Next Article in Journal
First Detection of Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus 2 on the Surfaces of Tourist-Recreational Facilities in Italy
Next Article in Special Issue
Pathways Improving Compliance with Preventive Behaviors during the Remission Period of the COVID-19 Pandemic
Previous Article in Journal
The Determinants of Panic Buying during COVID-19
Previous Article in Special Issue
COVID-19 and Health-Related Quality of Life: A Community-Based Online Survey in Hong Kong
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Vulnerable People Suffering from Depression: Two Studies on Adults in France

1
CNRS, LAPSCO, Université Clermont-Auvergne, F-6300 Clermont-Ferrand, France
2
CNRS, CeRCA, Université de Poitiers, F-86000 Poitiers, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(6), 3250; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063250
Submission received: 1 March 2021 / Revised: 15 March 2021 / Accepted: 17 March 2021 / Published: 21 March 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Mental Health in the Time of COVID-19)

Abstract

:
This study investigated the difficulties experienced by people suffering from depression in coping with the stressful context of the COVID-19 pandemic and the lockdown. Two large samples of the French population were classified on the basis of their depressive symptoms and completed an online questionnaire on their emotions and their behaviors during the lockdown. Results showed that, compared to participants with no or mild mental health-related symptoms, participants with moderate to severe depressive symptoms suffered from greater psychological effects of the pandemic and the lockdown (fear, anxiety, sadness, sleep quality, loss of daily routine). However, health risk behaviors (smoking, drinking, non-compliance with lockdown and barrier gestures) and perceived vulnerability did not differ between the participant groups, although more severely depressed participants tended to be less respectful of health guidelines. In addition, the most heightened effects on the depressed participants were boredom and the feeling of social isolation, which was not compensated by the search for social affiliation. Supporting people with depression should be a public health priority because they suffer psychologically more than others from the pandemic and the lockdown.

1. Introduction

Depression has been declared a public health priority. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), depressive disorders are the leading cause of the global burden of disease and the associated economic burden [1]. A growing number of studies conducted worldwide suggest that the COVID-19 pandemic and its management—lockdown measures—have resulted in increased levels of depression in the general population [2,3,4]. However, they have not examined how people suffering from depression have coped with this pandemic and the lockdown. This vulnerable population may indeed require specific public health interventions for mental health. It is essential to maintain their wellbeing during the COVID-19 period in order to prevent the chronicity of depressive symptoms and their aggravation.
Studies that provide statistical data on the psychological effects of the COVID-19 pandemic indicate an increase in psychological distress (stress, fear, anxiety) [5,6,7]. These negative emotions are frequent in people exposed to an infectious disease [8]. A recent review of previous studies on nationally imposed quarantines for various pandemics (SARS, Ebola, H1N1 influenza) reminds us of their negative psychological consequences. Quarantine measures may even cause symptoms of post-traumatic stress when they are of long duration (>10 days) [9]. During the COVID-19 lockdown, people also reported feeling very bored and found that time slowed down [5,6,7,8,9,10]. Moreover, a significant proportion of people engaged in health risk behaviors, such as drinking alcohol [11,12].
It is well established that some of these psychological problems are habitual in depressed people. Depressed people are more anxious as anxiety is closely linked to depression [13]. They feel overwhelmed by sadness, by a loss of energy and decreased interest in activities. They suffer more from social isolation [14] and insomnia [15]. As a result, the COVID-19 pandemic and the lockdown measures may have greatly amplified depressed people’s initial difficulties. This might have made life during lockdown unbearable for them compared to those with little or no depression. They may also have been at risk of developing health risk behaviors such as the use of tobacco, alcohol and/or cannabis. In a recent study conducted in Australia, participants with moderate depressive symptoms reported suffering from sleep disorders and higher alcohol consumption [16]. It is also possible that depressed people might have been willing to take more risks by not closely following lockdown measures (e.g., leaving home more frequently) or by ignoring preventive measures (i.e., reduced observance of social physical distancing). However, this seems to depend on their level of fear or anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic. Conversely, if their level of fear and anxiety is very high, they may also be more likely to comply with lockdown and preventive measures.
The aim of this study was to examine how people with moderate and severe depressive symptoms experienced the lockdown situation (e.g., feelings of isolation, emotion, sleep, substance consumption) and complied with the lockdown and preventive measures, as compared to people with no or fewer depressive symptoms. In France, the rules of lockdown were strict with only one hour of authorized outings (food shopping, individual physical exercise, pet needs, travel permits for professional activity that could not be carried out by teleworking) with a certificate of exit and police controls. However, the majority of workers kept their salary to stay at home during the lockdown. A survey was therefore conducted with French people (more than 2000 participants) in these lockdown conditions.

2. Study 1

2.1. Materials and Methods

2.1.1. Participants

During the lockdown, a total of 1175 French participants (889 women and 286 men) completed an online questionnaire. There were 124 additional participants (9.55%) who started the survey, but did not complete the depression scale (BDI) used in our study. The participants gave their informed consent by agreeing to complete the questionnaire. This survey was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki ethical principles and was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University Clermont Auvergne (IRB00011540-2020-31). Participants’ anonymity was guaranteed and they were allowed to stop answering the questionnaire at any time.

2.1.2. Procedure

The survey was created with the LimeSurvey software. It was distributed on online social networks during the French lockdown period from 1 to 29 April 2020. The survey consisted of a large set of questions and self-reported scales and took 40 minutes to complete. Here, we only report the results for the questions and scales of interest. We focused on reported emotion (boredom, anxiety, happiness, fear, anger, arousal), sleep quality, regularity of the rhythm of life and product consumption (alcohol, sleeping pills and anxiolytics, coffee/tea, cigarette and electronic cigarettes, stimulants, other substances) before and during the lockdown. The participants responded on a 7-point scale for most of the questions, and a 4-point scale for the consumption behaviors. There was also a series of questions with an 8-point response scale on self-reported compliance with the lockdown and respect for preventive measures (lockdown compliance and barrier gestures), perceived vulnerability (probability of being affected by the virus) and affiliation behaviors (i.e., contact with others via telephone, internet and social networks). For these last questions (compliance and affiliation), we used the average of the participants’ answers to the corresponding questions. The scales used in this study were the BDI (Beck Depression Inventory) for the assessment of depressive symptoms [17] and the 10-item short version of the UCLA loneliness scale for rating feelings of social isolation [18]. Their reliability was satisfactory (α = 0.82; α = 0.84, respectively).

2.1.3. Statistical Analyses

We used IBM SPSS-25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) for our statistical analyses. We ran a series of ANOVAs with repeated measures with the depression group as between-subjects factor (depression) and the considered period (before vs. during the lockdown) as within-subject factor (lockdown). A Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied when the sphericity assumption was violated. When a significant depression effect or depression x lockdown interaction was found, Student t tests were performed for two-way comparisons, with the p-values adjusted after the false discovery rates (FDR) correction (i.e., q-value) had been performed using the Benjamini–Hochberg approach [19]. The statistical analyses showed numerous significant results with p-values < 0.05, but with small effect sizes. We therefore considered the significant results only when the corresponding effect size was medium or large (Cohen’s d > 0.40 and partial eta-squared (η2p) > 0.06).

2.2. Results

Figure 1 shows the percentage of participants according to their depressive symptoms (BDI scale); 50.2% of participants were non-depressive (N = 590), 26.7% (N = 314) presented mild depressive symptoms and 23.1% (271) showed moderate or severe depressive symptoms (18.6% (219) and 4.4% (52), respectively). As reported in Table 1, the groups did not differ in education level (p > 0.05) (Mno = 14.85, SD = 3.11; Mmild = 15.01, SD = 3.06; Mmod-sev = 14.62, SD = 2.63) or in their living conditions during lockdown, either in terms of living space (assessed in square meters) (Mno = 108.15, SD = 57.39; Mmild = 104.33, SD = 57.2; Mmod-sev = 97.82, SD = 60.53) or the number of people sharing the living space (including the participant) (Mno = 2.71, SD = 1.27; Mmild = 2.67, SD = 1.24; Mmod-sev = 2.79, SD = 1.30, all p > 0.05). However, the older the participants were, the less severe their depression tended to be (Mno = 45.68, SD = 15.67; Mmild = 40.31, SD = 15.81; Mmod-sev = 32.98, SD = 13.30; B = −0.099, β = −0.312, t = −11.67, p < 0.0001, 95% CI [−0.116; −0.082]).
Our results (Table 1) indicate that the participants with depressive symptoms tended to report having complied with the lockdown rules and the preventive measures, just like the other participants (F(2, 1130) = 2.52, p = 0.11; F(2, 1114) = 14.32, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.025, respectively). Nevertheless, people with more severe signs of depression tended to be less compliant with preventive measures compared to the non-depressive group, Cohen’s d = 0.395, q < 0.05 (Table 2). The perception of vulnerability to COVID-19 was almost identical across groups, F(2, 1172) = 3.71, p = 0.025, η2p = 0.006.
The major difference between the depression groups during the lockdown lay in a stronger feeling of social isolation (loneliness scale, F(2, 1116) = 156.33, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.22) (Figure 2). The feeling of social isolation increased with the severity of the depressive symptoms, B = 0.501, ES = 0.026, β = 0.503, t = 19.46, p < 0.0001, 95% CI [0.451; 0.552].
For all participants, affiliation needs increased significantly during the lockdown as compared to before, F(1, 1123) = 131.68, p < 0.0001, η2p =.11. There was nevertheless little evidence that depressed participants sought more contact through social media than the other participants, F(2, 1123) = 10.74, p < 0.0001, η2p =.019, especially during the lockdown period, as suggested by the non-significant interaction between depression and lockdown, F(2, 1123) = 0.22, p = 0.81.
Finally, our statistical analyses showed a large effect of depression on several emotional feelings (Figure 3); i.e., anxiety, F(2, 1172) = 158.76, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.21; boredom, F(2, 1172) = 97.92, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.14; low-arousal, F(2, 1172) = 76.49, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.115; and happiness, F(2, 1172) = 203.39, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.258 (Table 1). Depression also affected the emotions of fear and anger, but to a lesser extent (F(2, 1172) = 58.08, p < 0.0001, η2p =.09; F(2, 1172) = 60.05, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.09, respectively). In addition, the feelings of boredom, F(1, 1172) = 388.78, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.249, fear F(1, 1172) = 411.16, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.26, and of sadness (lower scores on happiness scale), F(1, 1172) = 275.12, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.19, increased during lockdown (compared to before) for all participants (Figure 3). The other feelings, i.e., anxiety and anger, also increased, but only moderately, F(1, 1172) = 79.495, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.064, F(1, 1172) = 104.32, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.08. No difference was observed for low-arousal, F(1, 1172) = 1.25, p = 0.27. The effect size of the interaction between depression and lockdown (all η2p < 0.04) suggests little difference between the depression groups in terms of the emotions felt during lockdown compared to before, except in the case of boredom, F(2, 1172) = 33.24, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.05. The increase in the boredom felt during compared to before lockdown (difference index) was greater in the participants with more severe depressive symptoms than in their counterparts, both those with mild depression (Cohen’s d = 0.329, q < 0.05) and those with no depressive symptoms (Cohen’s d = 0.597, q < 0.05), with the latter two groups of participants differing slightly in their levels of boredom (Cohen’s d = 0.25, q = 0.05). Indeed, the increase in boredom during lockdown was greater the higher the level of depression reported by the participants (B = 0.644, ES = 0.077, β = 0.237, t = 8.37, p < 0.0001, 95% CI [0.493; 0.795]. In short, the lockdown had the same emotional effects on all the participants, although it caused more boredom in those who were depressed. However, because the more depressed participants usually felt negative effects (fear, happiness, anxiety, anger) more strongly (compared with their less depressed counterparts), their reported emotions reached a critical level during the lockdown.
The participants also reported that lockdown disrupted the regularity of their life rhythm, F(1, 1172) = 188.83, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.14, and affected their sleep, albeit moderately, F(1, 1172) = 91.38, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.07. Furthermore, our results showed that depression itself disrupted the rhythm of life and the quality of sleep (F(2, 1172) = 49.29, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.08, F(2, 1172) = 54.41, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.09). Consequently, the lockdown increased problems related to daily rhythm and sleep in participants with depressive symptoms, although few differences were observed between the depression groups as a function of the periods considered, i.e., before and during lockdown (depression x lockdown interaction, F(2, 1172) = 21.68, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.036; F(2, 1172) = 20.48, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.034). Indeed, although all the comparisons between groups were significant, the only noteworthy differences were between the participants without and those with moderate to severe depression for the before/during lockdown difference index for both life rhythm (0.36 vs. 1.28, Cohen’s d = 0.48, q < 0.05) and sleep (0.09 vs. 0.78, Cohen’s d = 0.47, q < 0.05) (Table 2). It thus appears that quality of sleep and life rhythm was more disturbed in the depressed people during lockdown (Figure 4).
The reported consumption of sleeping pills and anxiolytics, F(1, 1172) = 7.17, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.006, cigarettes, F(1, 1172) = 25.89, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.02, coffee/tea, F(1, 1172) = 50.83, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.042, stimulants, F(1, 1172) = 19.84, p = 0.0001, η2p = 0.017, and other substances, F(1, 1171) = 29.98, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.025, varied little between the periods before and during the lockdown. The consumption of other substances such as cannabis even tended to decrease (.17 vs. 11). Individuals only reported drinking more alcohol during the lockdown, F(1, 1172) = 116.17, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.09. Furthermore, there was little or no difference in self-reported consumption across the groups, with depressed participants not drinking more alcohol than the other groups (depression, F(2, 1172) = 0.84, p = 0.43; depression x lockdown, F(2, 1172) = 2.57, p = 0.08). The only exception was found in the consumption of other substances such as cannabis (depression, F(2, 1171) = 19.35, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.09; depression x lockdown, F(2, 1172) = 3.99, p = 0.02, η2p = 0.007), with a modest difference between the participants without and with moderate to severe signs of depression (Cohen’s d = 0.47, q < 0.05, Table 2).

3. Study 2

The aim of Study 2 was to replicate and extend the results of Study 1 with a new sample of participants. Indeed, the first sample, contacted via our social networks, was not representative of the population as a majority of women (76%) responded to the survey. Therefore, we recruited a new and more balanced sample by using a survey company (EasyPanel). This survey started three weeks after the first one on 24 April and ended on 28 April 2020.

3.1. Methods

The procedure and statistical analyses were similar to those used in Study 1. Unlike Study 1, however, Study 2 involved an equal proportion of men and women. The sample was composed of 1039 new participants: 526 women and 513 men. There were 156 missing values (13.05%) due to participants who did not complete the survey. All participants gave informed consent after reading the ethics form approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Clermont-Auvergne, France (IRB00011540-2020-31).

3.2. Results

The BDI scores showed that 53% of participants did not show signs of depression (N = 551, 258 women, 293 men), against 23.9% who presented signs of mild depression (N = 247, 138 women, 109 men) and 23.2% with signs of moderate or severe depression (N = 241, 130 women, 111 men) (Figure 1). The depression groups did not differ in their education level (p > 0.05) (Mno = 13.26, SD = 2.95; Mmild = 12.91, SD = 2.76; Mmod-sev = 12.88, SD = 2.85), and the number of people living with them during lockdown (p > 0.05) (Mno = 2.64, SD = 1.21; Mmild = 2.64, SD = 1.21; Mmod-sev = 2.50, SD = 1.23) (Table 3). However, the living space (expressed in sq. meters) tended to be smaller in those presenting more severe depressive disorders (Mno = 102.10, SD = 48.55; Mmild = 95.83, SD = 46.12; Mmod-sev = 90.29, SD = 43.86; B = −0.013, ES = 0.004, β = −0.105, t = −3.41, p = 0.001, 95% CI [−0.02; −0.005]). However, only a small difference was observed between the no-depression and the moderate-severe depression group (Cohen’s d = 0.25, q < 0.05). The younger the participants were, the more severe their depressive symptoms were likely to be (B = −0.03, ES = 0.12, β = −0.079, t = −2.56, p = 0.01, 95% CI [−0.05; −0.007]). However, the age difference between the no-depression and the moderate-severe group also remained very small (Cohen’s d = 0.22, q = 0.05).
The results (Table 3) are entirely consistent with the findings of Study 1. There were few differences in compliance with lockdown and barrier gestures between groups, as indicated by the small effect size of our significant results (F(2, 1036) = 8.55, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.016; F(2, 1036) = 18.99, p = 0.0001, η2p = 0.035, respectively). The depressive group did not report feeling more vulnerable to the virus than the other groups of participants, F(2, 1036) = 21.33, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.04. The major difference between groups was the feeling of social isolation during the lockdown, F(2, 1036) = 153.24, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.23 (Figure 2). The more depressed the participants were, the more socially isolated they felt (B = 0.556, ES = 0.029, β = 0.509, t = 19.05, p < 0.0001, 95% CI [0.498; 0.613]). This feeling of isolation did not change with participants’ sex (p > 0.05).
The affiliation behaviors changed with depression (Mno = 0.98, SD = 0.83; Mmild = 1.04; SD = 0.81, Mmod-sev. = 1.32, SD = 1.24, F(2, 1036) = 11.05, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.02, and with lockdown (Mbefore = 1.11, SD = 1.01; Mlock = 1.04, SD = 0.94, F(1, 1036) = 14.88, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.014), but the interaction between depression and lockdown was not significant (p > 0.05). We also found a non-significant effect for the sex variable (p > 0.05). However, the effect size for these significant main factors (depression, lockdown) remained small, suggesting that out-of-home contacts were limited each day, but with more contacts for the participants with moderate to severe depression than for the other participants (Cohen’s d(Mno, Mmod-sev) = 0.35; d(Mmild, Mmod-sev) = 0.27, both q < 0.05).
With regard to the emotions felt during compared to before the lockdown, the participants felt more bored (Figure 3) and less happy (F(1, 1036) = 322.81, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.24; F(1, 1036) = 425.36, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.29). Their levels of fear, anxiety and anger also dramatically increased with the experience of lockdown (F(1, 1036) = 579.29, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.36; F(1, 1036) = 195.62, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.16; F(1, 1036) = 183.37, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.15). The level of arousal also decreased, albeit to a lesser extent, F(1, 1036) = 72.81, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.06. In addition, with the exception of anger, which was characterized by a small effect, F(2, 1036) = 54.05, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.09, all these negative emotions were heightened by depression (boredom, F(2, 1036) = 62.80, η2p = 0.11; happiness, F(2, 1036) = 120.45, η2p = 0.19; fear, F(2, 1036) = 55.122, η2p = 0.10; anxiety, F(2, 1036) = 87.42, η2p = 0.14; all p < 0.0001). Consequently, the level of these emotions was particularly high during lockdown among those suffering from depression. However, as in Study 1, the effect size of the interaction between depression and lockdown remained small, although significant, for each emotion (all p < 0.01 and η2p < 0.03).
The lockdown also disrupted the regularity of the rhythm of life and of sleep in participants (F(1, 1036) = 185.26, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.15; F(1, 1036) = 122.39, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.11). In addition, the participants suffering from depression usually experienced more problems with daily rhythm and sleep (F(2, 1036) = 28.89, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.06; F(2, 1036) = 56.99, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.10). Indeed, the participants with the highest level of depression had a more irregular rhythm of life and slept less well than the other participants, both those with no depression (Cohen’s d = 0.58 and 0.78, both q < 0.05, Table 2) and those with mild depression (Cohen’s d = 0.38 and 0.38, both q < 0.05, Table 2). The mild depression sample also reported suffering from these problems (sleep and life rhythm) compared to the non-depressive participants. However, the difference only reached significance for sleep (Cohen’s d = 0.457, q < 0.05) but not for life rhythm (Cohen’s d = 0.21, q = 0.05, Table 2). Consequently, when compared to their initial status, rhythm and sleep problems during lockdown were particularly high in the depressed groups. However, this only resulted in a small effect size at the level of the depression x lockdown interaction (F(2, 1036) = 10.69, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.02; F(2, 1036) = 13.89, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.03).
As in Study 1, participants reported little or no lockdown-related change in their consumption of sleeping pills and anxiolytics, F(1, 1036) = 8.68, p = 0.003, η2p = 0.008, cigarettes, F(1, 1036) = 0.26, p = 0.61, coffee/tea, F(1, 1036) = 2.39, p = 0.12, stimulants, F(1, 1036) = 1.25, p = 0.26, and other substances, F(1, 1036) = 2.09, p = 0.15, as well as of alcohol, F(1, 1036) = 21.24, p = 0.0001, η2p = 0.02. The depression x lockdown interaction was not significant for any of these consumed products (all p > 0.05). Only the consumption of sleeping pills and anxiolytics changed across the depression groups, F(2, 1036) = 35.76, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.07. As might be expected, their consumption increased with the severity of the depressive symptoms (B = 0.034, ES = 0.003, β = 0.291, t = 9.79, p < 0.0001, 95% CI [0.027; 0.04]).

4. Discussion

Our two studies show a prevalence of depression of about 23% of people with moderate and severe depressive symptoms, and of 27% with mild depressive symptoms. Data on the prevalence of depression in France are quite rare and vary between studies according to the criteria applied and the methods used to assess depression [20,21]. The prevalence rate is close to 8%–10%, a number that corresponds to the average global rate [22]. Therefore, in France, as in other countries, the number of depressive episodes experienced by participants increased during the lockdown period [2,3,4].
In our research we carried out two surveys, each on a large sample of participants, which provided entirely consistent results. These results revealed no or only a slight difference in demographic factors (age, education, living space) between the participants in the different depression groups. As several studies have suggested, demographic factors have little influence on the psychological effects related to a pandemic [8]. However, a study conducted in Turkey found that people living in urban areas are at higher risk of depression [23], potentially owing to differences in the quality of living conditions between the examined populations. In our research, the participants in the moderate or severe depression groups tended to be younger than those in the other groups. Depression scores are known to reach their lowest level at about 40–50 years of age [24], with higher economic levels in middle age being one of the factors that protect from depression. Our results also showed that a large majority of people abode by the lockdown and took preventive measures, even though those with more severe depressive symptoms tended to be less respectful of health guidelines. As a result, few participants, even those suffering from mental illness, were concerned about becoming infected with COVID-19. This finding was observed in an older study on the quarantine imposed during the H1N1 influenza pandemic in 2009 [25].
Provided responses to our survey were not influenced by social desirability, our results contradict the cliché that the French population is undisciplined and does not respect preventive rules. As in many other nations in the world, the French population appeared to be relatively afraid of the COVID-19 pandemic and stayed home. Our results on emotional factors indeed showed a significant increase in negative self-reported emotions in all participants during the lockdown period as compared to before (anxiety, fear, happiness, anger). This adds to the findings of studies indicating a high level of fear and stress felt in healthy populations during the COVID-19 pandemic, combined with a decrease in people’s level of happiness [5]. Nevertheless, our study indicated that the magnitude of the increase in these negative emotions was not greater for the participants with (vs. without) depression. However, because these negative emotions were already at a higher level (before lockdown) in the participants with moderate or severe depressive symptoms, they reached a particularly high emotional level with a mean of about 5 on a 6-point anxiety and fear scales. Self-reported levels of fear and anxiety in people with the most severe depression symptoms are particularly worrying.
Unlike in a survey conducted in Australia [16], this emotional context caused by COVID-19 did not result in higher self-reported health risk behaviors in our study. We noticed only a slight increase in reported alcohol consumption in Study 1, a finding not verified in Study 2. This is consistent with a recent Spanish study showing that alcohol consumption decreased as the lockdown continued [26]. The use of other substances such as cannabis even tended to decrease in our study in the first sample of participants. This can be explained by the fact that lockdown limits opportunities to buy and use prohibited substances, or that exposure to other family members further decreases consumption. Our statistical analyses only indicated that the participants with more severe depressive symptoms claimed to have a lower quality of sleep coupled with a more irregular daily routine during the lockdown than the other participants. This is consistent with studies showing more insomnia in depressed people [15]. In our study, however, sleeping problems in particular, increased during lockdown for all participants, including those with depression who already had a low quality of sleep.
In sum, the negative psychological effects of the lockdown in the context of COVID-19 were similar in all groups of participants, with or without depression, but these effects were nevertheless more severe in the participants with depression because of their vulnerability, i.e., their previous high level of psychological problems. Finally, our statistical analyses only found a significant interaction between depression and lockdown (before vs. after) factors for one emotional dimension, that is, boredom. The participants in the moderate/severe-depression group were more bored than those in the mild and the no-depression group. Their feeling of boredom was exacerbated during the lockdown as compared to the other groups. Indeed, their boredom scores were twice as high as those of non-depressed people. This was likely related to the feeling of social isolation. Indeed, in our research, the feeling of social isolation was very high in the participants with moderate or severe depressive symptoms compared to the others. In addition, our research suggests that these participants did not compensate for this sense of social isolation by increasing telephone or social network contacts. Affiliation behaviors increased during compared to before the lockdown, but not by much more in the depressed participants. This is likely due to the fact that individuals with depression usually have fewer social contacts [14]. As noted in recent COVID studies, during the lockdown, individuals had little physical activity and spent a lot of time alone in front a screen, which contributed to poor mental health [27,28].
One limitation of the present work was the classification of participants into different depression groups based of their scores on a self-reported scale (BDI), and not on advanced clinical diagnosis. Our results should therefore be taken with caution. They may reflect mental health problems rather than actual psychiatric illnesses. In addition, a depressive episode during the lockdown may not result in psychiatric illness. Therefore, it would be important to contact people to monitor their mental health problems. In addition, although participants described their behaviors prior to the lockdown, we do not have their depression score prior to this period (baseline). Therefore, among the participants tested there are likely people who have been depressed for a long time and people who have recently been depressed (contextual). In all cases, the pandemic and the lockdown caused intense psychological distress that can lead to psychiatric illness, especially if other periods of lockdown are imposed in response to successive waves of the pandemic, with an accumulation of stressful events (job loss, financial difficulties, illness, etc.).

5. Conclusions

Our studies using a questionnaire completed by two large samples of French participants confirmed the numerous negative psychological effects of the COVID-19 pandemic and of lockdown on the general population, and more particularly on depressed people. Among the most exacerbated negative effects observed in the depressed groups were boredom and the feeling of social isolation during lockdown. Supporting depressed people during the COVID-19 pandemic period should therefore be a public health priority because they suffer psychologically more than others from the pandemic and its management by health authorities.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization of the survey, N.M., S.G., C.B., P.H., S.D.-V.; Secured funding, P.H. and S.D.-V.; Statistical analyses, N.M., J.C. and S.D.-V.; Writing—original draft preparation, N.M. and S.D.-V.; Writing—review and editing, N.M., S.G., J.C., C.B., G.D., P.H., S.D.-V. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The French Research Agency (Agence Nationale de la Recherche—ANR) supported this work under grant ANR-Flash-COVID19.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University Clermont Auvergne (IRB00011540-2020-31).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in these two studies are available on request from N.M. and S.D.-V. The data are not publicly available now because they are part of a larger project still in progress.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Worth Health Organization. The World Health Report 2001, Mental Health: New Understanding, New Hope; WHO library: Geneva, Switzerland, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  2. Salari, N.; Hosseinian-Far, A.; Jalali, R.; Vaisi-Raygani, A.; Rasoulpoor, S.; Mohammadi, M.; Rasoulpoor, S.; Khaledi-Paveh, B. Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Glob. Health 2020, 16, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bueno-Notivol, J.; Gracia-García, P.; Olaya, B.; Lasheras, I.; López-Antón, R.; Santabárbara, J. Prevalence of depression during the COVID-19 outbreak: A meta-analysis of community-based studies. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2020, 21, 100196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. O’ Connor, R.; Wetherall, K.; Cleare, S.; McClelland, H.; Melson, A.; Niedzwiedz, C.; O’Carroll, R.; O’Connor, D.; Platt, S.; Scowcroft, E.; et al. Mental health and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: Longitudinal analyses of adults in the UK. Br. J. Psychiatry 2020, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Droit-Volet, S.; Gil, S.; Martinelli, N.; Andant, N.; Clinchamps, M.; Parreira, L.; Rouffiac, K.; Dambrun, M.; Huguet, P.; Dubuis, B.; et al. Time and Covid-19 stress in the lockdown situation: Time Free, Dying of Boredom and Sadness. PLoS ONE 2020, 5, e0236465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; McIntyre, R.; Choo, F.; Tran, B.; Ho, R.; Sharma, V.; et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav. Immun. 2020, 87, 40–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Mazza, C.; Ricci, E.; Biondi, S.; Colasanti, M.; Ferracuti, S.; Napoli, C.; Roma, P. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among italian people during the COVID-19 pandemic: Immediate psychological responses and associated factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Brooks, S.; Webster, R.; Smith, L.; Woodland, L.; Wessely, S.; Greenberg, N.; Rubin, G.J. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 2020, 395, 912–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Hawryluck, L.; Gold, W.L.; Robinson, S.; Pogorski, S.; Galea, S.; Styra, R. SARS control and psychological effects of quarantine, Toronto. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 2005, 11, 354–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ogden, R.S. The passage of time during the UK Covid-19 lockdown. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0235871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Rehm, J.; Kilian, C.; Ferreira-Borges, C.; Jernigan, D.; Monteiro, M.; Parry, C.D.H.; Sanchez, Z.M.; Manthey, J. Alcohol use in times of the COVID 19: Implications for monitoring and policy. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2020, 39, 301–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kim, J.U.; Majid, A.; Judge, R.; Crook, P.; Nathwani, R.; Selvapatt, N.; Lovendoski, J.; Manousou, P.; Thursz, M.; Dhar, A.; et al. Effect of COVID-19 lockdown on alcohol consumption in patients with pre-existing alcohol use disorder. Lancet 2020, 5, 886–887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ballenger, J.C.; Davidson, J.R.T.; Lecrubier, Y.; Nutt, D.J.; Borkovec, T.D.; Rickels, K.; Stein, D.J.; Wittchen, H.U. Consensus statement on generalized anxiety disorder from the International Consensus Group on Depression and Anxiety. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2001, 62, 53–58. [Google Scholar]
  14. Santini, Z.I.; Koyanagi, A.; Tyrovolas, S.; Mason, C.; Haro, J.M. The association between social relationships and depression: A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 175, 53–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sivertsen, B.; Salo, P.; Mykletun, A.; Hysing, M.; Pallesen, S.; Krokstad, S.; Nordhus, I.H.; Overland, S. The Bidirectional Association Between Depression and Insomnia. Psychosom. Med. 2012, 75, 758–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Van Rheenen, T.E.; Meyer, D.; Neill, E.; Phillipou, A.; Tan, E.J.; Toh, W.L.; Rossell, S. Mental health status of individuals with a mood-disorder during the COVID-19 pandemic in Australia: Initial results from the COLLATE project. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 275, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Beck, A.T.; Ward, C.; Mendelson, M. Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1961, 4, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Russell, D.W. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess. 1996, 26, 20–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Benjamin, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery Rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat. Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fond, G.; Lancon, C.; Auquier, P.; Boyer, L. Prévalence de la dépression majeure en France en population générale et en populations spécifiques de 2000 à 2018: Une revue systématique de la littérature. La Presse Médicale 2019, 48, 365–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Filipovic-Pierucci, A.; Samson, S.; Fagot, J.-P.; Fagot-Campagna, A. Estimating the prevalence of depression associated with healthcare use in France using administrative databases. BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Lim, G.Y.; Tam, W.W.; Lu, Y.; Ho, C.S.; Zhang, M.W.; Ho, R.C. Prevalence of depression in the community from 30 countries between 1994 and 2014. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 2861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Özdin, S.; Bayrak Özdin, Ş. Levels and predictors of anxiety, depression and health anxiety during COVID-19 pandemic in Turkish society: The importance of gender. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2020, 66, 504–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Mirowsky, J.; Ross, C.E. Age and Depression. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1992, 33, 187–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Braunack-Mayer, A.; Tooher, R.; Collins, J.E.; Street, J.M.; Marshall, H. Understanding the school community’s response to school closures Canada during the H1N1 2009 influenza pandemic. BMC Public Health 2013, 13, 344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. López-Bueno, R.; Calatayud, J.; Casaña, J.; Casajús, J.A.; Smith, L.; Tully, M.A.; Andersen, L.L.; López-Sanchez, G.F. COVID-19 Confinement and Health RiskBehaviors in Spain. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. López-Bueno, R.; Calatayud, J.; Ezzatvar, Y.; Casajús, J.A.; Smith, L.; Andersen, L.L.; López-Sanchez, G.F. Association between current physical activity and current perceived anxiety and mood in the initial phase of COVID-19 Confinement. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Smith, L.; Jacob, L.; Trott, M.; Yakkundi, A.; Butler, L.; Barnett, Y.; Armstrong, N.C.; McDermott, D.; Schuch, F.; Meyer, J.; et al. The association between sreen time and mental health during COVID-19: A corss sectional study. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 292, 113333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Severity of the depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Figure 1. Severity of the depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Ijerph 18 03250 g001
Figure 2. Feeling of social isolation as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Figure 2. Feeling of social isolation as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Ijerph 18 03250 g002
Figure 3. Feelings of boredom and of fear as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Figure 3. Feelings of boredom and of fear as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Ijerph 18 03250 g003
Figure 4. Quality of sleep and of life rhythm as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Figure 4. Quality of sleep and of life rhythm as a function of severity of depressive symptoms in Study 1 and Study 2.
Ijerph 18 03250 g004
Table 1. Mean (SD) of the participants’ answers to the survey questions used in Study 1 according to depressive symptoms: no, mild and moderate-severe.
Table 1. Mean (SD) of the participants’ answers to the survey questions used in Study 1 according to depressive symptoms: no, mild and moderate-severe.
No DepressionMild DepressionModerate-Severe Depression
BeforeLockdownBeforeLockdownBeforeLockdown
MSDMSDMSDMSDMSDMSD
Age 45.6815.67 40.3115.81 32.9813.30
Education 14.853.11 15.013.06 14.622.63
poeple home 2.711.27 2.671.24 2.791.30
Living space (m2) 108.1557.39 104.3357.2 97.8260.53
Affiliation0.880.621.080.680.920.571.110.5961.070.741.300.80
Compliance lockdown 5.010.58 4.930.56 5.0040.59
Compliance gestures 4.490.89 4.360.86 4.121.03
Vulnerability perception 1.610.96 1.680.99 1.811.09
Social isolation 17.674.19 20.214.49 23.645.21
Boredom1.681.282.291.562.041.413.071.812.451.614.142.03
Anxiety2.561.602.761.553.351.713.711.684.251.794.901.77
Happiness5.491.135.141.185.011.204.401.224.231.564.501.46
Fear2.011.322.721.512.391.463.461.702.791.663.981.89
Anger2.311.562.481.662.801.743.221.803.141.704.011.85
Low-Arousal4.131.544.441.483.841.573.901.593.221.553.031.54
Sleep5.111.655.021.614.801.644.321.784.261.843.481.90
Life rhythm5.531.625.161.695.401.644.541.824.941.903.652.00
Acohol1.110.750.960.791.130.780.930.801.230.850.970.96
sleeping pills-anxiolytics0.080.300.060.240.120.320.090.300.260.510.250.56
Coffee/tea1.410.631.300.671.400.711.340.721.300.701.200.80
Cigarette0.260.540.250.530.310.570.260.560.470.680.290.58
Stimulant0.060.320.030.200.030.170.010.100.130.410.060.31
Other substances0.070.330.040.250.140.480.100.430.300.670.200.64
Table 2. Statistical results of comparisons between two groups (moderate-severe vs. mild vs. no-depression).
Table 2. Statistical results of comparisons between two groups (moderate-severe vs. mild vs. no-depression).
Group ComparisonDependent Variable Cohen’s dddltpRank q a
Study 1
Mod-Sev vs. NoBoredom0.608598.13<0.0011=0.050
Mod-Sev vs. NoLife rhythm0.488596.56<0.0012=0.043
Mod-Sev vs. NoSleep0.478596.37<0.0013=0.036
Mod-Sev vs. NoOther substances0.478596.37<0.0014=0.029
Mod-Sev vs. NoCompliant gestures0.408145.24<0.0015=0.021
Mod-Sev vs. MildBoredom0.335833.97<0.0016=0.014
Mild vs. NoBoredom0.259023.59<0.0017=0.007
Study 2
Mod-Sev vs. NoSleep0.7879010.14<0.0011=0.005
Mod-Sev vs. NoLife rhythm0.587907.46<0.0012=0.01
Mild vs. NoSleep0.4577965.97<0.0013=0.15
Mod-Sev vs. NoAffiliation0.357904.49<0.0014=0.02
Mod-Sev vs. MildLife rhythm0.384864.21<0.0015=0.025
Mod-Sev vs. MildSleep0.344863.77<0.0016=0.03
Mod-Sev vs. NoLiving space0.257893.24=0.0017=0.035
Mod-Sev vs. MildAffiliation0.274862.94=0.0038=0.04
Mod-Sev vs. NoAge0.227902.84=0.0049=0.045
Mild vs. NoLife rhythm0.217962.79=0.0110=0.05
aq–values = FDR ajusted p-values using Benjamini & Hochberg’s approach.
Table 3. Mean (SD) of the participants’ answers to the survey questions used in Study 2 according to depressive symptoms: no, mild and moderate-severe.
Table 3. Mean (SD) of the participants’ answers to the survey questions used in Study 2 according to depressive symptoms: no, mild and moderate-severe.
No DepressionMild DepressionModerate-Severe Depression
BeforeLockdownBeforeLockdownBeforeLockdown
MSDMSDMSDMSDMSDMSD
Age 46.6215.03 46.6515.36 43.3813.92
Education 13.262.95 12.912.76 12.882.85
poeple home 2.641.21 2.641.21 2.501.23
Living space (m2) 102.1048.55 95.8346.12 90.2943.86
Affiliation1.000.880.960.831.080.891.000.811.351.301.291.25
Compliance lockdown 4.490.89 4.360.86 4.121.03
Compliance gestures 4.510.94 4.470.97 4.051.07
Vulnerability perception 1.430.97 1.511.00 1.951.23
Social isolation 19.324.54 21.734.44 25.404.55
Boredom2.171.322.971.752.431.443.661.873.111.614.341.84
Anxiety2.801.493.361.663.431.484.341.604.001.604.781.60
Happiness5.301.124.671.314.861.093.851.294.281.413.151.48
Fear2.311.383.461.752.721.404.261.613.211.644.631.71
Anger2.451.393.131.782.901.433.741.763.391.584.301.75
Low-Arousal4.541.354.241.454.091.363.481.433.691.443.201.59
Sleep4.981.604.751.644.571.613.811.674.021.773.371.73
Life rhythm5.371.494.911.655.341.414.381.714.851.593.871.81
Acohol1.020.740.930.801.090.750.970.871.050.801.000.88
sleeping pills-anxiolytics0.140.470.160.510.260.660.280.720.530.870.600.97
Coffee/tea1.840.871.760.891.780.791.790.861.850.941.841.00
Cigarette0.470.900.470.920.450.900.450.920.801.070.791.13
Stimulant0.090.370.090.400.130.420.110.410.300.670.280.69
Other substances0.070.350.060.350.070.320.090.410.190.550.220.65
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martinelli, N.; Gil, S.; Chevalère, J.; Belletier, C.; Dezecache, G.; Huguet, P.; Droit-Volet, S. The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Vulnerable People Suffering from Depression: Two Studies on Adults in France. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3250. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063250

AMA Style

Martinelli N, Gil S, Chevalère J, Belletier C, Dezecache G, Huguet P, Droit-Volet S. The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Vulnerable People Suffering from Depression: Two Studies on Adults in France. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(6):3250. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063250

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martinelli, Natalia, Sandrine Gil, Johann Chevalère, Clément Belletier, Guillaume Dezecache, Pascal Huguet, and Sylvie Droit-Volet. 2021. "The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Vulnerable People Suffering from Depression: Two Studies on Adults in France" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 6: 3250. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063250

APA Style

Martinelli, N., Gil, S., Chevalère, J., Belletier, C., Dezecache, G., Huguet, P., & Droit-Volet, S. (2021). The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Vulnerable People Suffering from Depression: Two Studies on Adults in France. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(6), 3250. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063250

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop