Next Article in Journal
Subjective Social Status, Area Deprivation, and Gender Differences in Health among Chinese Older People
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effects of Physical Activity on Positive Emotions in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Youth Psychiatric Hospitalization in Israel during COVID-19: A Multi-Center Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of Exercise Intensity on Affective and Repetition Priming in Middle-Aged Adults

by
Cristina Perez-Rojo
,
Jennifer A. Rieker
and
Soledad Ballesteros
*
Departamento de Psicología Básica II, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, C/Juan del Rosal, 10, 28040 Madrid, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(16), 9873; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169873
Submission received: 27 June 2022 / Revised: 5 August 2022 / Accepted: 8 August 2022 / Published: 10 August 2022

Abstract

:
Previous research has shown that physical exercise improves memory. In the present study, we investigated the possible effects of the intensity of physical exercise as a function of the affective valence of words on implicit memory. In the study, 79 young adult volunteers were randomly assigned to perform moderate- (50% VO2max) or high-intensity exercise (80% VO2max) on a stationary bike. Once the required exercise intensity was achieved, participants performed an affective and repetition priming task concurrently with the physical exercise. Both groups showed similar repetition priming. The moderate-intensity exercise group showed affective priming with positive words, while affective priming was not found in the high-intensity exercise group. Facilitation occurred in both groups when a negative target word was preceded by a positive prime word. Our results suggest that the positive effect of physical exercise on memory is modulated by the affective valence of the stimuli. It seems that moderate-intensity exercise is more beneficial for implicit memory than high-intensity exercise.

1. Introduction

Physical activity (PA) can be defined as any body movement produced by skeletal muscles that require energy expenditure. Both moderate- and vigorous-intensity physical activity improve health [1]. Regular PA helps to prevent and treat at least 25 chronic medical conditions such as heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and breast and colon cancer [2]. It also helps to prevent hypertension, overweight, and obesity and can improve mental health, quality of life, and well-being [3], as well as some cognitive functions [4]. Aerobic exercise has been related to an improved ability to cope with stress, anxiety, and depression [5,6]. Moderate-intensity exercise increases positive effects [7,8] and has also been associated with improvements in executive control [9] and cognitive performance [10]. A recent systematic review and meta-analysis showed that aerobic exercise interventions effectively improve episodic memory. Mixed-effects analyses demonstrated the positive effect on episodic memory among studies with a high percentage of females (65–100%) and participants with normal cognition. Subgroup analyses revealed the moderating effect of age (p = 0.027), particularly studies on participants with a mean age between 55 and 68 years, as opposed to 69–85 [11].
According to the transient hypo-frontality model [12,13], the higher the exercise intensity, the larger the demands for neuronal and metabolic resources. Thus, high-intensity PA might impair proper prefrontal cortex functioning and affect higher cognitive functions while exercising. On the other hand, according to cognitive–energetic models (e.g., Refs. [14,15]), high-intensity exercise acts as a stressor that elevates the arousal level above a cognition beneficial threshold, inducing poorer cognitive performance. Exercise increases epinephrine and norepinephrine levels in the bloodstream [16].
Other studies have investigated the effects of acute high-intensity interval training (HIIT) as a time-efficient strategy to improve physical health and executive functioning, e.g., Refs. [17,18]. HIIT is characterized by repeated sessions of brief, intermittent, and intensive exercise separated by short phases of recovery. A recent systematic review [19] indicated that the majority of HIIT analyses showed the positive effects of HIIT on executive function.
It seems that exercise intensity affects higher cognitive processes rather than low-level cognitive processes. For example, simple reaction time is not significantly affected by an increase in exercise intensity, whereas performance on choice reaction time tasks decreases with increasing exercise intensity [20]. Several studies have reported that exercise can affect reaction time. For example, Kashihara and Nakahara [21] found that vigorous exercise improves the choice reaction time, but only for the first 8 min after exercise. Exercise did not affect the percent of correct choices the subjects made. Collardeau et al. [22] found no post-exercise effect in runners but did find that exercise improved reaction time during the exercise, perhaps due to increased arousal. With high-level cognitive demands, the optimum performance occurs with exercise levels of moderate intensity and duration. Two meta-analyses investigated the effects of exercise intensity on several cognitive functions [23,24]. Moderate-intensity exercise, as opposed to low- or high-intensity exercise, has shown the facilitating effects on processing speed and, with lower effect sizes, on attention and memory recall. Furthermore, effects vary as a function of cognitive demands and moment of assessment, with larger effects associated with executive functions than in other cognitive domains. Longer time gaps between exercise and test administration also had larger effects. When test administration followed exercise immediately, only light and moderate-intensity exercise produced cognitive enhancement, whereas after a short delay, the benefits of light exercise disappeared, and an enhancement was found after the performance of moderate-intensity exercise [23].
Evidence on the relationship between exercise and implicit memory is scarce (for a review, see [25]). Implicit memory is usually measured by showing priming, which is understood as the involuntary response facilitation or inhibition produced by one stimulus (prime) upon another stimulus (target) presented afterward. Repeated stimuli are identified more quickly and/or more accurately than non-repeated stimuli in young adults and healthy older adults [26,27,28], as well as in cognitively impaired older adults [29,30,31,32]. Most investigations studied visual and auditory priming, but it also exists in touch [33,34] and taste [35].
Recently, in two experiments using a word completion task, Loprinzi et al. [36] investigated if exercise intensity can modulate implicit memory. Neither the effect produced by a 15-min self-selected “brisk walk” (moderate-intensity exercise) nor the one produced by a 15-min run at 80% of the maximum heart rate (high-intensity exercise) differed from those produced in their respective active control groups.
Stimuli can also be categorized as a function of emotional valence. Affective priming relates to response facilitation produced by the automatic activation of attitudes from memory [37]. This type of priming is defined as the difference in performance on congruent trials (prime and target words have the same emotional valence) and incongruent trials (prime and target words have different emotional valences) [38]. In affective priming, the response to the target word is modulated by the valence of the prime stimulus. Primes with positive or negative emotional valence might either inhibit or facilitate the subsequent response to the target stimuli [39,40,41,42,43]. For example, Yao and Wang [39] examined the affective priming effect in a lexical decision task and found that positive prime words facilitated the processing of subsequent related stimuli, whereas negative prime words did not. According to the affect-as-information approach [44], affective information can be represented as nodes in a semantic network, in which the emotional load of prime stimuli modulates both the activation and use of affective associations. Positive primes increased the accessibility of the associative network, whereas negative primes produced inhibition [45]. This might be explained by the neural networks that underlie emotional processing.
Besides the common brain areas involved in memory processing, emotional memory additionally implicates amygdala networks that store hedonic features, and whose stimulation modulates the arousal levels [46,47]. However, to enhance memory stimuli, a certain threshold of emotional arousal must be passed. Only memories with a high emotional charge seemed to affect cognitive performance [48].
Previous research demonstrated gender differences in priming affective words. While the left hemisphere seems to prime affective words quickly regardless of gender, gender differences are likely in the right hemisphere because affective word processing occurs slowly in males but rapidly in females. This gender difference may result from increased sensitivity to the emotional feature of affective words in females [49].
As far as we know, this is the first study conducted to investigate the effect of exercise intensity on implicit memory as a function of stimulus repetition and the emotional valence of the stimuli. So, the present study aims (1) to investigate the effect of moderate versus high-intensity exercise on implicit memory, assessed with repetition priming, and (2) to study the effect of exercise intensity on the implicit processing of emotional words assessed by an affective priming task.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample Size

We conducted an a priori power analysis using G*Power 3.1.9.6 (Franz Faul, Kiel, Germany) [50] to calculate the appropriate sample size. Using an α value of 0.05, a power value of 0.80, and a medium effect size (f = 0.25) for differences in implicit memory [51] as a function of exercise intensity, as well as two groups within the F-test family, a total sample size of 76 was required. Considering a possible dropout rate of about 5%, a total of 80 participants was sufficient to detect the main significant effects.

2.2. Participants

Eighty healthy middle-aged adults were recruited at sports and leisure facilities located in Madrid (Spain). Before the experimental session, the physical condition of the participants was assessed with the Rockport fitness walking test [52], which is an indirect measure of the maximum amount of oxygen that one can utilize during high-intensity exercise (VO2max). The test consists of walking as fast as possible for one mile (1609 m) on a flat ground using a heart rate monitor. At the completion of the distance, the heart rate and time of completion were recorded. VO2max was calculated using the formula [52]:
VO 2 max   mL / kg / min = 132.853 + 6.315 · gender 0.3877 · age 0.0769 · weight ) 3.2649 · time   min 0.1565 · f inal   heart   rate
where gender = 1 for male and 0 for female.
Participants also completed the physical activity readiness questionnaire (PAR-Q) to determine the safety or possible risks of exercising based on their health history, current symptoms, and risk factors. Furthermore, the participants fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: they had a score of 17 or below on the Beck’s depression inventory [53,54], no physical contraindications to perform the required exercise, no current history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, no major surgery at least 6 months before the experiment, and normal or corrected to normal vision. One participant did not meet the inclusion criteria (a score above 16 on the depression inventory) and was excluded from further analysis. So, a final sample was composed of 79 healthy volunteers, who were randomly assigned to perform the priming task either at a moderate-intensity exercise at 50% of VO2max or a high-intensity exercise at 80% of VO2max. The moderate-intensity group was composed of 39 participants (16 males, M a g e = 35.58, SD = 7.88) and the high-intensity group of 40 participants (20 males, M a g e = 34.63, SD = 7.64). All participants individually performed their assigned exercise intensity while simultaneously performing the implicit memory task. The maximum oxygen consumption (VO2max), which is the highest rate at which oxygen can be transported and used during aerobic activities, was used to define the physical exercise load [55]. We considered moderate-intensity exercise at 50% of VO2max and high-intensity exercise at 80% of VO2max. [56]. The heart rate (HR) was calculated with the formula provided by Karvonen et al. [57]:
Target   HR   % = maximum   HR resting   HR × intensity   % + resting   HR
To calculate the maximum HR, we used the formula of Gellish et al. [58]:
maximum   HR = 207 0.7 × age
HR at rest was obtained by measuring the participant’s pulse after lying in the supine position for 10 min, remaining as relaxed as possible. Therefore, the resulting equations for the calculation of the exercise load for each group are as follows:
Moderate-intensity exercise:
Exercise   at   50 %   of   VO 2 max = maximum   HR resting   HR × 0.5 + resting   HR )
High-intensity exercise:
Exercise   at   80 %   of   VO 2 max = maximum   HR resting   HR × 0.8 + resting   HR )
Table 1 shows the demographic and physical baseline characteristics of the moderate- and high-intensity training groups. All participants gave their written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki.

2.3. Affective and Repetition Priming Task

For the affective priming task, we selected 320 emotion-related words (i.e., words that directly label an emotional state, such as “happy” or “fear”) and emotion-laden words (i.e., words that are associated with an emotion, e.g., “Thinking about holidays makes me feel happy”) from the Madrid Affective Database for Spanish [59]. Of these, 160 words had a positive valence, while the other 160 words had a negative valence. All words had a length of between 6 and 9 letters and similar arousal levels. For a description of the normative data, see Table 2.
Prime words were matched to target words as closely as possible for valence, arousal, length, and orthographic neighborhood. Initial letters and suffixes were matched when possible. The valence values were used to construct 160 word pair words, forming four experimental conditions: (1) 40 congruent positive trials, in which a positive target word was paired with a positive prime word; (2) 40 congruent negative trials, in which a negative target was paired with a negative prime; (3) 40 incongruent positive trials, in which a positive target was paired with a negative prime; and (4) 40 incongruent negative trials, in which a negative target was paired with a positive prime.
The words were presented in Courier-font 18-point lowercase letters in the centre of a black computer screen. Prime words were presented in white and target words were presented in yellow. Each trial started with the presentation of the fixation point in the center of the screen for 300 ms. Then, the prime (positive or negative) appeared for 200 ms, followed by a 300 ms blank screen. After a stimulus-onset asynchrony of 500 ms, the target (positive or negative) appeared and remained on the screen for 2000 ms. The trial ended with a 2000 blank screen, leaving a response window of 4000 ms. See Figure 1 for a schematic representation of the task paradigm. Responses to positive targets were mapped to the right index and negative targets were mapped to the left index, or vice versa, in a counterbalanced order across participants. Participants were asked to attend to the word presented in yellow (target) and to respond as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the “+” key if the word contained a positive affective valence or the “−” key if the word contained a negative affective valence. The word pairs were presented randomly in two blocks of 160 trials each, with a pause of 60 seg in between. In the second block, the word order was inverted within each pair. Thus, participants responded in the second block to targets that had previously been presented as primes. Altogether, the experiment comprised 160 congruent trials (the prime and target words of which were both positive (80 trials) or negative (80 trials)) and 160 incongruent trials (half of which were positive prime and negative target words (80 trials) and the other half were negative prime and positive target words (80 trials)), yielding a total of 320 trials per run.

2.4. Procedure

Participants were assessed individually at the sports facility, where the temperature was kept constant at 21 degrees Celsius. For the pulse measurement, a Suunto Ambit 3 Sport heart rate monitor with a chest belt was used. While performing the experimental tasks, participants were pedaling on a Tomahawk S Series stationary bicycle. On the handlebar of the bicycle, a platform was installed with a VAIO Sony VPCEH laptop (15.5 inches) and a 2.20 GHz Intel Core processor, and data collection was managed with E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA) experimental software. Participants maintained a constant distance of about 60 cm from the eyes to the screen. Two buttons of the computer keyboard were assigned for the responses: one for the right hand and one for the left hand. To avoid injuries, before starting the experimental tasks, participants performed a 5 to 10 min warm-up at a constant rhythm and resistance on the stationary bicycle, while reading the task instructions. The task began when the participants maintained their pulse uniform at the corresponding pulse rate (+/−10 beats), either at 50% of VO2max or at 80% of VO2max. Once the experimental run started, they were asked to keep the pulse stable until the task had finished. In the case of the affective priming task, participants could pedal at a light pace for 1 min after the first block, which lasted approximately 15 min, and could return to their corresponding pulse at the start of the second block. The whole experimental session lasted approximately 60 min.

2.5. Data Analysis

For all reaction time (RT) analyses, only correct trials were included. Trials with response latencies below 200 ms and above 1200 ms were excluded from the analysis. The RT-trimming procedure eliminated 8.8% and 7.1% of trials with targets with a positive valence, and 8.71% and 6.47% of trials with targets with a negative valence for the moderate- and high-intensity exercise groups, respectively. After data trimming, all distributions of response latencies showed acceptable levels of normality, homoscedasticity, and independence. There were no negative associations between error rates and RTs in any experimental condition, thus ruling out the possibility of a speed–accuracy trade-off. Error rates were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. A significance level of p < 0.05 was adopted for all statistical contrasts. All the data analyses were conducted with SPSS Statistic for Windows, version 28.0, Armonk, NY, USA, IBM Coorporation.

3. Results

Affective and Repetition Priming

In the present study, affective priming is defined as the difference in performance between congruent trials (prime and target words have the same emotional valence) and incongruent trials (prime and target words have different emotional valences). The valence could be either positive or negative. The word pairs were presented in two runs; in the second run, the word order was inverted. Thus, participants responded in the second trial run to words that were presented as primes during the first run. Table 3 presents a summary of the response latencies and error rates, as per the experimental and exercise conditions for the affective priming task.
To analyze the effect of emotional valence and exercise intensity on the facilitation produced by congruency, we conducted a 2 (group: moderate- vs. high-intensity exercise) × 2 (valence: positive vs. negative) × 2 (congruency: congruent vs. incongruent) × 2 (block: first trial run vs. second trial run) mixed ANOVA approach on RTs as a dependent variable, with the group as a between-subjects factor and the valence, congruency, and block as within-subject factors. The main effect of valence was significant F 1 , 77 = 133.383 ,   M S E = 3962.332 ,   p < 0.001 ,   η p 2 = 0.632 ,   1 β = 1 , indicating that the overall reaction times (RTs) were faster in response to positive words compared to negative words. This effect was not influenced by the exercise intensity, as shown by a non-significant valence × group interaction p = 0.219 . The main effect of the group was not statistically significant p = 0.96 . On the other hand, the main effect of congruency was significant F 1 , 77 = 10.355 ,   M S E = 837.328 ,   p = 0.002 ,   η p 2 = 0.119 ,   1 β = 888 , suggesting that RTs were different for trials in which target and prime shared the same valence (congruent trial) than when prime and target had different valences (incongruent trial). A significant congruency × valence interaction F 1 , 77 = 35.16 ,   M S E = 595.76 ,   p < 0.001 ,   η p 2 = 0.314 ,   1 β = 1 suggested that the congruency effect was different for positive vs. negative words. Pairwise comparisons showed that RTs in response to negative words were faster when preceded by a positive prime mean   difference = 19   ms ,   p < 0.001 , producing an inverted priming effect in incongruent trials with negative target words. Trials with positive target words produced a very small priming effect, without reaching statistical significance mean   difference = 4   ms ,   p = 0.184 , and the congruency × group interaction did not produce statistically significant results p = 0.063 . Finally, a significant three-way valence × congruency × group interaction F 1 , 77 = 5.778 ,   M S E = 595.76 ,   p = 0.019 ,   η p 2 = 0.07 ,   1 β = 0.66 suggests that this effect might differ in both exercise groups as a function of the emotional valence of the target word. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the moderate-intensity exercise group obtained a significant priming effect in response to positive words mean   difference = 13   ms ,   p = 0.004 , whereas this effect was absent in the high-intensity exercise group mean   difference = 5   ms ,   p = 0.258 . Furthermore, both groups responded faster to negative words when they were preceded by a positive prime (mean difference = 19   ms   and   19   ms for moderate- and high-intensity exercise, respectively, p < 0.001 ) , producing the beforementioned inverted priming effect in incongruent trials with negative targets (see Figure 2).
Regarding repetition priming, measured as the difference in response latencies between the first presentation and the repetition of the word-pair list, overall performance improved in the second trial run compared to the first run F 1 , 77 = 17.244 ,   M S E = 3134.634 ,   p < 0.001 ,   η p 2 = 0.183 ,   1 β = 0.984 , independently of the exercise intensity p = 0.498 . The repetition of the trial list had no significant influence on the differential response pattern to positive versus negative words p = 0.673 in either exercise group p = 0.77 . The response facilitation produced by congruent trials slightly improved in the second trial run F 1 , 77 = 3.914 ,   M S E = 394.635 ,   p < 0.051 ,   η p 2 = 0.048 ,   1 β = 0.498 in both groups to a similar degree p = 0.613 , suggesting a relationship between the strength of the association between the prime and target and the magnitude of the priming effect. Neither the three-way block × valence × congruency interaction p = 0.642 nor the block × valence × congruency × group interaction p = 0.659 produced significant results.
The analysis of the error rates showed a similar pattern for the moderate- and high-intensity exercise groups. Overall, participants committed less errors in congruent trials when the target and prime were positive [positive congruent: 5.49 % , negative congruent: 8.13 %   ( z = 4.275 ,   p < 0.001 ,   r 2 = 0.231 ) ], as well as in incongruent trials when the prime was positive [positive incongruent: 6.15 % , negative incongruent: 8.78 %   ( z = 4.906 ,   p < 0.001 ,   r 2 = 0.3 ) ]. Statistically significant group differences in error rates were not found in either positive ( z = 0.015 ,   n . s . ,   r 2 < 0.001 ) or negative congruent trials ( z = 2.51 ,   n . s . ,   r 2 < 0.001 ) , nor in positive ( z = 3.7 ,   n . s . ,   r 2 < 0.001 ) , or in negative incongruent trials z = 0.281 ,   n . s . ,   r 2 < 0.001 .

4. Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of the intensity level of PA (high or moderate) while healthy young adults performed an affective priming verbal task. Implicit affective memory was assessed with a verbal priming task which included positive and negative affective words. Repetition priming was assessed by comparing the performance of the two groups in the first and second blocks.
Our results showed the classical repetition priming effect previously reported using different types of stimuli such as words (e.g., [31,60]), as well as familiar (e.g., [61,62]) and unfamiliar (e.g., [63,64]) objects. We found that both groups of young adults benefitted from the repetition of the stimuli to a similar degree, suggesting that exercise intensity did not influence repetition priming.
As regards affective priming, only the moderate-exercise group showed a priming effect in response to positive words. Strikingly, both moderate- and high-intensity groups showed more response facilitation in incongruent trials than in congruent trials, suggesting that lexical emotional information might not be completely processed at the pre-attentive processing stage. This assumption is supported by the fact that the congruency effect slightly increased on the second trial run, signaling that a larger stimulus exposition can produce a larger affective priming effect. Furthermore, our results show that positive primes produced more response facilitation than negative primes, independently of the valence of the target word, and that RTs in response to negative words were faster when preceded by a positive prime. This inverted priming effect has been observed previously. For example, Rossell and Nobre [42] investigated affective semantic priming using a lexical decision task with neutral, happy, fearful, and sad affective categories. Their results indicated a priming effect for positive and neutral stimuli, whereas fearful stimuli produced a null effect, and negative information response inhibition. Sass et al. [40] also found that only positive primes produced priming in a lexical decision task. Moreover, in another study, Yao et al. [65] reported that only positive primes produced significant priming effects, whereas negative primes produced an opposite priming effect.
Bower [44] proposed an associative network with a different organization of emotional states represented as nodes in a semantic network, i.e., the asymmetric effects of positive and negative information, and suggested that positive relationships could be better elaborated and interconnected than negative ones [66,67]. Therefore, a positive prime might have a different effect on the processing of a target compared to a negative prime, as positive and neutral information share a similar semantic network, whereas negative information induces compensatory mechanisms that inhibit the propagation of associations between related concepts [44]. The processing of negative effects could reduce the optimization of information processing by requiring additional effort and time to process the target stimulus [42].
Only the moderate-intensity exercise group showed priming for positive words. It seems that high-intensity exercise interferes with the processing of positive information. One explanation could be that the processing of emotional valence goes in line with the physical stress level. High-intensity exercise causes discomfort, which could hinder the processing of dissonant (in this case positive) affective information. Our results are in line with those of Niven et al. [68], in that that affective valence during high-intensity continuous exercise and high-intensity interval exercise is consistently less positive than moderate-intensity continuous exercise, suggesting that they are less pleasurable. It seems that moderate-intensity exercise produces positive mood effects, indicating a greater priming in response to positive words. In contrast, high-intensity exercise produces a negative effect by inhibiting the evaluation of the target word [69,70].
It appears that priming is differentially influenced by exercise intensity and emotional valence, and that the processing of emotional information is modulated by the physical condition a person experiences when the information is processed. Overall, our results contribute to the knowledge of how physical exercise influences cognition and emotional processing. Nonetheless, more research is needed to fully understand the complex relationship between exercise intensity, metabolic resources, and the ability to process emotional information. Previous research suggests that complex neuromodulation, associated with extreme stress, might inhibit certain neural networks that are involved in the processing of higher cognitive functions.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, moderate-intensity exercise seems to keep energetic resources at a cognition-beneficial threshold, whereas an increase in intensity goes beyond it. In the present study, moderate-intensity exercise was related to an enhancement in implicit affective memory, and this was not the case for high-intensity exercise. On the other hand, both groups showed repetition priming. The practice of high- and moderate-intensity exercise benefitted similarly from stimulus repetition, suggesting that exercise intensity did not affect implicit processing per se, but was limited to the implicit processing of emotional information.

6. Limitations and Future Directions

This study is not without limitations. Our study was a two-arm uncontrolled study, and the absence of a control group does not allow us to generalize the results. On the other hand, even though the sample was selected as homogeneously as possible, most of the participants were in good physical shape, and results should be replicated with less active adults. Another possible limitation refers to the higher intensities of physical exercise. From a certain intensity of exercise onwards, certain parts of the task, such as simply pressing a button, are hindered, or in some cases even by drying sweat, which can lead to longer RTs. The results of this study therefore show the effect of exercise on implicit memory in young adults. Physical exercise influences cognition in different ways at different ages of the life cycle. It would be interesting to extend the study to different populations, such as different age groups or clinical populations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and study design: C.P.-R. and S.B.; participation enrollment and data collection: C.P.-R.; data analysis: C.P.-R. and J.A.R.; interpretation and manuscript preparation: All the authors. Final approval: All the authors. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The European Commission: H2020-SC1-DTH-03-2018 (grant ref. Nº826506) to SB. J.A.R. was supported by a Doctoral Fellowship from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (grant ref. #BES-2017-079760).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Ethics Committee of the Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED) approved the study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Acknowledgments

We thank John O’Sullivan for his helpful revisions on language issues. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. World Health Organization. Global Action Plan on Physical Activity 2018–2030: More Active People for a Healthier World; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/272722/9789241514187-eng.pdf (accessed on 15 May 2021).
  2. Pedersen, B.K.; Saltin, B. Exercise as medicine–evidence for prescribing exercise as therapy in 26 different chronic diseases. Scand. J. Med. Sci. 2015, 25, 1–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Warburton, D.E.; Bredin, S.S. Reflections on physical activity and health: What should we recommend? Can. J. Cardiol. 2016, 32, 495–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Hötting, K.; Röder, B. Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2243–2257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Scully, D.; Kremer, J.; Meade, M.M.; Graham, R.; Dudgeon, K. Physical exercise and psychological well-being: A critical review. Br. J. Sports Med. 1998, 32, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Bai, Y.; Wang, Y.; Li, Y.; Liu, D. Influence of1 exercises of different intensities on adolescent depression. Rev. Argent. Clín. Psicol. 2020, 29, 417–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Morgan, W.P.; OConnor, P.J. Exercise and mental health. In Exercise Adherence: Its Impact on Public Health; Dishman, R.K., Ed.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1988; pp. 91–121. [Google Scholar]
  8. Rocheleau, C.A.; Webster, G.D.; Bryan, A.; Frazier, J. Moderators of the relationship between exercise and mood changes: Gender, exertion level, and workout duration. Psychol. Health 2004, 19, 491–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Audiffren, M.; Tomporowski, P.D.; Zagrodnik, J. Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: Modulation of executive control in a random number generation task. Acta Psychol. 2009, 132, 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Murray, N.P.; Russoniello, C. Acute physical activity on cognitive function: A heart rate variability examination. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2012, 37, 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Aghjayan, S.L.; Bournias, T.; Kang, C.; Zhou, X.; Stillman, C.M.; Donofry, S.D.; Kamarck, T.W.; Marsland, A.L.; Voss, M.W.; Fraundorf, S.H.; et al. Aerobic exercise improves episodic memory in late adulthood: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Commun. Med. 2022, 2, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dietrich, A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Conscious. Cogn. 2003, 12, 231–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Dietrich, A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Res. 2006, 145, 79–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice Hall Inc.: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  15. Sanders, A.F. Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychol. 1983, 53, 61–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zouhal, H.; Jacob, C.; Delamarche, P.; Gratas-Delamarche, A. Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med. 2008, 38, 401–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Alves, C.R.R.; Tessaro, V.H.; Teixeira, L.A.C.; Murakava, K.; Roschel, H.; Gualano, B.; Takito, M.Y. Influence of acute high-intensity aerobic interval exercise bout on selective attention and short-term memory tasks. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2014, 118, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Kujach, S.; Byun, K.; Hyodo, K.; Suwabe, K.; Fukuie, T.; Laskowski, R.; Dan, I.; Soya, H. A transferable high-intensity intermittent exercise improves executive performance in association with dorsolateral prefrontal activation in young adults. NeuroImage 2018, 169, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ai, J.-Y.; Chen, F.-T.; Hsieh, S.-S.; Kao, S.-C.; Chen, A.-G.; Hung, T.-M.; Chang, Y.-K. The Effect of Acute High-Intensity Interval Training on Executive Function: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Draper, S.; McMorris, T.; Parker, J.K. Effect of acute exercise of differing intensities on simple and choice reaction and movement times. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2010, 11, 536–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kashihara, K.; Nakahara, Y. Short-term effect of physical exercise at lactate threshold on choice reaction time. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2005, 100, 275–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Collardeau, M.; Brisswalter, J.; Audiffren, M. Effects of a prolonged run on simple reaction time of well-trained runners. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2001, 93, 679–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chang, Y.K.; Labban, J.D.; Gapin, J.I.; Etnier, J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. McMorris, T.; Hale, B.J. Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation. Brain Cogn. 2012, 80, 338–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Loprinzi, P.D.; Edwards, M.K. Exercise and implicit memory: A brief systematic review. Psychol. Rep. 2018, 121, 1072–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Ballesteros, S.; Bischof, G.N.; Goh, J.O.; Park, D.C. Neural correlates of conceptual object priming in young and older adults: An event-related fMRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 2013, 34, 1254–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Ballesteros, S.; Reales, J.M.; Manga, D. Implicit and explicit memory for familiar and novel objects presented to touch. Psicothema. 1999, 11, pp. 785–800. Available online: http://www.psicothema.com/pdf/328.pdf (accessed on 16 February 2020).
  28. Mitchell, D.B.; Bruss, P.J. Age Differences in implicit memory: Conceptual, perceptual, or methodological? Psychol. Aging 2003, 18, 807–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ballesteros, S.; Mayas, J.; Reales, J.M. Cognitive function in normal aging and in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Psicothema 2013, 25, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ballesteros, S.; Reales, J.M.; Mayas, J. Picture priming in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Psicothema. 2007, 19, pp. 239–244. Available online: http://www.psicothema.com/pdf/3354.pdf (accessed on 15 March 2020).
  31. Redondo, M.T.; Beltrán-Brotóns, J.L.; Reales, J.M.; Ballesteros, S. Word-stem priming and recognition in type 2 diabetes mellitus, Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy older adults. Exp. Brain Res. 2015, 233, 3163–3174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Schacter, D.L.; Cooper, L.A.; Valdiserri, M. Implicit and explicit memory for novel visual objects in older and younger adults. Psychol. Aging 1992, 7, 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ballesteros, S.; Reales, J.M. Intact haptic priming in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence for dissociable memory systems. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Easton, R.D.; Greene, A.J.; Srinivas, K. Transfer between vision and touch: Memory for 2-D patterns and 3-D objects. Psych. Bull. Rev. 1997, 4, 403–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Caballero, A.; Reales, J.M.; Ballesteros, S. Taste priming and cross-modal taste-olfactory priming in normal aging and in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Psicothema 2018, 30, 304–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Loprinzi, P.D.; Gilbert, M.; Robinson, G.; Dickerson, B. Experimental investigation examining the effects of acute exercise on implicit memory function. Eur. J. Psychol. 2019, 15, 700–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Klauer, K.C. Affective priming. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 8, 67–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fazio, R.H. How do attitudes guide behavior? In Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior; Sorrentino, R.M., Higgins, E.T., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986; pp. 204–243. [Google Scholar]
  39. Yao, Z.; Wang, Z. The effects of the concreteness of differently valenced words on affective priming. Acta Psychol. 2013, 143, 269–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sass, K.; Habel, U.; Sachs, O.; Huber, W.; Gauggel, S.; Kircher, T. The influence of emotional associations on the neural correlates of semantic priming. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 676–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kissler, J.; Koessler, S. Emotionally positive stimuli facilitate lexical decisions—An ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 2011, 86, 254–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rossell, S.L.; Nobre, A.C. Semantic priming of different affective categories. Emotion 2004, 4, 354–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Rossell, S.L.; Shapleske, J.; David, A.S. Direct and indirect semantic priming with neutral and emotional words in schizophrenia: Relationship to delusions. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2000, 5, 271–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bower, G.H. Mood and memory. Am. Psychol. 1981, 25, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Clore, G.L.; Storbeck, J. Affect as information about liking, efficacy, and importance. In Affect in Social Thinking and Behavior; Forgas, J.P., Ed.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2006; pp. 123–141. [Google Scholar]
  46. LeDoux, J.E. Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behav. Brain Res. 1993, 58, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Loprinzi, P.D.; Frith, E.; Edwards, M.K. Exercise and emotional memory: A systematic review. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2019, 3, 94–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Canli, T.; Zhao, Z.; Brewer, J.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Cahill, L. Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. J. Neurosc. 2000, 20, RC99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Abbassi, E.; Blanchette, I.; Sirmon-Taylor, B.; Ansaldo, A.I.; Ska, B.; Joanette, Y. Lateralized affective word priming and gender effect. Front. Psychol. 2019, 9, 2591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Eich, T.S.; Metcalfe, J. Effects of the stress of marathon running on implicit and explicit memory. Psych. Bull. Rev. 2009, 16, 475–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Kline, G.M.; Porcari, J.P.; Hintermeister, R.; Freedson, P.S.; Ward, A.; McCarron, R.F.; Ross, J.; Rippe, J.M. Estimation of VO2max from a one-mile track walk, gender, age, and body weight. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1987, 19, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Beck, A.T.; Ward, C.H.; Mendelson, M.; Mock, J.; Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1961, 4, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A.; Brown, G.K. Beck Depression Inventory Manual; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1996; pp. 785–791. [Google Scholar]
  55. Powell, K.E.; Paluch, A.E.; Blair, S.N. Physical activity for health: What kind? How much? How intense? On top of what? Annu. Rev. Public Health 2011, 32, 349–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Borer, K.T. Exercise Endocrinology; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  57. Karvonen, M.J.; Kentala, E.; Mustala, O. The effects of training on heart rate: A longitudinal study. Ann. Med. Exp. Biol. Fenn 1957, 35, 307–315. [Google Scholar]
  58. Gellish, R.L.; Goslin, B.R.; Olson, R.E.; McDonald, A.; Russi, G.D.; Moudgil, V.K. Longitudinal modeling of the relationship between age and maximal heart rate. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2007, 39, 822–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hinojosa, J.A.; Martínez-García, N.; Villalba-García, C.; Fernández-Folgueiras, U.; Sánchez-Carmona, A.; Pozo, M.A.; Montoro, P.R. Affective norms of 875 Spanish words for five discrete emotional categories and two emotional dimensions. Behav. Res. Methods 2016, 48, 272–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Light, L.L.; Singh, A. Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1987, 13, 531–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Ballesteros, S.; Mayas, J.; Reales, J.M. Does a physically active lifestyle attenuate decline in all cognitive functions in old age? Curr. Aging Sci. 2013, 6, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Biederman, I.; Cooper, E.E. Size invariance in visual object priming. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1992, 18, 121–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Cooper, L.A.; Schacter, S.L.; Ballesteros, S.; Moore, C. Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: Effects of size and reflection. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 18, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Schacter, D.L.; Cooper, L.A.; Delaney, S.M. Implicit memory for unfamiliar objects depends on access to structural descriptions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen 1990, 119, 5–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Yao, Z.; Wang, Y.; Lu, B.; Zhu, X. Effects of valence and arousal on affective priming vary with the degree of affective experience denoted by words. Intern. J. Psychophysiol. 2019, 140, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Isen, A.M. Asymmetry of happiness and sadness in effects on memory in normal college students: Comment on Hasher, Rose, Zacks, Sanft, and Doren. J. Exp. Psychol. 1985, 114, 388–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Isen, A.M. Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 20, 203–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Niven, A.; Thow, J.; Holroyd, J.; Turner, A.P.; Phillips, S.M. Comparison of affective responses during and after low volume high-intensity interval exercise, continuous moderate- and continuous high-intensity exercise in active, untrained, healthy males. J. Sports Sci. 2018, 36, 1993–2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Alexopoulos, T.; Lemonnier, A.; Fiedler, K. Higher order influences on evaluative priming: Processing styles moderate congruity effects. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 57–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Storbeck, J.; Clore, G.L. The affective regulation of cognitive priming. Emotion 2008, 8, 208–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. A schematic representation of the priming paradigm. In the first block, 80 positive and 80 negative word pairs, which could either be congruent (the valence of the prime word and the target word were the same) or incongruent (both valences differed), were presented. In the second block, primes and targets reversed. So, the prime words of the first block were now the target words, and targets of the first block were now primes.
Figure 1. A schematic representation of the priming paradigm. In the first block, 80 positive and 80 negative word pairs, which could either be congruent (the valence of the prime word and the target word were the same) or incongruent (both valences differed), were presented. In the second block, primes and targets reversed. So, the prime words of the first block were now the target words, and targets of the first block were now primes.
Ijerph 19 09873 g001
Figure 2. (a) Affective priming; (b) repetition priming as a function of exercise intensity and valence. Error bars: +/−1 SE.
Figure 2. (a) Affective priming; (b) repetition priming as a function of exercise intensity and valence. Error bars: +/−1 SE.
Ijerph 19 09873 g002
Table 1. Mean values of socio-demographic background variables for the moderate- and high-intensity exercise group. SDs are shown in parentheses.
Table 1. Mean values of socio-demographic background variables for the moderate- and high-intensity exercise group. SDs are shown in parentheses.
Moderate-Intensity
(n = 39)
High-Intensity
(n = 40)
t (df)p
Men/women16/2320/20t (77) = −0.7940.430
Age35.54 (7.78)34.65 (7.74)t (77) = 0.5090.612
Education 11.38 (1.14)1.58 (1.11)t (77) = −0.7540.453
Depression 24.77 (3.97)4.03 (3.29)t (77) = 0.9080.367
VO2max 342.22 (6.53)43.80 (7.90)t (77) = −0.9650.338
1 Level of educational attainment was defined as follows: 0 = junior high school, 1 = senior high school, 2 = short-cycle tertiary education, 3 = Bachelor’s or equivalent, 2 Beck’s depression inventory (BDI) [53]. 3 VO2max measured after the 1-mile walk using the Rockport fitness walking test [51].
Table 2. Valence means and norming data 1 for positive and negative words. SDs are shown in parentheses.
Table 2. Valence means and norming data 1 for positive and negative words. SDs are shown in parentheses.
Positive Valence (n = 160)Negative Valence (n = 160)
Valence7.48 (0.59)2.14 (0.61)
Arousal6.01 (1.10)6.32 (1.01)
Length (letters)7.50 (1.10)7.75 (1.01)
Length (syllables)3.10 (0.69)3.17 (0.57)
Grammar class (a/n/v) 238/55/6727/59/74
1 Madrid Affective Database for Spanish (MADS) [59]. 2 a = adjective, n = noun, v = verb.
Table 3. Descriptive data of the affective priming task for the moderate-intensity and high-intensity exercise group per target valence, prime–target affective congruency, and trial run. Means (standard deviations) for reaction times in ms and median values (interquartile ranges) for error rates.
Table 3. Descriptive data of the affective priming task for the moderate-intensity and high-intensity exercise group per target valence, prime–target affective congruency, and trial run. Means (standard deviations) for reaction times in ms and median values (interquartile ranges) for error rates.
Trial RunPositive ValenceNegative Valence
CongruentIncongruentCongruentIncongruent
Response Latencies
Moderate-intensity exercise1673.44 (129.70)682.59 (130.10)753.94 (139.62)730.80 (131.57)
2648.25 (118.74)665.35 (116.61)728.21 (138.76)712.85 (128.52)
High-intensity exercise1686.25 (122.25)677.48 (130.97)744.92 (131.38)725.53 (122.46)
2668.64 (120.79)667.59 (120.67)726.95 (120.14)709.13 (120.22)
Error rates
Moderate-intensity exercise13.0 (0.0–7.0)5.0 (3.0–7.0)10.0 (5.0–12.0)7.0 (5.0–15.0)
23.0 (3.0–5.0)5.0 (3.0–5.0)5.0 (3.0–10.0)7.0 (5.0–10.0)
High-intensity exercise14.0 (0.0–10.0)5.0 (0.7–10.0)7.0 (3.5–10.0)7.0 (3.0–11.5)
23.0 (0.0–6.5)4.0 (3.0–7.0)5.0 (3.0–10.0)7.0 (5.0–12.0)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Perez-Rojo, C.; Rieker, J.A.; Ballesteros, S. The Effect of Exercise Intensity on Affective and Repetition Priming in Middle-Aged Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 9873. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169873

AMA Style

Perez-Rojo C, Rieker JA, Ballesteros S. The Effect of Exercise Intensity on Affective and Repetition Priming in Middle-Aged Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(16):9873. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169873

Chicago/Turabian Style

Perez-Rojo, Cristina, Jennifer A. Rieker, and Soledad Ballesteros. 2022. "The Effect of Exercise Intensity on Affective and Repetition Priming in Middle-Aged Adults" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 16: 9873. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169873

APA Style

Perez-Rojo, C., Rieker, J. A., & Ballesteros, S. (2022). The Effect of Exercise Intensity on Affective and Repetition Priming in Middle-Aged Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(16), 9873. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169873

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop