Next Article in Journal
Retrospective Study: Lateral Ridge Augmentation Using Autogenous Dentin: Tooth-Shell Technique vs. Bone-Shell Technique
Previous Article in Journal
Air Pollution Status in 10 Mega-Cities in China during the Initial Phase of the COVID-19 Outbreak
Previous Article in Special Issue
Cognitive and Learning Outcomes in Late Preterm Infants at School Age: A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

Special Issue: Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes

by
Annalisa Guarini
1,*,
Miguel Pérez Pereira
2,
Anneloes van Baar
3 and
Alessandra Sansavini
1,*
1
Department of Psychology “Renzo Canestrari”, University of Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy
2
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, 15782 Santiago, Spain
3
Child and Adolescent Studies, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(6), 3169; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063169
Submission received: 7 March 2021 / Accepted: 16 March 2021 / Published: 19 March 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes)
As indicated by the World Health Organization, preterm birth is a relevant public health issue, being one of the leading causes of death in children under five years of age. Its incidence has been increasing in most countries, with inequalities in terms of survival rates and negative outcomes around the world [1,2]. Preterm birth is a critical event that affects not only child development, but also families and societies, particularly in the care and educational settings [3].
The articles included in this Special Issue will add new considerations on prematurity, focusing on: (a) the effect of preterm birth on developmental outcomes from early life to adulthood, taking into account risk and protective factors; (b) the role of parenting and social environment; and (c) interventions that can promote the well-being of preterm children and their families.
The first section of articles explores the effect of preterm birth on developmental outcomes from infancy to adulthood. Some articles reveal that preterm participants show significant differences with respect to normative values and control groups. A higher incidence of learning impairments in very preterm children (gestational age < 33 weeks), compared to normative values, is described at the end of primary school [4]. Delays in very preterm samples (<32 weeks and very low birth weight < 1500 g) also persist into adulthood [5]. Indeed, preterm adults show lower motor and IQ scores compared to full-term adults and adult motor outcomes are predicted by early motor difficulties [5]. A systematic review [6] describes persistent mild difficulties in executive function, short-term verbal memory, literacy, attention skills and processing speed, revealing suboptimal outcomes in late preterm children (34–36 weeks of gestation). In contrast, other studies suggest that the differences between preterm and full-term samples are not revealed by group comparisons but are instead detected by analysing the interplay between risk and protective factors. A first study finds that even if the preterm and full-term groups do not differ in parenting stress level and mind-mindedness (the caregivers’ representation of a child’s mental life), the mothers of preterm infants with higher stress show more non-attuned comments with their children [7]. In a second study, preterm (<37 weeks of gestation) and full-term late talkers do not differ in child language measures and parental speech input at 30 months [8]. However, child and parent risk and protective factors explain the interindividual variability in children’s spontaneous speech and reported language measures, which reveals a mutual influence between the late talkers’ speech and the quality of the parents’ speech input [8]. Another study finds that healthy preterm children do not show cognitive delay at 22 and 60 months, even when including children with different degrees of immaturity [9]. In contrast, cognitive development is moderated by biological factors and the quality of the home environment [9].
These studies confirm the need to overcome the notion of impaired versus intact domains suggesting that, as proposed for other populations with atypical development, scores in the normal range or the absence of differences between preterm and control groups do not necessarily provide evidence of typical developmental trajectories [10]. In addition, these studies highlight the importance of follow-up programs with standardised and repeated assessments after discharge, in agreement with the European Standards of Care for Newborn Health [11]. As suggested by the papers in this Special Issue, to detect early risks and minimize negative impacts, there is a need to include late preterm children in follow-up programs, as well as to extend standardised assessment beyond the first years of primary school.
A second section of the studies analyses the role of parenting and social environment in modulating the effect of preterm birth on developmental outcomes. Concerning parenting, a first study analyses the complex relationship among gestational age (32–41 weeks of gestation), attention capacities, and maternal sensitivity at 18 months and receptive and expressive language at 24 months, revealing an effect of gestational age on receptive language, both direct and indirect, through the child’s alerting attention, and a direct effect of maternal sensitivity on expressive language [12]. A second study analyses whether parenting behaviour at 18 months can mediate the association between birth status and child problems at six years [13]. The mediation of the parenting behaviour is not confirmed, as preterm birth predicts less limit-setting in mother–child interaction at 18 months and child attention problems at six years, while maternal structure at 18 months predicts fewer internalizing and externalizing problems in both preterm and full-term children [13]. Other studies investigate the role of the social environment on preterm delivery and child development. A strong association between maternal mental illness and preterm delivery is found in socioeconomically disadvantaged areas of inland Southern California [14]. In addition, older mothers, non-Hispanic Black mothers, and mothers with Medicaid or other insurance status have a higher risk of preterm delivery [14]. Another study describes a developmental disadvantage at 24 months in preterm infants born in Italy from migrant mothers, originating from less-developed countries (i.e., Pakistan, Côte d’Ivoire, Cameroon, Nigeria, Eritrea, Senegal, Ethiopia) and exclusively fed formula milk [15]. This study confirmed the benefit of human milk that should be the first choice for preterm newborns [1]. However, when this is impossible, an intact protein preterm formula, compared to an extensively hydrolysed infant formula, is associated with a shorter time to full enteral feeding and higher achieved feeding volume in preterm infants [16].
The studies grouped in this second section stress the importance of identifying the factors that may mediate the effect of preterm birth on developmental outcomes, opening up new research challenges on possible cascading effects in the preterm population [3]. In addition, they point out the need to increase targeted prevention among vulnerable populations, confirming that more knowledge is needed to understand how to prevent preterm birth as well as its negative consequences [1].
The last section includes two articles that describe interventions to support families and promote the child’s skills. Preterm birth has an important impact on parents and families, especially when it occurs before 28 weeks of gestational age. Indeed, in the first year at home, the parents of children born in Sweden report prolonged worries for the child and their own emotional state, and they appreciated a post-discharge home-visiting program to support parent–child interaction and encourage well-being for parents and children [17]. A second study analyses the effect of a parent dialogic book reading intervention in a sample of late talkers, revealing the efficacy of the intervention in improving expressive lexical and syntactic skills in both full-term and low-risk preterm children (<37 weeks of gestation) between two and three years of age [18]. Concerning low-risk preterm late talkers, the intervention impacts mainly on their emergent syntactic skills [18].
The promising results of both studies suggest the relevance of proposing short, ecological and cost-effective programs for preterm children and their families in order to enrich their environment, modify constraints, and increase protective factors [19].
In conclusion, this Special Issue confirms that an interdisciplinary approach is mandatory to understand the complexity of the risk factors affecting preterm birth and to promote protective factors in order to support the families and children’s developmental trajectories. Only by integrating the results of studies carried out in collaboration among different professional areas of expertise, including paediatricians, psychologists, therapists, educators and teachers, can we improve the care of preterm children by taking into account their family and educational context. Secondly, multiple assessment points, integrating standardised assessment and observation in ecological contexts and emphasizing longitudinal design and multimethod approaches are the best way to grasp the complexity of the phenomenon. Finally, this Special Issue addresses the need to support vulnerable populations, improve follow-up programmes, and develop new interventions to promote the well-being of preterm born children and their families.

Author Contributions

A.G. wrote the first draft of this editorial; M.P.P., A.v.B. and A.S. edited the consecutive versions. All authors have agreed on the final version. The overall Special Issue was conceptualized by A.G., M.P.P., A.v.B. and A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from (a) AlmaIdea 2017 Grant Senior, University of Bologna, “Ritardo di linguaggio nei bambini nati pretermine: screening, valutazione e intervento” (“Language delay in preterm children: Screening, assessment and intervention”) awarded to A.S. as the main Principal Investigator, and (b) the Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research MIUR PRIN 2017 (2017HRCPE4_004) “Early Markers of Language-Learning Impairment,” awarded to A.S., as the Principal Investigator of the Unit at the University of Bologna.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. March of Dimes; PMNPC; Save the Children, World Health Organization. Born Too Soon: The Global Action Report on Preterm Birth; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2012.
  2. Chawanpaiboon, S.; Vogel, J.P.; Moller, A.B.; Lumbiganon, P.; Petzold, M.; Hogan, D.; Landoulsi, S.; Jampathong, N.; Kongwattanakul, K.; Laopaiboon, M.; et al. Global, Regional, and National Estimates of Levels of Preterm Birth in 2014: A Systematic Review and Modelling Analysis. Lancet Glob. Health 2019, 7, e37–e46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Sansavini, A.; Guarini, A.; Caselli, M.C. Preterm Birth: Neuropsychological Profiles and Atypical Developmental Pathways. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2011, 17, 102–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bucci, S.; Bevilacqua, F.; De Marchis, C.; Coletti, M.F.; Gentile, S.; Dall’Oglio, A.M. Learning Abilities in a Population of Italian Healthy Preterm Children at the End of Primary School. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Baumann, N.; Tresilian, J.; Bartmann, P.; Wolke, D. Early Motor Trajectories Predict Motor but not Cognitive Function in Preterm- and Term-Born Adults without Pre-existing Neurological Conditions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Martínez-Nadal, S.; Bosch, L. Cognitive and Learning Outcomes in Late Preterm Infants at School Age: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Suttora, C.; Spinelli, M.; Aureli, T.; Fasolo, M.; Lionetti, F.; Picciolini, O.; Ravasi, M.; Salerni, N. Mind-Mindedness and Parenting Stress: A Cross-Sectional Study in a Cohort of Mothers of 3-Month-Old Full-Term and Preterm Infants. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Suttora, C.; Guarini, A.; Zuccarini, M.; Aceti, A.; Corvaglia, L.; Sansavini, A. Speech and Language Skills of Low-Risk Preterm and Full-Term Late Talkers: The Role of Child Factors and Parent Input. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, M.P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.L.; Martínez-López, Z.; Arce, C. A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Karmiloff-Smith, A. Nativism versus Neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the Study of Developmental Disorders. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 56–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. EFCNI. European Standards of Care for Newborn Health Project Report; Mader, S., Thiele, N., Walz, J.M., Eds.; EFCNI: Munchen, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  12. Snijders, V.E.; Bogicevic, L.; Verhoeven, M.; Van Baar, A.L. Toddlers’ Language Development: The Gradual Effect of Gestational Age, Attention Capacities, and Maternal Sensitivity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Oosterom, L.; Bogičević, L.; Verhoeven, M.; Van Baar, A.L. Parenting Behavior at 18 Months Predicts Internalizing and Externalizing Problems at 6 Years in Moderately Preterm and Full Term Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lopez, A.; Becerra, M.B.; Becerra, B.J. Maternal Mental Illness Is Associated with Adverse Neonate Outcomes: An Analysis of Inpatient Data. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Gibertoni, D.; Sansavini, A.; Savini, S.; Locatelli, C.; Ancora, G.; Perrone, E.; Ialonardi, M.; Rucci, P.; Fantini, M.P.; Faldella, G.; et al. Neurodevelopmental Trajectories of Preterm Infants of Italian Native-Born and Migrant Mothers and Role of Neonatal Feeding. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Baldassarre, M.E.; Di Mauro, A.; Fanelli, M.; Capozza, M.; Wampler, J.L.; Cooper, T.; Laforgia, N. Shorter Time to Full Preterm Feeding Using Intact Protein Formula: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Baraldi, E.; Allodi, M.W.; Smedler, A.-C.; Westrup, B.; Löwing, K.; Ådén, U. Parents’ Experiences of the First Year at Home with an Infant Born Extremely Preterm with and without Post-Discharge Intervention: Ambivalence, Loneliness, and Relationship Impact. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Zuccarini, M.; Suttora, C.; Bello, A.; Aceti, A.; Corvaglia, L.; Caselli, M.C.; Guarini, A.; Sansavini, A. A Parent-Implemented Language Intervention for Late Talkers: An Exploratory Study on Low-Risk Preterm and Full-Term Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Westermann, G.; Mareschal, D.; Johnson, M.H.; Sirois, S.; Spratling, M.W.; Thomas, M.S. Neuroconstructivism. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Guarini, A.; Pereira, M.P.; van Baar, A.; Sansavini, A. Special Issue: Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3169. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063169

AMA Style

Guarini A, Pereira MP, van Baar A, Sansavini A. Special Issue: Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(6):3169. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063169

Chicago/Turabian Style

Guarini, Annalisa, Miguel Pérez Pereira, Anneloes van Baar, and Alessandra Sansavini. 2021. "Special Issue: Preterm Birth: Research, Intervention and Developmental Outcomes" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 6: 3169. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063169

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop