Next Article in Journal
“Enclave in Transition”: Ways of Coping of Academics from Ultra-Orthodox (Haredim) Minority Group with Challenges of Integration into the Workforce
Next Article in Special Issue
Early Motor Trajectories Predict Motor but not Cognitive Function in Preterm- and Term-Born Adults without Pre-existing Neurological Conditions
Previous Article in Journal
Environmental Factors that Impact the Workplace Participation of Transition-Aged Young Adults with Brain-Based Disabilities: A Scoping Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Maternal Mental Illness Is Associated with Adverse Neonate Outcomes: An Analysis of Inpatient Data
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children

by
Miguel Pérez-Pereira
1,*,
María Pilar Fernández
2,
María Luisa Gómez-Taibo
2,
Zeltia Martínez-López
1 and
Constantino Arce
3
1
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, 15782 Santiago de Compostela, Spain
2
Department of Psychology, University of A Coruña, 15190 A Coruña, Spain
3
Deparment of Social, Basic and Methodological Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, 15782 Santiago de Compostela, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(7), 2380; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17072380
Submission received: 3 March 2020 / Revised: 28 March 2020 / Accepted: 28 March 2020 / Published: 31 March 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes)

Abstract

:
The results of a longitudinal study on the cognitive development of one group of full-term and three groups of low risk preterm children with different gestational ages (GA) are presented. The 181 participants were divided into four GA groups of similar size. The aims were: 1) To check if there are differences in cognitive development (measured through the Batelle scale) among the GA groups. 2) To establish the predictive factors of cognitive development at 22 and 60 months of age, taking into account biomedical, environmental and individual factors. The results of the repeated measures ANOVA performed at 22 and 60 months of age indicated that the cognitive trajectories of the four GA groups were similar. Linear regression analyses showed that the effect of the different predictors changed in relation to the time of measurement of cognitive development. Biological factors and the quality of home environment had a moderate effect on the cognitive development at 22 months of age. Cognitive results obtained at 22 months of age, and, to a lesser extent, working memory had the greatest effect on cognitive development at 60 months. GA does not predict cognitive development. Preterm children do not show cognitive delay if they are healthy.

1. Introduction

Cognitive measures of children should be taken early to detect any developmental delay or differing patterns of development so that intervention programs may be provided. For this reason, follow up studies of the general cognitive abilities of preterm (PT) children in conjunction with those bio-medical, environmental and personal factors associated with cognitive outcomes are of great relevance [1]. In this introduction we will first review previous studies on cognitive development of preterm children. Secondly, we will identify those factors which predict cognitive development from the literature review.
Evidence exists that preterm children may show cognitive impairments in different domains when compared to full-term children [2]. A wide spectrum of cognitive impairments are observed among very preterm or very low birth weight (BW) children [3].
Gestational age (GA) is associated to cognitive development in such a way that the lower the GA, the worse the cognitive performance of preschool and school age PT children [4,5,6]. Differences between full-term and preterm children are particularly high between extremely preterm (EPT) or very preterm (VPT) and full-term (FT) children [3,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. There is no unanimity, however, among the studies carried out with EPT or VPT children, and heterogeneity of effect size across studies is important [21]. A few studies did not find such dramatic differences between these children and FT children [24,25,26,27,28,29].
Moderate (MPT) or late (LPT) preterm birth, however, seems to affect cognitive development in a lesser way. Yaari et al. [30] observed that the trajectories of cognitive development of MPT were more favorable than those of EPT and VPT children, although of higher vulnerability in relation to FT children. Cognitive performance of MPT or LPT children between 2 and 7 years of age of age was found to be lower than that of FT children [31,32,33,34,35].
In contrast with the above reported results, a review study [36] and several investigations [37,38] did not observe significant differences between LPT and FT children of different ages. Other studies carried out with healthy LPT children did not find differences with FT children or with normative normal range at preschool ages [39] or between 4 and 15 years of age [40]. Even low risk preterm children with GA below 32 weeks (VPT) without major health problems obtained cognitive scores in the normal range, although significantly lower than those of the group of full-term children [41].
These latter results seem to indicate that healthy condition is an important factor which prevents cognitive delay. The need of new studies is particularly relevant given the dearth of longitudinal studies on cognitive development of low risk preterm children [31].
Different factors were suggested as predictors of cognitive outcomes in PT children. We will first comment on biomedical factors. GA (and BW) were considered the most relevant predictors of cognitive development [5,42,43], although other authors did not find this relationship to exist [27]. Prematurity probably interacts in a dynamic way with genetic, bio-medical, and environmental factors throughout development [8,14].
Major brain pathology was found to affect low cognitive level [13,17], and neonatal white matter abnormalities (in addition to family socioeconomic adversity) predicted cognitive risk in VPT children [16,44]. Bronchopulmonary dysplasia was found to be a crucial factor for cognitive outcome [21].
Further research is needed to determine if length of stay in neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) is an independent predictor of poor neurodevelopment outcome [45,46]. Evidence of the influence of the Apgar scores on cognitive outcomes is confusing. Apgar tests are administered to newborn children in the first minute of life (and also at minutes 5 and 10, if necessary) and provides a measure of the health state of the children in five vital areas, offering a score from 0 to 10. Children with low Apgar scores were more likely to have low IQ score at 18 years of age [47], however, unit increase in the Apgar score at 5 minutes was not associated with significant decreases in vulnerability on the language and communication domain at 5 years of age [48].
Male sex and non-white ethnicity (together with low parental education and low BW) were predictive of cognitive impairments in VPT at 5 years of age [13,49].
Maternal prenatal smoking has detrimental effects on prenatal brain development [50], is a risk for suboptimal cognitive and neuropsychological development in preterm infants [51,52], and was consistently associated with decrements in test scores of academic performance [53], and increased risk of poor scholastic achievement [54]. Maternal smoking habits has also been found to have a negative effect on cognitive development at 4 years of age [55], although this effect has been called into question [56]. Mother’s age at birth was associated with increasing cognitive vulnerability in a population-based cohort study [57].
As children grow older, however, environmental factors and social context seem to explain the greatest part of variation in cognitive development, and the influence of perinatal risk factors appears to diminish over time [49,58,59,60,61].
Socioeconomic status (SES) seems to be a prevalent factor related to cognitive development of EPT children. The risk of cognitive deficits seems to rise under adverse socioeconomic conditions [16,18,62], and the opposite seems to be true too: upper income level acts as a protection factor for EPT children [5]. Wong and Edwards [63] discovered that the mother’s educational level was the most highly valued indicator of socio-economic status as well as the factor that was most consistently associated with cognitive development in PT children. Low parental education was found to predict global cognitive impairment in VPT children [5,49]. Other studies corroborated that parents’ educational level is the best predictor of intelligence in PT children with different ages and prematurity levels [13,41,64,65,66,67].
Other environmental factors such as parental styles [58,59,68], maternal stimulation [69], employment quality and educational level of parents [70], marital status [71], do have an effect on the cognitive development of PT children.
Different investigations, carried out using the HOME scale to measure the quality and level of stimulation of the home environment, indicate the influence of the home environment on the cognitive development of PT children [27,72]. These results are of particular interest for our study, since we used this instrument.
In other investigations, the effect of individual or psychological variables was studied. The cognitive development achieved at earlier ages (associated or not to other factors) was found to be a predictor of cognitive outcomes [39,73,74,75,76]. These results point to a stability in cognitive measures throughout time, supporting the results found by Mangin et al. [16].
PT children, particularly VPT and EPT children, were found to perform worse than FT children in working memory tasks [70,77], which in turn affects cognitive achievements.
There is a dearth of longitudinal studies on the cognitive development of low risk PT children whose cognitive achievements and developmental trajectories may differ from other high risk and/or EPR or VPT children who have a higher probability of having biomedical problems.
The present investigation has the following aims:
(1)
To check if there are differences in cognitive development among groups of healthy children of different gestational ages.
(2)
To establish the predictive factors of cognitive development at 22 and 60 months of age, taking into account biomedical, environmental and individual factors.
The hypotheses of the study are:
(1)
Given the low risk nature of the sample we do not expect to find differences in cognitive development between the GA groups.
(2)
Biomedical factors will have a higher effect on early cognitive development, and their influence on cognitive development will vanish later.
(3)
Environmental factors will increase their effect on cognitive development as children grow older.
(4)
There will be an influence of previous cognitive achievements on later cognitive development.

2. Materials and Methods

This study was part of a longer longitudinal project.

2.1. Participants

There were 181 children of different gestational ages who were assessed at 22 months of age and were followed up to the age of 60 months. The children were classified into 4 GA groups: 1) very preterm (VPT) and extremely preterm (EPT) children with GA of 31 weeks or below; 2) moderately preterm children (MPT) with GA between 32 and 33 weeks; 3) late preterm children (LPT) with GA between 34 and 36 weeks; and 4) full term children (FT) with GA of 37 weeks or above. We intended that the number of participants in each group was similar for analysis purposes, given the scarcity of children EPT. Obviously, there was a loss in the number of participants from 22 to 60 months (see below).
The group of PT children did not show any additional serious complications. Children were excluded from the sample if they had any of the following characteristics: cerebral palsy, periventricular leukomalacia, intraventricular hemorrhage higher than II, encephalopathy, hydrocephalus, genetic malformations, chromosomal syndromes, metabolic syndromes associated to mental retardation, severe motor or sensorial impairments, or Apgar scores below 6 at 5 min.
The characteristics of the PT children together with the exclusion criteria used allow us to consider the sample as a low risk PT group.
At 22 months of age, the 138 PT children had a mean GA (and SD) of 32.62 (2.41), a mean BW of 1721.70 (435.36), and a mean Apgar score (first minute) of 7.94 (1.30). As for the 43 FT children, they had a mean GA of 39.70 (1.48), a mean BW of 3373.83 (433.09), and a mean Apgar score (1st minute) of 8.13 (1.20). Maternal education has been categorized into three levels: 1) Basic maternal education (primary and secondary education), 2) High school and technical school and 3) University degree.
Stay in NICU was categorized into three levels 1) No stay, 2) 1-15 days, 3) over 15 days.
The characteristics of the four gestational age groups and chi square and one factor ANOVA comparisons among them are shown in Table 1.
There were no differences between the four GA groups in most of the variables displayed in Table 1, except for stay in NICU, mother’s age, and Apgar score in the first minute. As for the results of the chi square test, the PT children presented longer stays than FT children, as expected. The higher the degree of prematurity, the longer the stay in NICU. In the case of the ANOVA, Bonferroni post hoc analysis indicated that significant differences (p < 0.05) in Apgar score between the GA group of ≤31 weeks and the other 3 groups (32–33, 34–36 and ≥37 weeks of gestational age) were responsible for the results. The group of children with GA ≤31 weeks obtained significantly lower Apgar scores than the other groups.
In the case of mother’s age, significant differences were found between the group of children with GA ≥37 weeks and the groups of children with gestational ages of ≤31 and 32–33 weeks. The group with GA ≥37 had mothers significantly younger than the others.
Although the number of participants varied at each point of measurement, the characteristics of the PT sample did not vary throughout the period studied. The number of participants at each point of cognitive ability measurement appears below.

2.2. Procedure

Parents’ consent, and approval by the Galician Ethics Committee of Clinical Research (code 2008/010) were obtained before the beginning of the investigation. The children were assessed when they were 22, 48 and 60 months of age. Corrected age for PT children was used at 22 months of age. The following instruments were administered at the indicated ages.

2.3. Instruments

When the children were 22 months of age and 60 months of age, the Spanish version of the Batelle Developmental Inventory (BDI) [78] was administered to assess cognitive development. The skills assessed by the BDI scale are adaptive, personal-social, communication, motor, and cognitive. The cognitive raw score was used for the analysis. The cognitive score is composed of the following elements: perceptive discrimination, memory, reasoning and school skills, and conceptual development.
The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) [79] in its Spanish version [80] was used. The HOME is designed to measure the quality and quantity of stimulation and support available to a child in the home environment. The information is gathered through personal interview to mothers and through observation of the children’s homes and the interactions between the children and the mothers during the interview session. Higher total HOME scores indicate a more enriched home environment. The version for infants and toddlers (0–2 years) was administered when the children were 22 months of age. This inventory is composed of 45 items that are presented as statements to be scored as YES or NO. The HOME includes six subscales: Emotional and verbal responsiveness of the primary caregiver, Avoidance and restriction of punishment, Organization of the physical and temporal environment, Provision of appropriate play materials, Parental involvement with the child, and Opportunities for variety in daily stimulation. When the children were 48 months of age, the HOME scale for preschool children (3–5 years) was administered. This inventory is composed of 55 items that are presented as statements to be scored as YES or NO. The HOME includes seven subscales: Learning materials, Linguistic stimulation, Physical environment, Tenderness and affection, Academic stimulation, Modeling, Diversity of experiences and Acceptance.
The total score of the two HOME versions (for infants and toddlers and for preschoolers) was used for the analyses. The results obtained by the four GA groups in the two HOME tasks are offered in Table 1.
The CORSI ordering task [81,82] was used to assess the non-verbal working memory when the children were 60 months-old. Colored blocks are highlighted in a given sequence. The children must repeat the sequence. The total raw score was used for the analysis. The results of the Corsi task are shown in Table 1.
In addition, a sociodemographic and health interview was applied to the mothers shortly after the child’s birth, and biomedical information was collected from medical records. Information on gender, gestational age, length of stay in NICU, Apgar score at 1st minute, mother’s age, number of cigarettes smoked during pregnancy per day and mothers’ educational level, among other factors, was gathered.

2.4. Analysis Performed

First of all the data gathered with the BDI at 22 and 60 months of age were analyzed using a 2 (age) × 4 (GA groups) repeated measures ANOVA in order to test if there were intra subjects differences (age related differences in the same participants), inter subjects differences among GA groups and a combined effect age × GA group.
A linear regression analysis was performed using BDI cognitive score at 22 months of age as dependent variable (DV). As independent variables (IV), gender, gestational age (numerical), stay in NICU, Apgar score at 1st minute, mother’s age, and number of cigarettes smoked during pregnancy per day were introduced in Model 1. In addition to those factors, mother’s educational level and the total score obtained in the HOME scale at 22 months of age were introduced in Model 2. As can be observed, Model 1 included biomedical factors, while in Model 2 environmental factors were added, in order to test the effect of these types of factors on cognitive development at 22 months of age.
A linear regression analysis was performed using BDI cognitive score at 60 months of age as DV. As independent variables, gender, gestational age (numerical), stay in NICU, Apgar score at 1st minute, mother’s age, and number of cigarettes smoked during pregnancy per day were introduced in Model 1. In addition to those, mother’s educational level and the total score obtained in the HOME scale at 48 months of age were introduced in Model 2. Finally, in Model 3 the BDI cognitive score at 22 months, and the Corsi score at 60 months were added.

3. Results

Descriptive results obtained in the two cognitive measures taken at 22 and 60 months of age in the 4 GA groups are shown in Table 2.
In contrast with former studies [70,77], we did not find significant differences between FT and PR children of any GA group in working memory performance. The results of the HOME scale indicate that the children’s families offered similar experiences and opportunities for development to the children, independently of the group.
The results of the 2 (age) × 4 (GA groups) repeated measures ANOVA performed on the results obtained in the BDI at 22 and 60 months of age indicate that there was a highly significant effect of age (intra-subjects differences) on cognitive development (F(1)= 5315.463, p < 0.001, η² = 0.975). No significant combined effect of age × GA groups was found (F(3)= 0.930, p > 0.05, η² = 0.020), and no significant difference among GA groups (inter-subjects effects) was found (F(3)= 0.923, p > 0.05, η² = 0.020).
The linear regression analysis performed using BDI cognitive score at 22 months of age (BDI 22) as DV (see Table 3) indicates that the introduction of biomedical variables in Model 1 has a significant effect on cognitive development (p < 0.01) and explains 10.2% of the variance (R2 = 0.102). The variables which have a significant effect (Standardized β) are gender and mother’s age at birth. When environmental variables (maternal educational level, and HOME score at 22 months) are added in Model 2, the effect of the independent variables on cognitive development (BDI 22) increments the significance (p < 0.001), and the variance explained reaches 0.213 (change in R2 = 0.111). Now, the variables which have a significant effect are gender and mother’s age at birth (as in Model 1), plus the quality of home environment (HOME total score at 22 months of age).
The linear regression analysis performed using BDI cognitive score at 60 months of age as DV (BDI 60) (see Table 4) indicates that the introduction of the biomedical variables in Model 1 does not have any significant effect on the variance of cognitive development (BDI 60). The variance explained is minimal (R2 = 0.014), and no single variable reaches significance. When environmental variables (mother’s educational level and HOME score at 48 months) are added in Model 2, the variance explained increases 0.068 (change in F is significant), and the model reaches significance (p < 0.05). The only variable which reaches a unique significant effect is the HOME score administered at 48 months of age (p < 0.05). In Model 3 previous cognitive score (BDI 22) is introduced, together with the Corsi score. Model 3 reaches a highly significant effect on cognitive development measured at 60 months of age (p < 0.001), and the introduction of the BDI cognitive score at 22 months of age and the Corsi score significantly incremented the variance explained (Change in R2 = 0.165). Working memory and previous cognitive score have a unique significant effect (standardized β), which was particularly high in the case of BDI 22 previous cognitive score (p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

The first objective of this research was to check if differences exist in the cognitive development of 4 groups of children with different gestational ages, and, in general terms, healthy. The results of the repeated measures ANOVA performed on the raw scores obtained with the BDI at 22 and 60 months of age clearly indicate that there was no significant difference among the four groups. There was no inter-subjects difference, and no combined effect of age and GA group. The latter result indicates that the trajectories that the children with different GAs follow are similar. As logical, the analysis performed with the raw scores point to a very important age effect (η² = 0.975) on cognitive development. The effect of belonging to a different GA group has practically no effect (η²= 0.020).
These results reinforce the idea that low risk PT children do not have cognitive delay as compared to FT children [37,40,41], and that GA on its own does not seem to affect cognitive development at least up to the age of 5 years, unless GA is associated with other factors (such as biomedical problems, or unfavorable environmental circumstances) [8,14]. Even comparing the most distant GA groups (FT versus VPT and EPT groups), no significant difference was found. The results found reinforce the idea that healthy condition is an important factor which prevents cognitive delay.
With regard to the second objective, the results of the linear regression analyses showed that the effect of the different predictors changes in relation to the time of measurement of cognitive development. Biological factors seem to have a significant effect on cognitive development measured early in development (22 months of age), particularly gender and mother’s age. The results indicate that boys tend to have lower results than girls [13,49], and that children whose mothers are younger have higher cognitive results during infancy [57]. These effects, however, do not exist when cognitive development is measured later in development, at 60 months of age. This result indicates that the effect of biomedical factors on cognitive achievement descends as children develop [49,59].
In the case of environmental factors, the quality of home environment stands out as the most important factor and it has an effect on cognitive development at 22 months. Therefore, both biological and environmental variables (particularly gender, mother’s age at birth and HOME score) have a significant effect on cognitive development at 22 months of age. In any case the biological and environmental variables introduced in model 2 explain 0.213 of the variance of the BDI cognitive score at 22 months of age, and, therefore, the model reaches significance. The effect of environmental factors is higher in early cognitive development (BDI 22) (Change in R2 = 0.111) than in later cognitive development (BDI 60) (Change in R2 = 0.068).
In relation to the explanation of cognitive score at 60 months of age, the biological variables introduced in model 1 have no significant effect and model 1 does not reach significance, while the introduction of environmental variables (particularly the HOME score) in model 2 increment the variance explained to 0.082 and the model reaches significance, reinforcing the predictive role of the HOME scores on cognitive development [27,72].
By far, the variables which have a higher effect on cognitive development at 60 months are the cognitive result obtained earlier in development (BDI 22), and the working memory. The introduction of these independent variables makes the variance explained reach 0.232 (Change in R2 = 0.165), which is an important effect. The cognitive scores obtained at 22 months of age has the most significant effect, but non-verbal working memory score at 48 months have a moderate effect as well. At the same time these cognitive factors subsume the effect of the quality of home environment, which now, in model 3, does not reach significance.
Therefore, our results coincide with those studies which pointed to the relevance of former cognitive scores in the prediction of later cognitive development [39,73,74,75,76].
A final point, but not of minor relevance, is that gestational age does not have any significant effect on the prediction of cognitive development, indicating that GA is not a predictive factor of cognitive development when serious medical complications are controlled.

5. Conclusions

The most important conclusion of this study is that there are no differences in cognitive development among children with different gestational ages if they are healthy. Therefore, low risk PT children do not seem to present cognitive delay in relation to FT children, up to the age of 60 months. Future studies should test if differences might exist later in development.
Biological factors have a modest effect on first cognitive development as measured at 22 months of age, although these factors lose their effect on later cognitive development. Environmental factors (quality of home environment in particular) seem to be more important, and their effect continues over time. The most important predictors of cognitive development at 60 months of age are previous cognitive measures.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P.-P.; methodology, M.P.-P., M.P.F., M.L.G.-T. and C.A.; software, not applicable; validation, M.P.-P., M.P.F., M.L.G.-T. and Z.M.-L.; formal analysis, M.P.-P. and C.A.; investigation, M.P.-P., M.P.F., M.L.G.-T.; resources, M.P.-P.; data curation, Z.M.-L.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P.-P., M.P.F., M.L.G.-T.; writing—review and editing, M.P.-P., M.P.F., M.L.G.-T. and Z.M.-L.; visualization, Z.M.-L.; supervision, M.P.-P.; project administration, M.P.-P.; funding acquisition, M.P.-P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación of the Spanish Government, grants number PSI2008-03905, PSI2011-23210 and PSI2015-66697-R to the first author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lee, H.J.; Park, H.-K. Neurodevelopmental Outcome of Preterm Infants at Childhood: Cognition and Language. Hanyang Med. Rev. 2016, 36, 55–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Maxwell, J.R.; Yellowhair, T.; Oppong, A.; Camacho, J.; Lowe, J.; Jantzie, L.; Ohls, R. Cognitive Development in Preterm Infants: Multi-Faceted Deficits Reflect Vulnerability of Rigorous Neurodevelopmental Pathways. Minerva Pediatr. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Pascal, A.; Govaert, P.; Oostra, A.; Naulaers, G.; Ortibus, E.; Van den Broeck, C. Neurodevelopmental Outcome in Very Preterm and Very-Low-Birthweight Infants Born over the Past Decade: A Meta-Analytic Review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2018, 60, 342–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Anderson, P.J. Neuropsychological Outcomes of Children Born Very Preterm. Semin. Fetal. Neonatal Med. 2014, 19, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Brom Vieira, M.E.; Martins Linhares, M.B. Desenvolvimento e Qualidade de Vida Em Crianças Nascidas Pré-Termo Em Idades Pré-Escolar e Escolar. J. Pediatr. Rio J. 2011, 87, 281–291. [Google Scholar]
  6. Kerr-Wilson, C.; Mackay, D.F.; Smith, G.C.S.; Pell, J.P. Meta-Analysis of the Association between Preterm Delivery and Intelligence. J. Public Health 2012, 34, 209–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Anderson, P.J.; De Luca, C.R.; Hutchinson, E.; Spencer-Smith, M.M.; Roberts, G.; Doyle, L.V. Attention Problems in a Representative Sample of Extremely Preterm/Extremely Low Birth Weight Children. J. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Baron, I.S.; Erickson, K.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Baker, R.; Litman, F.R. Cognitive Deficit in Preschoolers Born Late-Preterm. Early Hum. Dev. 2011, 87, 115–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hoff-Esbjorn, B.; Hansen, B.M.; Greisen, G.; Mortensen, E.L. Intellectual Development in a Danish Cohort of Prematurely Born Preschool Children: Specific or General Difficulties? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hutchinson, E.A.; De Luca, C.R.; Doyle, L.W.; Roberts, G.; Anderson, P.J. School-Age Outcomes of Extremely Preterm or Extremely Low Birth Weight Children. Pediatrics 2013, 131, e1053–e1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Kim, H.S.; Kim, E.K.; Park, H.K.; Ahn, D.H.; Kim, M.J.; Lee, H.J. Cognitive Outcomes of Children with Very Low Birth Weight at 3 to 5 Years of Age. J. Korean Med. Sci. 2020, 35, e4–e16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Larson, G.; Baron, J.C.; Erickson, I.S.; Ahronovich, K.; Baker, M.D.; Litman, R.; Fern, R. Neuromotor Outcomes at School Age after Extremely Low Birth Weight: Early Detection of Subtle Signs. Neuropsychology 2011, 25, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Linsell, L.; Johnson, S.; Wolke, D.; O’Reilly, H.; Morris, J.K.; Kurinczuk, J.J.; Marlow, N. Cognitive Trajectories from Infancy to Early Adulthood Following Birth before 26 Weeks of Gestation: A Prospective, Population-Based Cohort Study. Arch Child 2018, 103, 63–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Lundequist, A.; Böhm, B.; Smedler, A.C. Individual Neuropsychological Profiles at Age 5½ Years in Children Born Preterm in Relation to Medical Risk Factors. Child Neuropsychol. J. Norm. Abnorm. Dev. Child. Adolesc. 2013, 19, 313–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Méio, M.D.; Lopes, C.S.; Streit-Morsch, D. Fatores Prognósticos Para o Desenvolvimento Cognitivo de Prematuros de Muito Baixo Peso. Rev. Saúde Pública 2004, 37, 311–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Mangin, K.; Horwood, L.; Woodward, L.J. Cognitive Development Trajectories of Very Preterm and Typically Developing Children. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 282–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Nyman, A.; Korhonen, T.; Munck, P.; Parkkola, R.; Lehtonen, L.; Haataja, L. Factors Affecting the Cognitive Profile of 11-Year-Old Children Born Very Preterm. Pediatr. Res. 2017, 82, 324–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Orchinik, L.J.; Taylor, H.G.; Espy, K.A.; Minich, N.; Klein, N.; Sheffield, T.; Hack, M. Cognitive Outcomes for Extremely Preterm/Extremely Low Birth Weight Children in Kindergarten. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 1067–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Pugliese, M.; Rossi, C.; Guidotti, I.; Gallo, C.; Della Casa, E.; Bertoncelli, N.; Coccolini, E.; Ferrari, F. Preterm Birth and Developmental Problems in Infancy and Preschool Age Part II: Cognitive, Neuropsychological and Behavioural Outcomes. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2013, 26, 1653–1657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sansavini, A.; Pentimonti, J.; Justice, L.; Guarini, A.; Savini, S.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldellac, G. Language, Motor and Cognitive Development of Extremely Preterm Children: Modeling Individual Growth Trajectories over the First Three Years of Life. J. Commun. Disord. 2014, 49, 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Twilhaar, E.S.; Wade, R.M.; de Kieviet, J.F.; van Goudoever, J.B.; van Elburg, R.M.; Oosterlaan, J. Cognitive Outcomes of Children Born Extremely or Very Preterm Since the 1990s and Associated Risk Factors: A Meta-Analysis and Meta-Regression. JAMA Pediatr. 2018, 172, 361–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Woodward, L.J.; Moor, S.; Hood, K.M.; Champion, P.R.; Foster-Cohen, S.; Inder, T.E.; Austin, N.C. Very Preterm Children Show Impairments across Multiple Neurodevelopmental Domains by Age 4 Years. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal 2009, 94, F339–F344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Rieck, M.; Arad, I.; Netzcer, D. Developmental Evaluation of Very-Low-Birthweight Infants: Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Studies. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1996, 19, 549–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W. Neurobehavioral Outcomes of School-Age Children Born Extremely Low Birth Weight or Very Preterm in the 1990s. JAMA 2003, 289, 3264–3272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Böhm, B.A.; Katz-Salamon, M.; Smedler, A.; Lagercrantz, H.; Forssberg, H. Developmental Risks and Protective Factors for Influencing Cognitive Outcome at 51⁄2 Years of Age in Very-Low-Birthweight Children. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2002, 44, 508–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bhutta, A.T.; Cleves, M.A.; Casey, P.H.; Cradock, M.M.; Anand, K.J. Cognitive and Behavioral Outcomes of School-Aged Children Who Were Born Preterm. JAMA 2002, 288, 728–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Feingold, C. Correlates of Cognitive Development in Low-Birth-Weight Infants from Low-Income Families. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 1994, 9, 91–97. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lind, A.; Korkman, M.; Lehtonen, L.; Lapinleimu, H.; Parkkola, R.; Matomäki, J.; Haataja, L.; The Pipari Study Group. Cognitive and Neuropsychological Outcomes at 5 Years of Age in Preterm Children Born in the 2000s. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2011, 53, 256–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sobaih, B.H. Long-Term Cognitive Outcome of Very Low Birth-Weight Saudi Preterm Infants at the Corrected Age of 24-36 Months. Saudi Med. J. 2018, 39, 368–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Yaari, M.; Mankuta, D.; Harel-Gadassi, A.; Friedlander, E.; Bar-Oz, B.; Eventov-Friedman, S.; Maniv, N.; Zucker, D.; Yirmiya, N. Early Developmental Trajectories of Preterm Infants. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2017, 81, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Caravale, B.; Tozzi, C.; Albino, G.; Vicari, S. Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Infants At3–4 Years of Life. Arch. Child. Fetal Neonatal 2005, 90, F474–F479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Cheong, J.L.; Doyle, L.W.; Burnett, A.C.; Lee, K.J.; Walsh, J.M.; Potter, C.R.; Treyvaud, K.; Thompson, D.K.; Olsen, J.E.; Anderson, P.J.; et al. Association Between Moderate and Late Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment and Social-Emotional Development at Age 2 Years. JAMA Pediatr. 2017, 171, e164805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Poulsen, G.; Wolke, D.; Kurinczuk, J.J.; Boyle, E.M.; Field, D.; Alfirevic, Z.; Quigley, M.A. Gestational Age and Cognitive Ability in Early Childhood: A Population-Based Cohort Study. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2013, 27, 371–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Talge, N.M.; Holzman, C.; Wang, J.; Lucia, V.; Gardiner, J.; Breslau, N. Late-Preterm Birth and Its Association With Cognitive and Socioemotional Outcomes at 6 Years of Age. Pediatrics 2010, 126, 1124–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Voigt, B.; Pietz, J.; Pauen, S.; Kliegel, M.; Reuner, G. Cognitive Development in Very vs. Moderately to Late Preterm and Full-Term Children: Can Effortful Control Account for Group Differences in Toddlerhood? Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Samra, H.A.; McGrath, J.M.; Wehbe, M. An Integrated Review of Developmental Outcomes and Late-Preterm Birth. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2011, 40, 399–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. McGowan, J.E.; Alderdice, F.A.; Doran, J.; Holmes, V.A.; Jenkins, J.; Craig, S.; Johnston, L. Impact of Neonatal Intensive Care on Late Preterm Infants: Developmental Outcomes at 3 Years. Pediatrics 2012, 130, e1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Odd, D.E.; Emond, A.; Whitelaw, A. Long-Term Cognitive Outcomes of Infants Born Moderately and Late Preterm. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2012, 54, 704–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Romeo, D.M.; Guzzardi, S.; Ricci, D.; Cilauro, S.; Brogna, C.; Cowan, F.; Romeo, M.G.; Mercuri, E. Longitudinal Cognitive Assessment in Healthy Late Preterm Infants. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2012, 16, 243–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Gurka, M.J.; LoCasale-Crouch, J.; Blackman, J.A. Long-Term Cognition, Achievement, Socioemotional, and Behavioral Development of Healthy Late-Preterm Infants. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2010, 164, 525–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Dall’Oglio, A.M.; Rossiello, B.; Coletti, M.F.; Bultrini, M.; De Marchis, C.; Rav, L.; Caselli, C.; Paris, S.; Cuttini, M. Do Healthy Preterm Children Need Neuropsychological Follow-up? Preschool Outcomes Compared with Term Peers. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2010, 52, 955–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Dueker, G.; Chen, J.; Cowling, C.; Haskind, B. Early Developmental Outcomes Predicted by Gestational Age From 35 to 41 Weeks. Obstet. Gynecol. Surv. 2017, 72, 211–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. van de Weijer-Bergsma, E.; Wijnroks, L.; Boom, J.; de Vries, L.S.; van Haastert, I.C.; Jongmans, M.J. Individual Differences in Developmental Trajectories of A-Not-B Performance in Infants Born Preterm. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010, 35, 605–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Weisglas-Kuperus, N.; Baerts, W.; Smrkovsky, M.; Sauer, P.J. Effects of Biological and Social Factors on the Cognitive Development of Very Low Birth Weight Children. Pediatrics 1993, 92, 658–665. [Google Scholar]
  45. Baron, I.S.; Weiss, B.A.; Baker, R.; Khoury, A.; Remsburg, I.; Thermolice, J.W.; Litman, F.R. Subtle Adverse Effects of Late Preterm Birth: A Cautionary Note. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Patra, K.; Greene, M.M. Health Care Utilization after NICU Discharge and Neurodevelopmental Outcome in the First 2 Years of Life in Preterm Infants. Am. J. Perinatol. 2018, 35, 441–447. [Google Scholar]
  47. Odd, D.E.; Rasmussen, F.; Gunnell, D.; Lewis, G.; Whitelaw, A. A Cohort Study of Low Apgar Scores and Cognitive Outcomes. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2008, 93, F115–F120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Razaz, N.; Boyce, W.T.; Brownell, M.; Jutte, D.; Tremlett, H.; Marrie, R.A.; Joseph, K.S. Five-Minute Apgar Score as a Marker for Developmental Vulnerability at 5 Years of Age. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2016, 101, F114–F120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Linsell, L.; Malour, R.; Morris, J.; Kurinczuk, J.; Marlow, N. Prognostic Factors for Poor Cognitive Development in Children Born Very Preterm or with Very Low Birth Weight a Systematic Review. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 1162–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Rozas, S.J.; Verburg, B.O.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Hofman, A.; Mackenbach, J.P.; Steegers, E.A.; Tiemeier, H. Effects of Maternal Smoking in Pregnancy on Prenatal Brain Development. The Generation R Study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007, 25, 611–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.; González, L.; Trisasc, J.L.; Casares, J.; Dominguez, M. Neurobehavioral Development of Preterm and Full-Term Children: Biological and Environmental Influences. Early Hum. Dev. 2013, 89, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Ylijoki, M.K.; Ekholm, E.; Ekblad, M.; Lehtonen, L. Prenatal Risk Factors for Adverse Developmental Outcome in Preterm Infants—Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Anthopolos, R.; Edwards, S.E.; Miranda, M.L. Effects of Maternal Prenatal Smoking and Birth Outcomes Extending into the Normal Range on Academic Performance in Fourth Grade in North Carolina, USA. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2013, 27, 564–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Lambe, M.; Hultman, C.; Torrang, A.; MacCabe, J.; Cnattingius, S. Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy and School Performance at Age 15. Epidemiology 2006, 17, 524–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Julvez, J.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Torrent, M.; Forns, M.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Sunyer, J. Maternal Smoking Habits and Cognitive Development of Children at Age 4 Years in a Population-Based Birth Cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 36, 825–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Gilman, S.; Gardener, H.; Buka, S. Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy and Children’s Cognitive and Physical Development: A Causal Risk Factor? Am. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 168, 522–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Falster, K.; Hanly, M.; Banks, E.; Lynch, J.; Chambers, G.; Brownell, M.; Eades, S.; Jorm, L. Maternal Age and Offspring Developmental Vulnerability at Age Five: A Population-Based Cohort Study of Australian Children. Plos Med. 2018, 15, e1002558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W. Cognitive and Educational Deficits in Children Born Extremely Preterm. Semin. Perinatol. 2008, 32, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Miceli, P.J.; Goeke-Morey, M.; Whitman, T.L.; Kolberg, K.S.; Miller-Loncar, C.; White, R.D. Brief Report: Birth Status, Medical Complications, and Social Environment: Individual Differences in Development of Preterm, Very Low Weight Infants. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2000, 25, 353–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Thompson, R.J.; Catlett, A.T.; Oehler, J.M.; Gustafson, K.E.; Goldstein, R.F.; Prochaska, J.J. Home Environment and Developmental Outcome of African American and White Infants With Very Low Birthweight. Child. Health Care 1998, 27, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Laucht, M.; Esser, G.; Schmidt, M.H. Developmental Outcome of Infants Born with Biological and Psychosocial Risks. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 843–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Beauregard, J.L.; Drews-Botsch, C.; Sales, J.M.; Flanders, W.D.; Kramer, M.R. Preterm Birth, Poverty, and Cognitive Development. Pediatrics 2018, 141, e20170509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Wong, H.S.; Edwards, P. Nature or Nurture: A Systematic Review of the Effect of Socio-Economic Status on the Developmental and Cognitive Outcomes of Children Born Preterm. Matern. Child. Health, J. 2013, 17, 1689–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Asztalos, E.; Church, P.T.; Riley, P.; Fajardo, C.; Shah, P.S. Association between Primary Caregiver Education and Cognitive and Language Development of Preterm Neonates. Am. J. Perinatol. 2017, 34, 364–371. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  65. Cserjesi, R.; van Braeckel, K.; Timmerman, M.; Butcher, P.R.; Kerstjen, J.; Reijveveld, S.A.; Bouma, A.; Bos, A.F.; Geuze, R.H. Patterns of Functioning and Predictive Factors in Children Born Moderately Preterm or at Term. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2012, 54, 710–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Patra, K.; Greene, M.; Patel, A.; Meier, P. Maternal Education Level Predicts Cognitive, Language, and Motor Outcome in Preterm Infants in the Second Year of Life. Am. J. Perinatol. 2016, 33, 738–744. [Google Scholar]
  67. Weisglas-Kuperus, N.; Hille, E.T.M.; Duivenvoorden, H.J.; Finken, M.J.J.; Wit, J.M.; van Buuren, S.; van Goudoever, J.B.; Verloove-Vanhorick, S.P. Intelligence of Very Preterm or Very Low Birthweight Infants in Young Adulthood. ADC Fetal Neonatal 2009, 94, F196–F200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Assel, M.A.; Landry, S.H.; Swank, P.R.; Steelman, L.; Miller-Loncar, C.; Smith, K.E. How Do Mothers’ Childrearing Histories, Stress and Parenting Affect Children’s Behavioural Outcomes? Child. Care Health Dev. 2002, 28, 359–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. de Jong, M.; Verhoeven, M.; Hooge, I.T.C.; Maingay-Visser, A.P.G.F.; Spanjerberg, L.; van Baar, A.L. Cognitive Functioning in Toddlerhood: The Role of Gestational Age Attention Capacities, and Maternal Stimulation. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 648–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Baron, I.S.; Erickson, K.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Litman, F.R.; Brandt, J. Spatial Location Memory Discriminates Children Born at Extremely Low Birth Weight and Late-Preterm at Age Three. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 787–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bacharach, V.R.; Baumeister, A.A. Effects of Maternal Intelligence, Marital Status, Income, and Home Environment on Cognitive Development of Low Birthweight Infants. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 1998, 23, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Piteo, A.M.; Yelland, L.N.; Makrides, M. Does Maternal Depression Predict Developmental Outcome in 18 Month Old Infants? Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 651–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. de la, P. García-Martínez, M.; Sánchez-Caravaca, J.; de P. Montealegre-Ramón, M.; Pérez-López, J. Predictive Value of the Bayley Scales Applied to a Group of Preterm Infants, on Their Results in the Wechsler Scales at 10 Years Old. An. Psicol. 2018, 35, 95–105. [Google Scholar]
  74. Gick-Fan, R.; Wetters-Portuguez, M.; Lahorgue-Nunes, M. Cognition, Behavior and Social Competence of Preterm Low Birth Weight Children at School Age. Clinics 2013, 68, 915–921. [Google Scholar]
  75. Potharst, E.S.; Houtzager, B.A.; van Sonderen, L.; Tamminga, P.; Kok, J.H.; Last, B.F.; van Wassenaer, A.G. Prediction of Cognitive Abilities at the Age of 5 Years Using Developmental Follow-up Assessments at the Age of 2 and 3 Years in Very Preterm Children. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2012, 54, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Tsu-Hsin, H.; Ching-Fan, S.; Yung-Wen, H.; Tien-Ni, W.; Lan-Wan, W. Predicting Neurodevelopmental Outcomes at Preschool Age for Children with Very Low Birth Weight. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 48, 231–241. [Google Scholar]
  77. Vicari, S.; Caravale, B.; Carlesimo, G.; Casadei, A.; Allemand, A.M. Spatial Working Memory Deficits in Children at Ages 3-4 Who Were Low Birth Weight, Preterm Infants. Neuropsychology 2004, 18, 673–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Newborg, J.; Stock, J.R.; Wnek, L. Battelle. Inventario de Desarrollo, 1st ed.; TEA ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  79. Caldwell, B.M.; Bradley, R.H. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment, Revised Edition; University of Arkansas at Little Rock: Little Rock, AR, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  80. Moreno, C.; Escala, H.O.M.E. y C.V.C. Cuestionario de la Vida Cotidiana.; University of Sevilla: Sevilla, Spain, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  81. Kessels, R.P.; van Zandvoort, M.J.; Postma, A.; Kappelle, L.J.; de Haan, E.H. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and Normative Data. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2000, 7, 252–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Farrell-Pagulayan, K.; Busch, R.M.; Medina, K.L.; Bartok, J.A.; Krikorian, R. Developmental Normative Data for the Corsi Block-Tapping Task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2006, 28, 1043–1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive data and comparisons among the 4 GA groups a.
Table 1. Descriptive data and comparisons among the 4 GA groups a.
Chi square ≤31
EPT&VPT
32–33
MPT
34–36
LPT
≥37
FT
χ2df
Gender
(frequency)
Female191432212.643
Male24222623
Maternal educational level
(frequency)
Basic8671212.446
High school16163715
University19141416
Stay in NICU
(frequency)
No stay143140120.84 ***6
1–15 days1322232
>15 days291041
ANOVA ≤31
EPT&VPT
32–33
MPT
34–36
LPT
≥37
FT
Fdf
Mother’s ageMean34.8834.7233.2431.984.23 **179
SD5.064.243.634.41
CigarettesMean0.770.671.161.050.249179
SD2.532.393.613.34
Apgar score 1st minuteMean7.148.338.318.149.73 ***179
SD1.580.950.991.20
HOME score at 22 monthsMean37.3039.3338.2438.701.64179
SD4.543.854.353.97
HOME score at 48 monthsMean48.1449.1249.4049.971.86144
SD3.353.873.482.46
Corsi score at 60 monthsMean10.509.2511.7810.640.540127
SD7.428.308.477.60
a Chi square in the upper part, and one-way ANOVA in the lower part. ** p value < 0.01, *** p value < 0.001.
Table 2. Descriptive results of cognitive development by gestational age groups.
Table 2. Descriptive results of cognitive development by gestational age groups.
GA in WeeksnMeanSDRange
BDI cognitive score at 22 months of age≤314426.552.9820–34
32–333626.833.2222–33
34–365826.713.7112–36
≥374327.584.0222–39
Total18126.903.5312–39
BDI cognitive score at 60 months of age≤313384.488.5451–93
32–333185.585.3473–95
34–364282.4015.6411–94
≥373385.913.1878–92
Total13984.4410.0111–95
Table 3. Linear Regression analysis: predictors of BDI cognitive scores at 22 months.
Table 3. Linear Regression analysis: predictors of BDI cognitive scores at 22 months.
Predictors (IV)Standardized βR2Change in R2Change in FF(df)
Model 1 0.1020.1023.147 **3.147 (6,166)
Gender0.225 **
GA in weeks0.129
Stay in NICU−0.038
Apgar score 1st minute0.085
Mother’s age0.197*
Cigarettes per day−0.140
Model 2 0.2130.11111.536 ***5.543 (2,164)
Gender0.188 **
GA in weeks0.144
Stay in NICU0.001
Apgar score 1st minute0.061
Mother’s age0.171 *
Cigarettes per day0.044
Mother’s educational level 0.045
HOME score at 22 months0.341 ***
* p value < 0.05, ** p value < 0.01, *** p value < 0.001.
Table 4. Linear Regression: predictors of BDI cognitive scores at 60 months.
Table 4. Linear Regression: predictors of BDI cognitive scores at 60 months.
Predictors (IV)Standardized βR2Change in R2Change in FF(df)
Model 1 0.0140.0140.2830.283 (6,119)
Gender−0.042
GA in weeks−0.029
Stay in NICU−0.082
Apgar score 1st minute0.064
Mother’s age−0.027
Cigarettes per day0.009
Model 2 0.0820.0684.311 *1.302 (8,117)
Gender−0.036
GA in weeks−0.010
Stay in NICU−0.028
Apgar score 1st minute0.061
Mother’s age0.014
Cigarettes per day0.002
Mother’s educational level 0.073
HOME score at 48 months0.241 *
Model 3 0.2320.16511.259 ***3.476 (10,115)
Gender0.038
GA in weeks−0.045
Stay in NICU−0.034
Apgar score 1st minute0.037
Mother’s age−0.077
Cigarettes per day0.040
Mother’s educational level0.116
HOME score at 48 months0.103
BDI score at 22 months0.334 ***
Corsi score at 60 months0.200*
* p value < 0.05, ** p value < 0.01, *** p value < 0.001.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, M.P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.L.; Martínez-López, Z.; Arce, C. A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2380. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17072380

AMA Style

Pérez-Pereira M, Fernández MP, Gómez-Taibo ML, Martínez-López Z, Arce C. A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(7):2380. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17072380

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pérez-Pereira, Miguel, María Pilar Fernández, María Luisa Gómez-Taibo, Zeltia Martínez-López, and Constantino Arce. 2020. "A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 7: 2380. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17072380

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop