reductase: Cellular, enzymatic, and molecular perspectives and implications for biological function. Mol. Cell Neurosci. 1993, 4, 473–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Celotti, F.; Melcangi, R.C.; Martini, L. The 5 alpha-reductase in the brain: Molecular aspects and relation to brain function. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 1992, 13, 163–215. [Google Scholar]
  50. Coutinho, A.E.; Chapman, K.E. The anti-inflammatory and immunosuppressive effects of glucocorticoids, recent developments and mechanistic insights. Mol. Cell Endocrinol. 2011, 335, 2–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Bottasso, O.; Bay, M.L.; Besedovsky, H.; del Rey, A. The immuno-endocrine component in the pathogenesis of tuberculosis. Scand. J. Immunol. 2007, 66, 166–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Du, C.; Khalil, M.W.; Sriram, S. Administration of dehydroepiandrosterone suppresses experimental allergic encephalomyelitis in SJL/J mice. J. Immunol. 2001, 167, 7094–7101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Rontzsch, A.; Thoss, K.; Petrow, P.K.; Henzgen, S.; Brauer, R. Amelioration of murine antigen-induced arthritis by dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA). Inflamm. Res. 2004, 53, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Tan, X.D.; Dou, Y.C.; Shi, C.W.; Duan, R.S.; Sun, R.P. Administration of dehydroepiandrosterone ameliorates experimental autoimmune neuritis in Lewis rats. J. Neuroimmunol. 2009, 207, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Choi, I.S.; Cui, Y.; Koh, Y.A.; Lee, H.C.; Cho, Y.B.; Won, Y.H. Effects of dehydroepiandrosterone on Th2 cytokine production in peripheral blood mononuclear cells from asthmatics. Korean J. Intern. Med. 2008, 23, 176–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Sudo, N.; Yu, X.N.; Kubo, C. Dehydroepiandrosterone attenuates the spontaneous elevation of serum IgE level in NC/Nga mice. Immunol. Lett. 2001, 79, 177–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Kasperska-Zajac, A.; Brzoza, Z.; Rogala, B. Dehydroepiandrosterone and dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate in atopic allergy and chronic urticaria. Inflammation 2008, 31, 141–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Sterzl, I.; Hampl, R.; Sterzl, J.; Votruba, J.; Starka, L. 7 β-OH-DHEA counteracts dexamethasone induced suppression of primary immune response in murine spleenocytes. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 1999, 71, 133–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Romagnani, S.; Kapsenberg, M.; Radbruch, A.; Adorini, L. Th1 and Th2 cells. Res. Immunol. 1998, 149, 871–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Pratschke, S.; von Dossow-Hanfstingl, V.; Dietz, J.; Schneider, C.P.; Tufman, A.; Albertsmeier, M.; Winter, H.; Angele, M.K. Dehydroepiandrosterone modulates T-cell response after major abdominal surgery. J. Surg. Res. 2014, 189, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Pettersson, H.; Lundqvist, J.; Norlin, M. Effects of CYP7B1-mediated catalysis on estrogen receptor activation. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 2010, 1801, 1090–1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tang, W.; Eggertsen, G.; Chiang, J.Y.; Norlin, M. Estrogen-mediated regulation of CYP7B1: A possible role for controlling DHEA levels in human tissues. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2006, 100, 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Ahlem, C.N.; Page, T.M.; Auci, D.L.; Kennedy, M.R.; Mangano, K.; Nicoletti, F.; Ge, Y.; Huang, Y.; White, S.K.; Villegas, S.; et al. Novel components of the human metabolome: The identification, characterization and anti-inflammatory activity of two 5-androstene tetrols. Steroids 2011, 76, 145–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Reading, C.L.; Frincke, J.M.; White, S.K. Molecular targets for 17alpha-ethynyl-5-androstene-3beta,7beta,17beta-triol, an anti-inflammatory agent derived from the human metabolome. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e32147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hill, M.; Parizek, A.; Cibula, D.; Kancheva, R.; Jirasek, J.E.; Jirkovska, M.; Velikova, M.; Kubatova, J.; Klimkova, M.; Paskova, A.; et al. Steroid metabolome in fetal and maternal body fluids in human late pregnancy. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2010, 122, 114–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. AbdulHussain, G.; Azizieh, F.; Makhseed, M.; Raghupathy, R. Effects of Progesterone, Dydrogesterone and Estrogen on the Production of Th1/Th2/Th17 Cytokines by Lymphocytes from Women with Recurrent Spontaneous Miscarriage. J. Reprod. Immunol. 2020, 140, 103132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Folstad, I.; Karter, A.J. Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. Am. Nat. 1992, 139, 603–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Meulenberg, P.M.; Hofman, J.A. Maternal testosterone and fetal sex. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 1991, 39, 51–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Cisler, J.M.; Olatunji, B.O.; Lohr, J.M. Disgust, fear, and the anxiety disorders: A critical review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 29, 34–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Olatunji, B.O.; Armstrong, T.; Elwood, L. Is Disgust Proneness Associated With Anxiety and Related Disorders? A Qualitative Review and Meta-Analysis of Group Comparison and Correlational Studies. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 613–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Giltay, E.J.; Enter, D.; Zitman, F.G.; Penninx, B.W.; van Pelt, J.; Spinhoven, P.; Roelofs, K. Salivary testosterone: Associations with depression, anxiety disorders, and antidepressant use in a large cohort study. J. Psychosom. Res. 2012, 72, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Soto-Balbuena, C.; Rodriguez, M.F.; Escudero Gomis, A.I.; Ferrer Barriendos, F.J.; Le, H.N.; Pmb-Huca, G. Incidence, prevalence and risk factors related to anxiety symptoms during pregnancy. Psicothema 2018, 30, 257–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Hill, M.; Hana, V., Jr.; Velikova, M.; Parizek, A.; Kolatorova, L.; Vitku, J.; Skodova, T.; Simkova, M.; Simjak, P.; Kancheva, R.; et al. A method for determination of one hundred endogenous steroids in human serum by gas chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry. Physiol. Res. 2019, 68, 179–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Trygg, J.; Wold, S. Orthogonal projections to latent structures (O-PLS). J. Chemom. 2002, 16, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Associations between disgust sensitivity and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model in the first trimester of pregnancy. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 1. Associations between disgust sensitivity and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model in the first trimester of pregnancy. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Ijms 25 06857 g001
Figure 2. Associations between disgust sensitivity and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model in the third trimester of pregnancy. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Associations between disgust sensitivity and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model in the third trimester of pregnancy. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Ijms 25 06857 g002
Table 1. The serum steroid levels of pregnant women in the first and third trimesters.
Table 1. The serum steroid levels of pregnant women in the first and third trimesters.
Steroids (nM)Trimester 1
Median (Quartiles)
Trimester 3
Median (Quartiles)
Trimester 3 − Trimester 1
Median (Quartiles)
p-Value
C21 Δ5 Steroids
Pregnenolone4.28 (2.86, 6.22)4.84 (3.46, 7.32)0.563 (−0.812, 2.99)<0.001
Pregnenolone sulfate180 (124, 259)212 (161, 295)32.6 (−14.1, 84.4)<0.001
20α-Dihydropregnenolone4.78 (3.55, 6.09)3.29 (2.66, 4.32)−1.23 (−2.44, −0.327)<0.001
20α-Dihydropregnenolone sulfate954 (639, 1390)849 (620, 1100)−125 (−378, 92.5)<0.001
17-Hydroxypregnenolone sulfate4.6 (3.07, 7.9)2.69 (2.01, 3.98)−2.04 (−4.86, −0.636)<0.001
17-Hydroxypregnenolone7.29 (4.63, 12)7.39 (5.05, 11.5)0.144 (−3.54, 3.08)0.704
16α-Hydroxypregnenolone0.462 (0.298, 0.68)0.806 (0.589, 1.13)0.344 (0.0694, 0.593)<0.001
C19 Δ5 Steroids
Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA)9.25 (6.01, 12.9)4.63 (3.29, 6.87)−4.12 (−6.63, −1.66)<0.001
DHEA sulfate2900 (1800, 4360)1150 (666, 1810)−1670 (−2560, −910)<0.001
7α-Hydroxy-DHEA0.909 (0.539, 1.43)0.344 (0.228, 0.526)−0.533 (−0.973, −0.233)<0.001
7-oxo-DHEA0.503 (0.306, 0.842)0.519 (0.41, 0.71)0.0141 (−0.307, 0.235)0.647
7β-Hydroxy-DHEA0.598 (0.342, 0.885)0.285 (0.175, 0.382)−0.334 (−0.565, −0.124)<0.001
Androstenediol2.72 (1.85, 4.1)1.13 (0.741, 2.15)−1.24 (−2.16, −0.51)<0.001
Androstenediol sulfate322 (194, 512)174 (112, 261)−130 (−307, −25.3)<0.001
5-Androstene-3β,7α,17β-triol0.459 (0.281, 0.729)0.123 (0.084, 0.233)−0.305 (−0.571, −0.165)<0.001
5-Androstene-3β,7β,17β-triol0.509 (0.319, 0.873)0.231 (0.144, 0.319)−0.312 (−0.572, −0.134)<0.001
5-Androstene-3β,16α,17β-triol0.206 (0.131, 0.319)0.496 (0.337, 0.788)0.266 (0.107, 0.536)<0.001
5-Androstene-3β,16α,17β-triol sulfate32.6 (18.9, 61.6)78.2 (46.9, 139)34 (10.5, 78.2)<0.001
C21 Δ4 Steroids
Progesterone104 (76.5, 135)382 (281, 489)277 (183, 390)<0.001
20α-Dihydroprogesterone22.5 (17.2, 29.2)49.2 (36.3, 67.7)26.3 (15.6, 43)<0.001
17-Hydroxyprogesterone11 (8.47, 14.1)17.4 (13.8, 21.3)5.7 (1.69, 10.9)<0.001
16α-Hydroxyprogesterone2.14 (1.57, 2.79)10.2 (7.62, 14.3)8.07 (5.31, 12.2)<0.001
17α,20α-Dihydroxy-4-pregnene-3-one3.08 (2.18, 4.12)4.44 (3.34, 5.85)1.25 (0.332, 2.54)<0.001
C19 Δ4 Steroids
Androstenedione7.22 (5.44, 9.79)6.62 (4.8, 9.26)−0.966 (−2.54, 0.667)<0.001
Testosterone2.9 (2.1, 3.9)2.54 (1.69, 4.17)−0.245 (−0.805, 0.784)0.308
Conjugated testosterone1.08 (0.407, 2.39)4.2 (2.12, 7.29)2.1 (0.335, 5.67)<0.001
5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.903 (0.596, 1.45)0.443 (0.27, 0.682)−0.446 (−0.761, −0.167)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-dihydrotestosterone1.42 (0.854, 2.09)1 (0.677, 1.58)−0.288 (−1.09, 0.364)<0.001
Estrogens
Estrone3.57 (1.99, 6.85)17 (10.4, 31.4)13.4 (5.26, 29.9)<0.001
Estrone sulfate5.67 (3, 11.7)27.3 (16.8, 47.6)21.7 (9.41, 38.2)<0.001
Estradiol3.31 (1.55, 5.77)31 (18.4, 48.5)27 (16.2, 44.4)<0.001
Estradiol sulfate3.98 (2.18, 6.83)19.8 (10.3, 35.2)15.8 (4.91, 29.6)<0.001
Estriol1.89 (0.726, 4.44)74.8 (40.8, 119)74.3 (40.9, 116)<0.001
C21 5α/β-reduced Steroids
5α-Dihydroprogesterone10.4 (7.09, 13.7)65.1 (42.8, 92.5)54.3 (30.7, 80.6)<0.001
Allopregnanolone6.71 (5.05, 9.09)31.1 (21.1, 42.7)23.8 (14.8, 34)<0.001
Allopregnanolone sulfate128 (82.7, 227)1220 (739, 1870)1080 (534, 1690)<0.001
Isopregnanolone2.05 (1.38, 2.86)7.96 (4.44, 11.9)5.28 (2.73, 9.51)<0.001
Isopregnanolone sulfate92.3 (59, 143)605 (344, 958)477 (263, 837)<0.001
5β-Dihydroprogesterone0.123 (0.043, 0.235)2.02 (1.1, 3.26)1.89 (0.978, 3.01)<0.001
Pregnanolone1.91 (1.06, 3.06)22.8 (14.5, 29.9)20 (13.5, 27.1)<0.001
Conjugated pregnanolone76 (54, 131)696 (480, 950)549 (361, 806)<0.001
Epipregnanolone0.267 (0.137, 0.538)1.36 (0.823, 1.97)0.935 (0.596, 1.53)<0.001
Conjugated epipregnanolone23.7 (14.7, 37.3)154 (95.4, 255)117 (67.8, 207)<0.001
17-Hydroxyallopregnanolone0.11 (0.04, 0.197)0.279 (0.124, 0.511)0.149 (0.019, 0.314)<0.001
17-Hydroxyallopregnanolone sulfate6.6 (3.82, 11)11.1 (6.83, 19.5)3.48 (0.727, 9.93)<0.001
17-Hydroxypregnanolone0.195 (0.103, 0.36)0.882 (0.595, 1.19)0.623 (0.388, 0.899)<0.001
Conjugated 17α-hydroxypregnanolone24.1 (16.8, 39.6)46.4 (31, 70.6)19.4 (8.95, 35.1)<0.001
5α,20α-Tetrahydroprogesterone6.04 (4.42, 8.2)21.7 (15.1, 31.1)15.3 (9.37, 22.2)<0.001
Conjugated 5α,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone5.35 (3.3, 8.59)15.5 (10.7, 25.2)10.1 (4.74, 18.5)<0.001
5α-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol7.23 (4.83, 9.68)23.1 (16, 32.4)14.2 (8.53, 23.9)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-pregnane-3α,20α-diol1650 (1100, 2580)6550 (4220, 9420)4440 (2660, 7260)<0.001
5α-Pregnane-3β,20α-diol2.24 (1.57, 3.27)6.59 (4.35, 10.5)3.65 (2.33, 7.26)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-pregnane-3β,20α-diol3720 (2150, 6210)10,900 (7600, 17,000)6550 (3540, 11,500)<0.001
5β,20α-Tetrahydroprogesterone0.096 (0.062, 0.171)1.96 (1.4, 2.66)1.8 (1.28, 2.47)<0.001
Conjugated 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone2.65 (1.68, 4.06)7.48 (5.02, 9.64)4.22 (2.05, 6.78)<0.001
5β-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol0.829 (0.584, 1.21)6.61 (5.06, 8.53)5.53 (4.19, 7.16)<0.001
Conjugated 5β-pregnane-3α,20α-diol297 (229, 427)1070 (794, 1460)750 (498, 998)<0.001
5β-Pregnane-3β,20α-diol0.18 (0.113, 0.313)0.593 (0.396, 0.927)0.393 (0.209, 0.665)<0.001
Conjugated 5β-pregnane-3β,20α-diol189 (127, 286)644 (461, 920)430 (253, 624)<0.001
5α-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol0.276 (0.146, 0.474)0.179 (0.104, 0.321)−0.082 (−0.199, 0.006)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol28.5 (10.5, 58)35.5 (11.8, 87.5)3.2 (−5.57, 23.6)0.001
5β-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol2.8 (1.88, 3.94)4.79 (3.16, 6.17)1.52 (0.35, 2.82)<0.001
Conjugated 5β-pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol116 (82.2, 216)162 (117, 269)34.8 (−21.4, 102)<0.001
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione0.521 (0.34, 0.744)0.425 (0.297, 0.625)−0.0559 (−0.197, 0.0546)<0.001
C19 5α/β-reduced Steroids
Androsterone0.897 (0.705, 1.21)0.568 (0.427, 0.815)−0.336 (−0.549, −0.12)<0.001
Androsterone sulfate1290 (718, 2100)594 (333, 986)−540 (−1250, −258)<0.001
Epiandrosterone0.333 (0.205, 0.497)0.158 (0.102, 0.251)−0.16 (−0.288, −0.062)<0.001
Epiandrosterone sulfate348 (214, 496)129 (76.8, 187)−199 (−312, −116)<0.001
Etiocholanolone0.265 (0.164, 0.421)0.233 (0.148, 0.396)−0.025 (−0.103, 0.062)0.103
Etiocholanolone sulfate53.5 (34.2, 84.6)36.6 (20, 56.2)−15.6 (−36.2, −4.48)<0.001
Epietiocholanolone sulfate17 (10.4, 35.5)9.45 (6.06, 17.8)−7.24 (−18.5, −2.09)<0.001
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol0.174 (0.118, 0.245)0.079 (0.061, 0.11)−0.09 (−0.145, −0.05)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol19.7 (14.2, 28.7)13.2 (8.46, 18)−7.12 (−14.7, −1.3)<0.001
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol0.064 (0.024, 0.141)0.032 (0.01, 0.075−0.022 (−0.081, −0.002)<0.001
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol26.6 (16.1, 56.5)12.5 (7.12, 23.6)−14.8 (−35.1, −4.69)<0.001
5β-Androstane-3α,17β-diol0.011 (0.006, 0.018)0.007 (0.003, 0.013)−0.004 (−0.009, 0.002)<0.001
Conjugated 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol3.98 (2.5, 6.59)2.85 (1.78, 4.9)−0.916 (−2.09, 0.272)<0.001
Corticoids and 11β-hydroxy-androstanes
Cortisol389 (308, 473)632 (537, 824)231 (148, 386)<0.001
Cortisone106 (77.1, 148)170 (128, 252)52.8 (22.8, 111)<0.001
Corticosterone12.5 (7.38, 18.6)19.5 (14.6, 27.2)6.96 (−0.918, 14.7)<0.001
11-Deoxycortisol0.74 (0.168, 1.87)3.49 (1.06, 7.02)2.57 (0.423, 5.22)<0.001
21-Deoxycortisol0.081 (0.029, 0.243)0.117 (0.0656, 0.259)0.029 (−0.025, 0.098)<0.001
3α,5α-Tetrahydrocorticosterone0.039 (0.0173, 0.087)0.028 (0.010, 0.056)−0.009 (−0.043, 0.003)<0.001
3α,5β-Tetrahydrocorticosterone0.124 (0.0409, 0.328)0.06 (0.022, 0.127)−0.052 (−0.228, 0.0112)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyandrostenedione48.3 (29.9, 74.9)60 (39.5, 102)8.58 (−1.77, 30)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyandrosterone1.2 (0.702, 2.14)0.316 (0.188, 0.528)−0.897 (−1.73, −0.432)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyandrosterone sulfate12.2 (8.5, 18.3)7.45 (4.59, 11.6)−4.56 (−9.27, −0.811)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyepiandrosterone0.048 (0.023, 0.101)0.012 (0.005, 0.025)−0.031 (−0.074, −0.009)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyepiandrosterone sulfate0.783 (0.413, 1.25)1.81 (0.938, 3.18)0.974 (0.203, 1.94)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyetiocholanolone0.927 (0.576, 1.39)0.422 (0.241, 0.673)−0.44 (−0.799, −0.213)<0.001
11β-Hydroxyetiocholanolone sulfate3.01 (1.82, 4.85)1.77 (1.05, 2.93)−1.06 (−2.17, −0.174)<0.001
Note: p-values show significant differences between trimesters.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the DS-R (Core, Animal reminder, Contamination disgust subscale scores and the overall DS-R score) and the Pathogen domain of the TDDS in the first (T1) and third (T3) trimester.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the DS-R (Core, Animal reminder, Contamination disgust subscale scores and the overall DS-R score) and the Pathogen domain of the TDDS in the first (T1) and third (T3) trimester.
QuestionnairenMeanMedian
(Quartiles)
SDMin.Max.Cronbach’s Alpha
Overall DS-R T116956.255 (45, 66)14.321930.843
Overall DS-R T317655.156 (44.8, 66)15.315910.874
Core T116929.430 (25, 35)6.9611480.676
Core T317628.428 (23, 34)7.568470.763
Contamination T11698.538 (6, 11)3.622190.562
Contamination T31768.539 (6,11)3.700190.622
Animal reminder T116918.318 (14, 23)6.373320.760
Animal reminder T317618.219 (13, 23)6.660320.792
Pathogen TDDS T117423.324 (17, 28)7.354400.734
Pathogen TDDS T317623.423 (18, 29)7.707410.798
Note: The table shows the total number of participants who filled out each questionnaire at each time point.
Table 3. Associations between disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the first trimester of pregnancy.
Table 3. Associations between disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the first trimester of pregnancy.
OPLS
(Predictive Component)
Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR aRegression Coefficientt-Statistics
DS-R, Overall score
Androstenediol0.9882.91*0.2898.960.559**0.0453.32**
Estrone0.9393.24**−0.062−2.43−0.114*−0.042−2.15*
17-Hydroxypregnanolone1.2452.88*0.2253.180.434**0.0562.72*
5β-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol0.7892.04*0.2284.580.441**0.0362.60*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione1.0254.26**0.36111.280.697**0.0463.35**
Androsterone1.0963.30**0.3889.550.750**0.0493.13**
Androsterone sulfate0.9813.68**0.31011.080.595**0.0442.21*
Epiandrosterone1.0102.81*0.3897.710.752**0.0461.95*
Epiandrosterone sulfate1.0925.81**0.3327.090.640**0.0493.21**
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol0.8222.76*0.3548.290.686**0.0374.73**
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol1.0072.34*0.30810.120.595**0.0452.08*
5β-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.0822.35*0.1944.040.369**0.0492.09*
3α,5β-Tetrahydrocorticosterone0.8252.57*0.1653.050.320**0.0371.66
DS-R, Overall score, trimester 1 1.0002.910.313*
Explained variability9.8% (6.8% after cross−validation)
DS-R, Core disgust
Androstenediol0.9982.34*0.3059.630.610**0.0512.14*
Testosterone1.1994.01**0.3366.800.672**0.0613.96**
5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.9743.16**0.38916.630.780**0.0502.78*
Estradiol sulfate0.7092.36*−0.017−0.41−0.028 −0.036−2.56*
Conjugated pregnanolone0.5981.94*−0.166−3.24−0.327**−0.031−2.06*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione1.1103.52**0.3595.490.719**0.0573.46**
Androsterone0.9238.32**0.39413.880.788**0.0477.34**
Androsterone sulfate0.9883.28**0.3155.400.632**0.0513.18**
Epiandrosterone sulfate0.9443.92**0.3348.530.670**0.0483.79**
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.1394.40**0.40916.910.820**0.0584.31**
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol1.2306.41**0.2166.000.423**0.0636.28**
DS-R, Core disgust, trimester 1 1.0007.400.338**
Explained variability11.4% (9.7% after cross−validation)
DS-R, Contamination disgust
Conjugated testosterone0.8202.78*−0.091−1.22−0.145 −0.051−2.06*
Estrone0.8106.88**0.2306.330.373**0.0513.54**
17-Hydroxypregnanolone0.8562.34*0.1060.960.170 0.0533.10**
Androsterone sulfate1.2142.76*0.4878.880.789**0.0762.31*
Epiandrosterone sulfate1.2213.39**0.5139.210.832**0.0762.55*
Etiocholanolone sulfate0.7882.31*0.39311.470.640**0.0491.99*
Epietiocholanolone sulfate1.2022.91*0.4729.850.751**0.0752.60*
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol0.9532.40*0.4016.130.654**0.0601.75
DS-R, Contamination disgust, trimester 1 1.0002.250.286*
Explained variability8.2% (5.2% after cross−validation)
DS-R, Animal reminder disgust
estosterone0.7522.24*0.3282.900.505*0.0611.89
Estrone1.0724.66**0.45010.640.723**0.0875.98**
Estradiol1.2436.54**0.44411.460.676**0.1004.63**
5β,20α-Tetrahydroprogesterone0.8332.02*0.3763.650.576**0.0672.14*
Conjugated 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol0.9403.08**0.3936.550.605**0.0762.89*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione1.0783.89**0.4498.420.690**0.0873.03**
DS-R, Animal reminder disgust, trimester 1 1.0002.570.303*
Explained variability9.2% (7.4% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Associations between Pathogen disgust measured via the TDDS and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the first trimester of pregnancy.
Table 4. Associations between Pathogen disgust measured via the TDDS and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the first trimester of pregnancy.
OPLS
(Predictive Component)
Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR a Regression Coefficientt-Statistics
Testosterone0.5211.94*0.1371.270.195 0.0561.71
5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.8963.30**0.3316.400.468**0.0972.45*
Estradiol0.7662.72*0.1682.530.231*0.0832.66*
5β,20α-Tetrahydroprogesterone1.1273.50**−0.326−2.76−0.462*−0.122−3.41**
5β-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol0.7922.11*0.4949.740.698**0.0861.98*
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol1.5655.93**0.5678.510.800**0.1696.74**
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione0.9966.58**0.50410.660.712**0.1084.69**
TDDS, Pathogen disgust, trimester 1 1.0004.380.407**
Explained variability16.6% (13.5% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Associations between disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the third trimester of pregnancy.
Table 5. Associations between disgust sensitivity measured via the DS-R and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the third trimester of pregnancy.
OPLS
(Predictive Component)
Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR a Regression Coefficientt-Statistics
DS-R, Overall score
7α-Hydroxy-DHEA0.7571.96*0.3908.960.626**0.0912.18*
Androstenediol0.8563.01**0.3566.130.572**0.1522.69*
5β-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.2138.34**0.40112.350.641**0.1642.37*
Cortisol0.9614.38**0.3647.180.584**0.1373.89**
DS-R, Overall score, trimester 3 1.0004.300.415**
Explained variability11.7% (8.8% after cross-validation)
DS-R, Core disgust
Pregnenolone0.7852.65*0.2024.880.419**0.0411.85
17-Hydroxypregnenolone1.3614.82**0.37710.590.782**0.0715.51**
DHEA0.9243.14**0.3869.490.801**0.0482.66*
7α-Hydroxy-DHEA1.1535.17**0.40716.530.844**0.0603.29**
7β-Hydroxy-DHEA1.0304.15**0.37410.360.775**0.0542.56*
Androstenediol1.1255.00**0.2758.360.569**0.0593.68**
5-Androstene-3 β,7α,17β-triol0.9842.89*0.33611.140.696**0.0511.97*
5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.5662.21*0.2106.000.434**0.0292.20*
Epiandrosterone1.0533.40**0.3456.850.716**0.0556.77**
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol0.7922.45*0.2005.180.415**0.0411.76
DS-R, Core disgust, trimester 3 1.0003.010.342**
Explained variability11.7% (8.8% after cross-validation)
DS-R, Contamination disgust
Pregnenolone0.6903.19**0.1582.330.222*0.1052.24*
Testosterone1.0484.14**−0.376−5.44−0.546**−0.151−4.06**
Estradiol sulfate0.9502.59*−0.269−2.70−0.393*−0.158−2.06*
Estriol sulfate0.6362.53*−0.167−1.85−0.255 −0.134−2.69*
Conjugated pregnanolone1.0325.42**0.3186.550.445**0.0350.90
Conjugated epipregnanolone1.0262.79*0.3102.920.431*0.0521.08
Conjugated 5α,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone1.1405.96**0.3014.730.410**0.0953.07**
Conjugated 5α-pregnane-3α,20α-diol1.1313.01**0.2703.000.368**0.1031.32
Conjugated 5β,20α-tetrahydroprogesterone0.9782.60*0.2652.620.362*0.0800.95
Conjugated 5β-pregnane-3β,20α-diol0.8774.29**0.2823.940.386**0.0250.73
Maternal age1.3573.74**0.3903.650.569**0.2373.60**
Multipara (1: yes/0: no)0.9253.14**0.2922.890.428*0.1855.37**
DS-R, Contamination disgust, trimester 3 1.00013.560.570**
Explained variability32.5% (23.1% after cross-validation)
DS-R, Animal reminder disgust
Epiandrosterone1.0804.83**0.5327.500.676**0.1354.59**
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol0.8942.93*0.5056.190.641**0.1122.34*
5β-Androstane-3α,17β-diol0.9514.60**0.4777.100.605**0.1193.54**
Conjugated 5β-androstane-3α,17β-diol1.0632.98*0.4893.080.613**0.1332.50*
DS-R, Animal reminder disgust, trimester 3 1.0002.090.317*
Explained variability10% (7.8% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 6. Associations between Pathogen disgust measured via the TDDS and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the third trimester of pregnancy.
Table 6. Associations between Pathogen disgust measured via the TDDS and predictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in the third trimester of pregnancy.
OPLS
(Predictive Component)
Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR aRegression Coefficientt-Statistics
DHEA0.7373.04**0.3685.370.667**0.0562.72*
DHEA sulfate0.9073.42**0.43114.400.783**0.0683.21**
Androstenediol0.5331.93*0.2834.150.513**0.0402.46*
Androsterone sulfate1.1166.79**0.42812.160.778**0.0845.63**
Epiandrosterone1.3835.35**0.42610.600.773**0.1045.53**
Epiandrosterone sulfate1.1295.90**0.46517.830.845**0.0853.84**
5β-Androstane-3α,17β-diol0.9572.90*0.2502.870.453*0.0722.17*
TDDS, Pathogen disgust, trimester 3 1.0006.000.361**
Explained variability13% (11.1% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 7. Associations between Δdisgust (trimester 3−trimester 1) measured via the DS-R and predictors in the first trimester and Δpredictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in pregnancy.
Table 7. Associations between Δdisgust (trimester 3−trimester 1) measured via the DS-R and predictors in the first trimester and Δpredictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in pregnancy.
OPLS
(Predictive Component)
Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR aRegression Coefficientt-Statistics
ΔDS-R, Overall score
Pregnenolone sulfate0.7712.97*−0.180−8.45−0.527**−0.021−3.14**
17-Hydroxypregnenolone sulfate0.6172.77*−0.162−5.63−0.470**−0.017−2.69*
16α-Hydroxypregnenolone0.7643.25**−0.194−6.32−0.569**−0.021−3.56**
DHEA sulfate1.0802.44*−0.207−5.54−0.605**−0.030−2.85*
7α-Hydroxy-DHEA0.7932.73*−0.237−5.40−0.693**−0.022−3.93**
7α-oxo-DHEA0.6422.71*−0.152−10.42−0.444**−0.018−2.29*
Androstenediol0.6413.62**−0.185−6.52−0.541**−0.018−3.16**
Androstenediol sulfate0.8524.59**−0.122−3.62−0.355**−0.023−4.11**
20α-Dihydroprogesterone0.6641.98*−0.092−2.22−0.268*−0.018−1.53
17-Hydroxyprogesterone0.9554.46**−0.146−4.67−0.428**−0.026−3.39**
16α-Hydroxyprogesterone1.1796.49**−0.144−4.17−0.420**−0.032−3.91**
17,20α-Dihydroxy-4-pregnen-3-one0.9193.25**−0.179−6.10−0.527**−0.025−2.61*
Androstenedione1.2666.07**−0.227−9.73−0.663**−0.035−3.75**
Testosterone1.4169.51**−0.232−8.69−0.680**−0.039−4.74**
5α-Dihydrotestosterone1.1645.27**−0.244−17.51−0.712**−0.032−4.52**
Estrone0.7183.21**−0.094−4.67−0.265**−0.020−4.78**
Estradiol0.7792.92*−0.105−4.24−0.304**−0.021−2.62*
17α-Hydroxyallopregnanolone1.2423.34**−0.167−4.75−0.490**−0.034−2.33*
17α-Hydroxypregnanolone1.3053.71**−0.125−2.41−0.366*−0.036−2.35*
5β-Pregnane-3α,17,20α-triol0.9703.31**−0.140−4.81−0.412**−0.027−2.44*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione1.4374.69**−0.233−6.53−0.682**−0.039−3.48**
Androsterone1.55212.86**−0.275−15.44−0.804**−0.042−4.75**
Androsterone sulfate0.7992.57*−0.134−4.14−0.391**−0.022−2.89*
Epiandrosterone1.2956.09**−0.260−9.38−0.761**−0.035−4.87**
Epiandrosterone sulfate0.8081.93*−0.151−5.84−0.441**−0.022−2.19*
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.61912.04**−0.260−14.96−0.762**−0.044−6.40**
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol1.1845.87**−0.192−10.04−0.561**−0.032−7.80**
Maternal age0.6922.67*0.1253.890.364**0.0192.08*
Male sex of the fetus0.7902.16*−0.035−1.26−0.103 −0.022−2.45*
DS-R Overall score0.8513.18**−0.066−2.62−0.194*−0.023−2.64*
Pregnancy length0.5642.04*0.0512.650.148*0.0152.27*
ΔDHEA sulfate1.1282.11*0.1945.720.566**0.0312.27*
ΔAndrostenediol0.8572.49*0.1564.550.458**0.0232.07*
Δ5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.9804.69**0.1495.410.437**0.0275.27**
ΔAndrosterone0.7543.00*0.1428.920.418**0.0212.54*
Δ5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol0.8482.06*0.1045.150.303**0.0232.39*
ΔAndrostenediol sulfate0.6082.34*0.0100.310.029 0.0172.31*
ΔDS-R, Overall score 1.00011.930.486**
Explained variability23.7% (19.3% after cross-validation)
ΔDS-R, Core disgust
17-Hydroxyprogesterone0.9142.49*−0.257−2.99−0.519*−0.044−1.82
16α-Hydroxyprogesterone0.7713.95**−0.203−2.47−0.410*−0.037−2.45*
Androstenedione0.9292.58*−0.361−7.89−0.729**−0.045−2.12*
Testosterone0.88611.65**−0.349−14.24−0.702**−0.043−4.23**
17-Hydroxypregnanolone1.2382.91*−0.220−2.24−0.446*−0.060−2.07*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione0.7934.33**−0.359−16.89−0.724**−0.038−2.82*
Androsterone0.8624.60**−0.373−7.41−0.752**−0.042−3.36**
Androsterone sulfate1.3795.09**−0.288−3.92−0.573**−0.067−7.75**
Epiandrosterone sulfate1.1944.75**−0.295−3.48−0.587**−0.058−10.59**
Epietiocholanolone sulfate0.7692.34*−0.227−2.75−0.450*−0.037−2.89*
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.0682.74*−0.342−6.91−0.689**−0.052−2.78*
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol0.8394.42**−0.273−4.22−0.550**−0.041−6.07**
DS-R, Overall score1.1213.30**−0.154−2.42−0.312*−0.054−3.09**
ΔDS-R, Core disgust 1.0002.800.352*
Explained variability12.4% (9.9% after cross-validation)
ΔDS-R, Contamination disgust
Pregnenolone sulfate0.6952.53*−0.190−10.55−0.427**−0.031−2.05*
20α-Dihydropregnenolone0.8684.04**−0.145−5.26−0.325**−0.038−4.67**
DHEA sulfate0.7302.68*−0.205−5.05−0.460**−0.032−3.03**
Androstenediol0.8724.48**−0.207−8.62−0.468**−0.039−5.64**
Androstenediol sulfate0.9123.64**−0.141−3.66−0.316**−0.040−2.66*
Androstenedione1.1712.79*−0.280−7.71−0.633**−0.052−3.31**
Testosterone1.0412.11*−0.278−4.95−0.628**−0.046−2.26*
Estrone0.9352.73*−0.186−5.90−0.399**−0.041−2.63*
Allopregnanolone0.9424.42**−0.172−4.80−0.389**−0.042−4.26**
Allopregnanolone sulfate0.7092.18*−0.073−1.89−0.164 −0.031−1.85
Isopregnanolone sulfate0.7851.90*−0.104−2.76−0.233*−0.035−1.59
17α-Hydroxypregnanolone1.2752.97*−0.199−5.27−0.448**−0.056−3.48**
5α,20α-Tetrahydroprogesterone0.9144.62**−0.164−3.81−0.372**−0.040−3.60**
5α-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol0.6324.27**−0.150−4.82−0.338**−0.028−5.52**
5β-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol0.7332.60*−0.150−4.56−0.340**−0.032−2.89*
5β-Pregnane-3β,20α-diol0.9004.13**−0.130−3.59−0.279**−0.040−4.20**
5β-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol0.9672.73*−0.203−5.01−0.457**−0.043−2.90*
5α-Androstane-3,17-dione1.2745.03**−0.318−13.20−0.720**−0.056−3.40**
Androsterone1.2225.01**−0.319−9.19−0.720**−0.054−5.36**
Androsterone sulfate1.3846.74**−0.246−6.37−0.553**−0.061−5.98**
Epiandrosterone0.7613.80**−0.269−17.69−0.609**−0.034−2.80*
Epiandrosterone sulfate1.1814.83**−0.246−8.24−0.551**−0.052−3.88**
Etiocholanolone0.8182.29*−0.131−2.69−0.296*−0.036−2.56*
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3α,17β-diol0.8703.59**−0.151−3.41−0.336**−0.039−3.15**
Conjugated 5α-androstane-3β,17β-diol0.8742.10*−0.136−3.15−0.301**−0.039−1.84
Cortisone1.2563.77**−0.202−4.02−0.456**−0.056−2.65*
Maternal age0.9653.43**0.1535.170.344**0.0433.14**
Pregnancy length1.1423.74**−0.077−1.15−0.172 −0.051−3.04**
ΔAndrostenediol sulfate1.47718.03**−0.133−6.36−0.298**−0.065−7.19**
ΔPregnancy length0.9823.15**0.0812.370.182*0.0432.69*
ΔDS-R, Contamination disgust 1.00011.030.545**
Explained variability29.7% (24.9% after cross-validation)
ΔDS-R, Animal reminder disgust
7-oxo-DHEA0.8372.81*−0.165−4.43−0.423**−0.029−3.41**
20α-Dihydroprogesterone1.2103.93**−0.238−4.74−0.608**−0.042−4.37**
16α-Hydroxyprogesterone1.1505.03**−0.248−6.41−0.634**−0.040−5.40**
17α,20α-Dihydroxy-4-pregnene-3-one0.8512.17*−0.239−6.31−0.612**−0.030−2.95*
Testosterone0.9903.34**−0.271−13.71−0.692**−0.035−3.99**
5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.9794.33**−0.258−5.93−0.660**−0.034−3.38**
Estradiol0.8442.62*−0.164−3.95−0.414**−0.030−2.50*
17α-Hydroxyallopregnanolone0.8984.74**−0.263−11.02−0.674**−0.031−3.03**
17α-Hydroxypregnanolone0.9813.14**−0.210−5.96−0.539**−0.034−2.82*
3α,5α-Tetrahydroprogsterone1.0422.61*−0.250−8.76−0.639**−0.036−2.59*
5α-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol0.8402.34*−0.248−5.20−0.635**−0.029−2.52*
3α,5β-Tetrahydroprogsterone1.4463.89**−0.204−7.26−0.521**−0.051−2.67*
5β-Pregnane-3α,20α-diol1.1463.78**−0.204−12.24−0.521**−0.040−2.69*
5β-Pregnane-3α,17α,20α-triol0.7744.00**−0.212−8.23−0.545**−0.027−3.78**
Androsterone0.97211.39**−0.273−7.07−0.699**−0.034−6.20**
Epiandrosterone1.0233.51**−0.215−4.07−0.550**−0.036−2.94*
5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.2313.52**−0.262−6.79−0.670**−0.043−2.91*
5α-Androstane-3β,17β-diol1.1481.93*−0.217−3.69−0.554**−0.040−1.79
Parity0.9083.68**0.0811.600.206 0.0322.84*
DS-R, Animal reminder disgust0.9443.23**−0.080−2.67−0.205*−0.033−2.36*
Pregnancy length0.7162.65*0.1143.140.292**0.0253.18**
Δ5α-Dihydrotestosterone0.7143.04**0.2045.830.523**0.0252.76*
Δ5α-Androstane-3α,17β-diol1.0133.00**0.1869.320.477**0.0352.32*
ΔPregnancy length0.9932.67*0.1022.130.261*0.0353.09**
ΔDS-R, Animal reminder disgust 1.00020.080.438**
Explained variability19.2% (15.3% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 8. Associations between ΔPathogen disgust (trimester 3−trimester 1) measured via the TDDS and predictors in the first trimester and Δpredictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in pregnancy.
Table 8. Associations between ΔPathogen disgust (trimester 3−trimester 1) measured via the TDDS and predictors in the first trimester and Δpredictors evaluated via an OPLS model and multiple regression in pregnancy.
OPLS, Predictive Component Multiple
Regression
VariableVariable Importancet-StatisticsComponent Loadingt-StatisticsR a Regression Coefficientt-Statistics
Pregnenolone0.6422.83*0.1831.980.259*0.0751.98*
TDDS_PATHOGEN1.0014.05**−0.333−4.28−0.474**−0.118−4.83**
ΔEstrone sulfate1.1403.91**0.51710.610.740**0.1342.92*
ΔEstradiol sulfate1.0613.80**0.56616.620.804**0.1252.67*
ΔEstriol sulfate1.0779.86**0.55314.690.804**0.1276.58**
ΔTDDS, Pathogen disgust 1.0002.720.375*
Explained variability14% (11.2% after cross-validation)
a R. Component loadings expressed as correlation coefficients with predictive component; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kaňková, Š.; Dlouhá, D.; Ullmann, J.; Velíková, M.; Včelák, J.; Hill, M. Association between Disgust Sensitivity during Pregnancy and Endogenous Steroids: A Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25, 6857. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25136857

AMA Style

Kaňková Š, Dlouhá D, Ullmann J, Velíková M, Včelák J, Hill M. Association between Disgust Sensitivity during Pregnancy and Endogenous Steroids: A Longitudinal Study. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2024; 25(13):6857. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25136857

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kaňková, Šárka, Daniela Dlouhá, Jana Ullmann, Marta Velíková, Josef Včelák, and Martin Hill. 2024. "Association between Disgust Sensitivity during Pregnancy and Endogenous Steroids: A Longitudinal Study" International Journal of Molecular Sciences 25, no. 13: 6857. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25136857

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop