Next Article in Journal
“If I Was Educated, I Would Call the Ambulance and Give Birth at the Health Facility”—A Qualitative Exploratory Study of Inequities in the Utilization of Maternal, Newborn, and Child Health Services in Northern Ethiopia
Previous Article in Journal
Proactive Criminal Thinking and Restrictive Deterrence: A Pathway to Future Offending and Sanction Avoidance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Psychophysiological Responses of Cut Flower Fragrances as an Olfactory Stimulation by Measurement of Electroencephalogram in Adults

1
Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Korea
2
Plant, Environment, and Health Lab, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Korea
3
Department of Horticulture and Landscape Architecture, National Taiwan University, Taipei 106032, Taiwan
4
Department of Systems Biotechnology, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(18), 11639; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639
Submission received: 15 July 2022 / Revised: 12 September 2022 / Accepted: 14 September 2022 / Published: 15 September 2022

Abstract

:
Horticultural therapy (HT) is green care that can help improve and recover the health of modern people living in cities through natural experiences. Many studies have been conducted to determine HT’s therapeutic effects and underlying mechanisms, but investigation for developing readily applicable clinical techniques is insufficient. We aimed to investigate adults’ brain activity and emotional state during flower arrangement (FA) with different flowers in an HT program. We recruited thirty adults and used a randomized cross-over study method to set them to participate in five FA tasks at 90-s intervals. While performing FA tasks, the participants’ prefrontal cortex brain waves were measured by a wireless electroencephalography device and their emotional states between FA tasks were measured by questionnaires. Results showed that each FA task resulted in a different attention level of the participants. The participants showed the highest attention level during FA with stocks and carnations, while FA with lilies showed the lowest attention level among the five FA tasks. Instead, the participants showed the highest arousal, tension, and anxiety for emotional states during FA with lilies. Therefore, this study confirmed the differences in attention level and emotional changes according to flower types for using clinical techniques of HT for various clients.

1. Introduction

Around the potential of the natural environment, more and more attention has been paid to the positive impact that the natural environment can provide on human health and well-being [1]. This trend is supported by theories like the attention restoration theory and psycho-evolutionary stress reduction theory, which claim the natural environment serves as a practical component for mental stability and attention recovery in humans [2,3]. Since more than half of the world’s population lives in urban areas, and modern city life brings about more attention to fatigue and daily stress, the natural environment and its integration into city landscapes and routines have become a remedy for stressed urban populations [1]. Making the situation even worse, the recent global COVID-19 pandemic crisis and resultant home quarantine and isolation, social distancing, and remote working at home cause social and psychological problems, such as anxiety, depression, or other temporary and chronic mental diseases than any other time in the modern history [4].
Under these circumstances, green care has drawn the attention of researchers and practitioners worldwide as an alternative, active process to promote the health and well-being of urban residents. Green care refers to activities designed to provide green and natural environments to humans. This new global trend has been established in recent years in leading agricultural countries in Europe, North America, and Asia [5,6,7]. As a type of green care, horticultural therapy is defined as the use of horticultural activities for clients with special needs in specific therapeutic sessions moderated by a professional therapist [8,9]. Six positive health benefits can be found in horticultural therapy: Physical, psychological, social, cognitive, behavioral, and educational [10]. Horticultural activity can enable modern people to readily interact with natural elements even within a limited urban environment, not restricted by the surrounding physical environment [11]. Examples of horticultural activities used in a therapeutic session include, but are not limited to, gardening outdoors, planting indoors, and making crafts with living plants and flower arrangements [10].
One of the most popular therapeutic horticultural activities is flower arrangement (FA) in which clients are given flowery materials and tools to create or install decorations in the living space. As an extension of nature, FA is a form of art and craft as the beauty of nature itself and the formative skills of the practitioner converge to create an aesthetic object with flowering plants and other natural objects [12]. Moreover, the ingredients in the rose extract can significantly reduce the cognitive impairment of dementia, which was found by Esfandiary et al. [13]. Many types of flowers have distinct scents and visual properties, which can result in health effects to a different extent through the sensory system when used in horticultural therapy programs [14,15]. Therapeutic effects of FA in horticultural therapy programs have been investigated in various aspects: Physical effects (upper limb function, grip strength), cognitive effects (concentration, perseverance, accuracy of task), and psychological effects (psychotic symptoms, self-efficacy, stress) [16,17,18,19].
The therapeutic mechanisms of FA have been revealed through studies using devices that measured physiological responses, such as electroencephalogram (EEG), electromyography (EMG), heart rate variability (HRV), blood pressure, and near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) [20,21,22,23,24]. FA using natural flowers was found to increase sympathetic nerve stability and relaxation while reducing negative emotions like depression and anxiety in adult clients, compared with those when artificial flowers were given [21]. Tao et al. [23] reported that a group of women in their 20s who participated in FA showed relaxation effects represented by increased brain alpha waves and reduced blood pressure. In another study, FA also showed effects on cognitive function activation, increased concentration, and relaxation in older adults and older adults with dementia [24,25].
Despite the previous mechanistic findings, there is a lack of information about clinical techniques or the selection of floral materials to maximize the therapeutic effects of FA in coordinating a horticultural therapy program. As a result, most horticultural therapy programs are developed and executed based on the therapist’s speculations or personally conceived ideas, not based on scientific findings [26]. Therefore, the objective of this study was set to investigate the psychophysiological responses of individuals participating in FA using different flowers.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The participants of this study were a total of thirty adults over 20 years old of age (average age: 34.87 ± 13.04 years). We posted a recruitment notice on bulletin boards at apartments located in Gwangjin-gu, Seoul, South Korea. Moreover, the notice was posted on online bulletin boards of the official website of the government district. The selection criteria of the participants of this study were (1) adults over 20 years old without mental illness, (2) normal olfactory function, and (3) right-handed persons. The third criterion was set to eliminate possible interruptions of the dominance of handedness to the brainwaves. Studies have shown that the human cerebral cortex and cerebellar cortex produce different brainwaves depending on the movement of the dominant and non-dominant hands [27,28]. As caffeine or similar kinds of oral stimulants can disturb brain activity, the participants were required to fast 2 h before the experiment began. In addition, they were asked to complete an olfactory function test, scent survey, and visual analog scale for screening purposes. We also collected demographic information, age, gender, and body composition of the participants.
At the end of the experiment, subjects received $8 as an incentive to complete the experiment. The Institutional Review Board of Konkuk University approved this study (7001355-202103-HR-429).

2.2. Experimental Environment

We conducted the experiment in a special indoor space of 220 × 190 cm, on the Seoul campus of Konkuk University, Republic of Korea. The average air temperature, relative humidity, and brightness during the experiment were 24.47 ± 3.62 °C, 37.86 ± 14.01%, and 8285.76 ± 6703.80 LUX. Ivory curtains were arranged in the front and both sides of the participants’ seating positions in the experiment space, while the table was covered with a white tablecloth to help them to focus on the visual stimuli. There were five flowering plants in the experimental design, chrysanthemum (Dendranthema grandiflora), lily (Lilium orientalis Casa Blanca), hoary stock (Matthiola incana), rose (Hybrid tea rose), and carnation (Dianthus caryophyllus Adorable series).
In consideration of color, we chose to unify the color of the flowers to white in the experiment. And we trimmed flowers and readjusted the flowers’ height to the level of the nose of each participant. We placed the trimmed flowers in a black barrel container (19 × 23 cm) on the left and the flower mud (11 × 7.5 × 14 cm) in an ivory white pot (13 × 15 × 15 cm) on the right (Figure 1).

2.3. Experimental Protocol

The participants conducted each FA task for 90 s on average. Once they entered the experiment space, they wore the EEG device and had a rest on the chair before performing FA tasks. Then, they were assigned to perform a FA task for 90 s in random order. After the FA task, the participants were asked to answer a subjective questionnaire to record the changes in their emotions. In sum, this measurement routine consisted of a 30 s rest, a 90 s task, and a survey, and was repeated until the participants completed all five FA tasks we designed (Figure 2) [29]. Experiment participants were told how to perform FA activities before the experiment started. All experimental procedure was conducted in a blinded manner using a randomized cross-over study method, so the participants were requested not to speak or take any unnecessary activity during the experiment.

2.4. Measurements

2.4.1. Electroencephalogram (EEG)

A wireless dry EEG device (Quick-8; Cognionics, San Diego, CA, USA) was used to measure the brainwaves of the participants during FA tasks. The dry type of EEG device ensures the safety of the user from a potential electric shock by employing electrolyte gel on the device, which is different from the wet type. The dry electrodes integrated into the scalp of the EEG device amplify and further process measured electrical signals. The Defense Advanced Research Planning Agency has approved the use of this device for this study. The international standard of 10- to 20-electrode arrangement system was used to collect the brainwave data, and the reference electrode was attached to the left earlobe (A1) [30]. The orbitofrontal cortex in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is the region that converges and integrates olfactory, visual, and gustatory senses [31]. As such, we measured and analyzed EEG results from the left (Fp1) and the right (Fp2) PFC channels (Figure 3).

2.4.2. Subjective Evaluation of the Emotional States

Along with the EEG brainwave data collection, we also used subjective evaluations to verify the EEG results. These were the profile of mood state (POMS) and semantic differential methods (SDM) that are widely accepted in assessing emotional states of individuals.
The POMS questionnaire, originally developed by McNair et al. [32] and translated into Korean by Kim et al. [33], consists of 30 items to measure six emotional state variables: Tension and Anxiety (T-A), Depression (D), Anger and Hostility (A-H), Vigor (V), Confusion (C), and Fatigue (F) [32,34]. These variables are evaluated on a 5-point scale in each question from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very much). Thus, lower scores indicate better mental state of respondents. The total mood disturbance (TMD) score was further evaluated using the same response, following the formula: TMD score = (T-A) + (D) + (A-H) + (F) + (C) − (V). Lower TMD scores indicate more positive moods of respondents.
Created by Osgood (1952), SDM is also a method to evaluate emotional states through adjectives. The SDM questionnaire is composed of three items: “Comfortable-uncomfortable”, “natural-artificial”, and “relaxed-awakening”, which are measured on a 13-point Likert scale from −6 to +6. Unlike the POMS, higher scores of SDM present positive emotions of respondents [35].

2.4.3. Scent Strength of Flowers

We used the Labeled Magnitude Scale (LMS) [36] to measure subjective scent strength of flowers. The perceived scent strength by the participants was plotted on 0-to-100-point LMS and categorized into six stages by intensity: No sensation (0–1.4 score), weak (1.4–6 score), moderate (6–17 score), strong (17–35 score), very strong (35–53 score), and strongest imaginable (54–100 score).

2.5. Data Analysis

The collected EEG data were analyzed by the Bioteck Analysis Program (Bioteck, Daejeon, South Korea). Across the entire frequency band (4–50 Hz), brain waves from PFC were classified into four regions: Theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma waves (30–50 Hz) before processing [37]. These first-hand data can be further parameterized into three indices: Relative fast alpha power spectrum (RFA), relative slow alpha power spectrum (RSA), and sensory-motor rhythm (SMR)/theta spectrum. These second-hand data present different states of the brain in attention (Table 1).
On SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 25; Armonk, N.Y., USA), one-way ANOVA was run to examine the effects of FA tasks on the EEG parameters, POMS and SDM scores, and scent strength of flowers. Duncan’s post hoc analysis was used to separate the differences in the means. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 in all of the comparisons made in the analyses. For the descriptive statistics to report the demographic data of the participants, WPS Office Excel (WPS Office for Windows, V.11.2.0.10101; Kingsoft Powerword, 2021) was used.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Information

A total of thirty adults over the age of 20 participated in the study. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of olfactory stimuli, and the olfactory function of the experimental participants was tested. The results are shown in Table 2. That means the participants in this experiment have normal olfactory function.

3.2. Electroencephalogram (EEG)

The RFA of the left region of PFC was significantly different among five FA tasks (Table 3). During the FA tasks with stock and carnation, the RFA was higher than those during the other FA tasks. In contrast, the RFA during the FA tasks with lily was lower than those during the other FA tasks.

3.3. Subjective Evaluation of the Emotional States

The results of the SDM showed that the participants felt natural and relaxed when performing FA tasks, but the emotional states were significantly different among FA tasks (Figure 4). The type of flower plants also affected the subjective emotional states. When performing FA tasks with chrysanthemum, rose, and carnations, the participants experienced the most natural and relaxed feelings over the other FA tasks. On the other hand, when performing FA tasks with lily, the participants experienced less natural and awakening feelings (Figure 4).
The results of the POMS showed that only T-A scores were significantly different between FA tasks. The participants showed the greatest T-A scores during FA tasks with lily, while no differences were observed in the TMD scores, V, F, C, A-H, and D indices (Figure 5 and Figure 6).

3.4. Scent Strength

Roses, chrysanthemums, carnations, and stocks resulted in a strong scent perceived by the participants in our LMS classification, all measured around 20. These values were significantly different from the score measured in lilies, which recorded the strongest imaginable over 50 (Figure 7).

4. Discussion

We characterized flower arrangements (FA) using five different types of flowers with the measurements of brain activity and emotional states of the participants. The key EEG results showed that the RFA of the left region of PFC, which is related to concentration with calmness and relaxation of the participants, was highly activated during the FA tasks with stocks and carnations, compared with the other flowers. This difference in brain activity in the PFC is thought to be derived from flowers’ scent and shape. Each flower has distinguished olfactory and visual stimuli conveyed throughout our sensory system, processed uniformly, and are eventually integrated into the processes of the PFC. The processed information leads to differences in physiological responses, which are reflected by the signature brain wave patterns [31,43,44].
The FA participants using carnations and stocks showed the highest concentration in the stable state. Carnations and stocks are popular flowers and the demand for these flowers comes from their interesting flower shapes and attractive scents [45,46]. Moreover, these two flowers have a common volatile substance and scent similar to cloves named ‘eugenol’ [45,46,47]. Cloves are the most highly identified floral aromas worldwide and are widely used for various purposes ranging from perfume to medicine. Psychological studies reflected when subjects worked in a fragrant indoor environment, self-confidence and work efficiency were both escalated [48]. Furthermore, the other study also reflected that flower fragrance can induce brain activity, which affects human activity, language, and memory function [43]. In addition, the scent of cloves has been reported to be effective in reducing restlessness and headaches [49].
Lilies are also one of the most popular cut flowers worldwide. But in this study, when the participants used lilies to perform the task, the experimental participants showed the lowest rest concentration among the five flowers, which means that the fragrance of lilies made the participants more relatively awakened. The olfactory stimuli induce physiological responses, like relaxation and arousal, depending on the intensity [50] (Figure 7). However, this strongest imaginable scent of lilies also can cause an adverse effect [51]. The main aromatic ingredients of lilies are 3,7-dimethyl linalool, homomenthyl salicylate, and others [52]. As these volatile compounds are released at a high level, producing a solid scent to the surroundings, they may cause arousal effects, making the subjects less stable than the other FA tasks [53]. The volatile substance of lily, ‘linalool,’ of lily aroma oil, had physiological arousal and stimulating effects, such as increasing β waves in the brain, galvanic skin response (GSR), and sympathetic nervous activity of the participants [52]. Other studies have shown that the α wave in the left brain decreases in response to lily aroma [54].
A significant difference was found between FA tasks in POMS and SDM scores. Especially, the participants responded that they felt relatively less natural and stable, while conducting FA tasks using lilies, indicating that tension and anxiety were higher than in FA tasks using the other flowers. Because the sensory system influences the induction of emotions [43,44], flowers’ olfactory and visual characteristics provide discerned stimuli to the brain, resulting in the psychophysiological response. Indeed, odors influence mood, evoke powerful sensory experiences of pleasure or displeasure and trigger long-forgotten emotional memories [55]. Similar to our results, Hui-Tang et al. [52] showed that most people chose commendatory terms (e.g., excited and enjoyable) to describe the lily aroma.
The differences in brain activation state and emotional changes during these five FA tasks could likely help horticultural therapy professionals to develop and apply more effective horticultural therapy programs by strategically targeting the subjects. For instance, it would be most effective to use carnations and stocks in therapeutic FA programs for clients like children, adolescents, and those with ADHD or autism because these clients need attention development and improvement [56,57]. In contrast, it would not be more appropriate to apply FA tasks using lilies in a client with a depression and anxiety disorder or under high-stress conditions with excessive arousal [24,57].
However, we also identified the limitations of the study. First, we had not considered the baseline of the participant’s brain activity by measuring it before they began FA tasks. Second, we could not control other factors and variables that might have affected the results, such as tactile, visual, and motor stimuli during FA tasks. Finally, the present study found that the positive psychophysiological effects of FA were derived from the olfactory stimulation through reviewing and discussing previous studies’ results. Thus, to improve the study and increase the reliability of the study, more well-controlled study designs should be employed to conduct scientifically solid research.

5. Conclusions

This study showed that the five FA tasks with different types of flowers stimulated the brain activity in the prefrontal cortex differently and also changed the emotional status of the participants performing the FA tasks. Carnations and stocks were the most effective in the participants’ brain activation for concentration in the stable state. On the other hand, FA tasks with lilies caused brain activation in arousal states and emotions of tension and anxiety. The results of this study could be used as reference data for the development and coordination of professional therapeutic programs of horticultural therapy for various clients. As the sensitivity of the human sensory system is also affected by gender, investigation related to gender responses should follow the present study. Furthermore, data for a broader range of client groups are required to ensure HT programs’ evidence-based development.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.-A.P. Validation, S.-A.P. and Y.-T.W. Investigation, S.-A.P. and N.-Y.C., Writing—original draft preparation, Y.-T.W. and A.-Y.L. Writing—review and editing, A.-Y.L. and S.-A.P. Supervision, S.-A.P. Funding acquisition, S.-A.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Joint Research Project (Analysis of agricultural healing resource elements for improving mental health and developing measurement technology for the effects of agro-healing, Project No. PJ0161442022) from the Rural Development Administration.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted with the approval of Konkuk University’s Institutional Review Board (7001355-202103-HR-429).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

This paper was supported by the KU Research Professor Program of Konkuk University.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ohly, H.; White, M.P.; Wheeler, B.W.; Bethel, A.; Ukoumunne, O.C.; Nikolaou, V.; Garside, R. Attention Restoration Theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B 2016, 19, 305–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Kaplan, S.; Kaplan, R. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989; p. 340. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ulrich, R.S. Natural versus Urban Scenes: Some Psychophysiological Effects. Environ. Behav. 1981, 13, 523–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gloster, A.T.; Lamnisos, D.; Lubenko, J.; Presti, G.; Squatrito, V.; Constantinou, M.; Nicolaou, C.; Papacostas, S.; Aydın, G.; Chong, Y.Y.; et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health: An international study. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0244809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Anderson, K.A.; Chapin, K.P.; Reimer, Z.; Siffri, G. On fertile ground: An initial evaluation of green care farms in the United States. Home Health Care Serv. Q. 2017, 36, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Lee, A.; Kim, S.O.; Gim, G.M.; Kim, D.S.; Park, S. Care farming program for family health: A pilot study with mothers and children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Sempik, J. Green care and mental health: Gardening and farming as health and social care. Ment. Health Soc. Incl. 2010, 14, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Relf, P.D. Historical perspective on theoretical models for research and program development in horticultural therapy. Acta Hortic. 2008, 775, 79–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Son, K.C.; Jung, S.J.; Lee, A.Y.; Park, S.A. The theoretical model and universal definition of horticultural therapy. Acta Hortic. 2016, 1121, 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Park, S.; Lee, A.; Lee, G.J.; Kim, D.S.; Kim, W.S.; Shoemaker, C.A.; Son, K.C. Horticultural activity interventions and outcomes: A review. Hortic. Sci. Technol. 2016, 34, 513–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chen, H.M.; Tu, H.M.; Ho, C.I. Understanding biophilia leisure as facilitating well-being and the environment: An examination of participants’ attitudes toward horticultural activity. Leis. Sci. 2013, 35, 301–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Oh, D.M. A Study on an Application of Horticultural Therapy to Social Welfare Policy. Ph.D. Thesis, Jeju University, Jeju, Korea, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  13. Esfandiary, E.; Karimipour, M.; Mardani, M.; Alaei, H.; Ghannadian, M.; Kazemi, M.; Mohammadnejad, D.; Hosseini, N.; Esmaeili, A. Novel effects of Rosa damascena extract on memory and neurogenesis in a rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. Res. 2014, 92, 517–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Haviland-Jones, J.; Rosario, H.H.; Wilson, P.; McGuire, T.R. An environmental approach to positive emotion: Flowers. Evol. Psychol. 2005, 3, 104–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kim, M.J.; Han, C.W.; Min, K.Y.; Cho, C.Y.; Lee, C.W.; Ogawa, Y.; Mori, E.; Kohzuki, M. Physical exercise with multicomponent cognitive intervention for older adults with Alzheimer’s disease: A 6-month randomized controlled trial. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. Extra 2016, 6, 222–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Lavin, J.; Lavin, C.; Bai, X.; Mastropaolo, S.; Feldman, D. Determining the effect of group flower arranging sessions on caregiver self-efficacy and stress levels in an in-patient hospice. OMEGA-J. Death Dying 2021, 84, 491–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Han, I.J. Effect of Horticultual Therapy Program on Mental Symptomatic Relief and Rehabilitation of Schizophrenics. Ph.D. Thesis, Catholic University of Daegu, Gyeongsan, Korea, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kang, S.J. Evaluation of the effects of horticultural therapy on physical ability in elderly people. Master’s Thesis, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea, 2002. Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
  19. Lee, S.; Um, S.; Song, J.; Son, K. Effect of horticultural therapy using the floral decoration training on the improvement of occupational performance ability and vocational rehabilitation in mentally retarded. Hortic. Sci. Technol. 2007, 25, 474–484. [Google Scholar]
  20. Lee, S.S.; Park, S.; Kwon, O.Y.; Song, J.E.; Son, K.C. Measuring range of motion and muscle activation of flower arrangement tasks and application for improving upper limb function. Hortic. Sci. Technol. 2012, 30, 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Yang, J.W.; Lee, M.S.; Joung, D.; Park, B.J. Effects of using Natural and Artificial Flowers in Flower Arrangement on Psychological and Physiological Relaxation. J. People Plants Environ. 2022, 25, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Du, J.; Yin, J.; Chen, X.; Hassan, A.; Fu, E.; Li, X. Electroencephalography (EEG)-Based Neural Emotional Response to Flower Arrangements (FAs) on Normal Elderly (NE) and Cognitively Impaired Elderly (CIE). Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Tao, J.; Hassan, A.; Qibing, C.; Yinggao, L.; Li, G.; Jiang, M.; Li, D.; Nian, L.; Bing-Yang, L.; Ziqin, Z. Psychological and physiological relaxation induced by nature-working with ornamental plants. Discret. Dyn. Nat. Soc. 2020, 2020, 6784512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Morita, Y.; Ebara, F.; Morita, Y.; Horikawa, E. Increased activity in the right prefrontal cortex measured using near-infrared spectroscopy during a flower arrangement task. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2018, 22, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Mita, S.; Hosokawa, M.; Hayashi, T. The effect of reproducing two-dimensional photographs of flower arrangements in three dimensions on prefrontal blood flow in elderly patients with dementia. Acta Hortic. 2020, 1330, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lee, J.H.; Choi, H.S.; Yun, S.Y.; Choi, B.J.; Jang, E.J. Effects of horticultural activities and flower tea drinking based on reminiscent storytelling on demented elders’ cognitive and emotional functions. J. People Plants Environ. 2017, 20, 351–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tarkka, I.M.; Hallett, M. Cortical topography of premotor and motor potentials preceding self-paced, voluntary movement of dominant and non-dominant hands. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1990, 75, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Moulton, E.; Galléa, C.; Kemlin, C.; Valabregue, R.; Maier, M.A.; Lindberg, P.; Rosso, C. Cerebello-cortical differences in effective connectivity of the dominant and non-dominant hand during a visuomotor paradigm of grip force control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Igarashi, M.; Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Ohira, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of olfactory stimulation by fresh rose flowers on autonomic nervous activity. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2014, 20, 727–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jasper, H.H. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1958, 10, 370–375. [Google Scholar]
  31. Rolls, E.T. The orbitofrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1996, 351, 1433–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. McNair, D.M.; Heuchert, J.P.; Shilony, E. Profile of Mood States: Bibliography 1964–2002; Multi-Health Systems: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim, E.J.; Lee, S.I.; Jeong, D.U.; Shin, M.S.; Yoon, I.Y. Standardization and reliability and validity of the Korean edition of Profile of Mood States (K-POMS). Sleep Med. Psychophysiol. 2003, 10, 39–51. [Google Scholar]
  34. McNair, D.M. Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  35. Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J.; Tannenbaum, P.H. The Measurement of Meaning; University of Illinois Press: Champaign, IL, USA, 1957; p. 47. [Google Scholar]
  36. Green, B.G.; Dalton, P.; Cowart, B.; Shaffer, G.; Rankin, K.; Higgins, J. Evaluating the ‘Labeled Magnitude Scale’for measuring sensations of taste and smell. Chem. Senses 1996, 21, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sowndhararajan, K.; Cho, H.; Yu, B.; Kim, S. Effect of olfactory stimulation of isomeric aroma compounds,(+)-limonene and terpinolene on human electroencephalographic activity. Eur. J. Integr. Med. 2015, 7, 561–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Cho, S.H. Effects of horseback riding exercise on the relative alpha power spectrum in the elderly. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2017, 70, 141–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Klimesch, W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 1999, 29, 169–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kober, S.E.; Witte, M.; Stangl, M.; Väljamäe, A.; Neuper, C.; Wood, G. Shutting down sensorimotor interference unblocks the networks for stimulus processing: An SMR neurofeedback training study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015, 126, 82–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Hong, S.C.; Yoo, Y.S.; Kim, E.S.; Kim, S.C.; Park, S.H.; Kim, J.K.; Kang, S.H. Development of KVSS test (Korean version of Sniffin’Sticks Test). Korean J. Otorhinolaryngol.-Head Neck Surg. 1999, 42, 855–860. [Google Scholar]
  42. Yang, Y.; Choi, H.R.; Cho, J.H.; Hong, S.C.; Kim, J.K. Clinical Feasibility of Scent Survey for Screening Test for Olfactory Function. J. Rhinol. 2018, 25, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Stevenson, R.J.; Wilson, D.A. Learning to Smell: Olfactory Perception from Neurobiology to Behavior; JHU Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  44. Huang, L.C. An Evaluation of Flower Visual and Olfactory Perceptions Influencing Consumer Attitudes within Socio-Economic Groups. Ph.D. Thesis, Kansas State University, Manhattan, KS, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  45. Çelikel, F.G.; Reid, M.S. Postharvest handling of stock (Matthiola incana). HortScience 2002, 37, 144–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Bahmanzadegan, A.; Rowshan, V. Static headspace analysis and polyphenol content of Tagetes erecta, Matthiola incana, Erysimum cheiri, Gaillardia grandiflora and Dahlia pinnata in Iran. Anal. Chem. Lett. 2018, 8, 794–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hussain, S.; Rahman, R.; Mushtaq, A.; Belaskri, A.E. Clove: A review of a precious species with multiple uses. Int. J. Chem. Biochem. Sci. 2017, 11, 129–133. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kadohisa, M. Effects of odor on emotion, with implications. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Weber, S.T.; Heuberger, E. The impact of natural odors on affective states in humans. Chem. Senses 2008, 33, 441–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Campenni, C.E.; Crawley, E.J.; Meier, M.E. Role of suggestion in odor-induced mood change. Psychol. Rep. 2004, 94, 1127–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Pan, H.-T.; Li, X.; Jin, Z.-L.; Sun, M.; Wang, J.; Yang, W.-R.; Kong, Y.; Zhang, Q.-X. Major aroma ingredients of oriental lily’Siberia’and their effect on humans. Acta Hortic. 2011, 925, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kong, Y.; Sun, M.; Pan, H.T.; Zhang, Q.X. Composition and emission rhythm of floral scent volatiles from eight lily cut flowers. J. Am. Soc. Hortic. Sci. 2012, 137, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lee, Y.; Jeoung, B. The relationship between the behavior problems and motor skills of students with intellectual disability. J. Exerc. Rehabil. 2016, 12, 598–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Ehrlichman, H.; Bastone, L. Olfaction and emotion. In Science of Olfaction; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 410–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Betts, K.A.; Sikirica, V.; Hodgkins, P.; Zhou, Z.; Xie, J.; DeLeon, A.; Erder, M.H.; Wu, E.Q. Period prevalence of concomitant psychotropic medication usage among children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during 2009. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2014, 24, 260–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Di Carmine, F.; Berto, R. Contact with nature can help ADHD children to cope with their symptoms. The state of the evidence and future directions for research. Vis. Sustain. 2020, 14, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ressler, K.J.; Nemeroff, C.B. Role of serotonergic and noradrenergic systems in the pathophysiology of depression and anxiety disorders. Depress. Anxiety 2000, 12, 2–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental space layout.
Figure 1. Experimental space layout.
Ijerph 19 11639 g001
Figure 2. Experiment protocol.
Figure 2. Experiment protocol.
Ijerph 19 11639 g002
Figure 3. Electrode locations of the electroencephalography (EEG) for this experiment.
Figure 3. Electrode locations of the electroencephalography (EEG) for this experiment.
Ijerph 19 11639 g003
Figure 4. The results of semantic differential method (SDM) evaluation when performing five activities (** is p < 0.01, a > b, NS = no significant difference).
Figure 4. The results of semantic differential method (SDM) evaluation when performing five activities (** is p < 0.01, a > b, NS = no significant difference).
Ijerph 19 11639 g004
Figure 5. The result of total mood disturbance (TMD) is the total score on the profile of mood state (POMS) when performing five activities (NS = no significant difference).
Figure 5. The result of total mood disturbance (TMD) is the total score on the profile of mood state (POMS) when performing five activities (NS = no significant difference).
Ijerph 19 11639 g005
Figure 6. The results of profile of mood state (POMS) are divided into six indicators: Vigor, confusion, anger and hostility, depression, tension and anxiety, fatigue (NS = no significant difference, ** is p < 0.01, a > b > c).
Figure 6. The results of profile of mood state (POMS) are divided into six indicators: Vigor, confusion, anger and hostility, depression, tension and anxiety, fatigue (NS = no significant difference, ** is p < 0.01, a > b > c).
Ijerph 19 11639 g006
Figure 7. Scent strength of five different types of flowers; no sensation (0 score), weak (1–5 score), moderate (6–15 score), strong (16–35 score), very strong (36–50 score), and strongest imaginable (51–100 score) (*** is p <0.001, a > b).
Figure 7. Scent strength of five different types of flowers; no sensation (0 score), weak (1–5 score), moderate (6–15 score), strong (16–35 score), very strong (36–50 score), and strongest imaginable (51–100 score) (*** is p <0.001, a > b).
Ijerph 19 11639 g007
Table 1. EEG power spectrum indicators used in this study.
Table 1. EEG power spectrum indicators used in this study.
Analysis IndicatorThe Full Name of the EEG Power Spectrum IndicatorIndicator Estimate (Ratio)State
RFARelative fast alphaHigher alpha (11–13 Hz)/Total frequency (4–50 Hz)Attention and concentration in a relaxed state [38,39].
RSARelative slow alphaLower alpha (11–13 Hz)/Total frequency (4–50 Hz)Stability and relax [38,39].
SMR/thetaSensory Motor Rhythm to thetaLower beta (12–15 Hz)/theta (4–8 Hz)Attention related to cognitive function [40].
Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of the participants (N = 30).
Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of the participants (N = 30).
VariableMale (n = 7)Female (n = 23)Total (N = 30)
Mean ± SD
Age (years)32.57 ± 12.4635.57 ± 13.4134.87 ± 13.04
Body Height (cm)175.94 ± 3.36162.16 ± 4.33165.61 ± 7.30
Body Weight (kg)70.94 ± 13.8657.56 ± 9.8060.91 ± 12.19
Olfactory
Function
SSS 185.71 ± 7.8983.7 ± 13.6683.77 ± 12.47
VAS 28.57 ± 0.987.78 ± 1.627.97 ± 1.51
1 SSS = Scent survey for screening; A score of 47 or higher is a normal olfactory group [41,42]. 2 VAS = Visual analog scale; A score of 5 or higher is a normal olfactory group [41,42].
Table 3. Results of EEG values of RFA (Relative Fast Alpha), SMR (Sensorimotor Rhythm), and RSA (Relative Slow Alpha) indicators according to the activity using each plant (N = 30).
Table 3. Results of EEG values of RFA (Relative Fast Alpha), SMR (Sensorimotor Rhythm), and RSA (Relative Slow Alpha) indicators according to the activity using each plant (N = 30).
Flower Arrangement ActivityRFA 1SMR 2RSA 3
Fp1 6Fp2 6Fp1Fp2Fp1Fp2
Mean ± SD
Total
(N = 30)
Chrysanthemum0.048 ± 0.006 ab50.049 ± 0.0080.060 ± 0.0070.064 ± 0.0120.121 ± 0.0160.122 ± 0.017
Lily0.044 ± 0.004 b0.049 ± 0.0090.057 ± 0.0090.062 ± 0.0130.113 ± 0.0220.116 ± 0.020
Stock0.052 ± 0.013 a0.051 ± 0.0070.065 ± 0.0150.066 ± 0.0130.118 ± 0.0210.118 ± 0.015
Rose0.048 ± 0.007 ab0.049 ± 0.0090.060 ± 0.0090.062 ± 0.0110.118 ± 0.0240.116 ± 0.016
Carnation0.050 ± 0.011 a0.049 ± 0.0080.063 ± 0.0180.061 ± 0.0100.121 ± 0.0270.119 ± 0.014
P 40.024 *0.886 NS0.157 NS0.518 NS0.508 NS0.676 NS
1 The frequency of Relative Fast Alpha (RFA) [(11 Hz–13 Hz)/(4 Hz–50 Hz)] is an indicator of concentration. 2 The frequency of SMR (Sensorimotor Rhythm) [(12 Hz–15 Hz)/(4 Hz–50 Hz)] is an indicator of concentration. 3 The frequency of RSA (Relative Slow Alpha) [(8 Hz–13 Hz)/(4 Hz–50 Hz)] is an indicator of emotional stability. 4 Statistical method used one-way ANOVA (NS had no significant difference, * p < 0.05). 5 The statistical method used Duncan’s post hoc analysis (a > b). 6 Fp1: left frontal lobe; Fp2: right frontal lobe.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wu, Y.-T.; Lee, A.-Y.; Choi, N.-Y.; Park, S.-A. Psychophysiological Responses of Cut Flower Fragrances as an Olfactory Stimulation by Measurement of Electroencephalogram in Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 11639. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639

AMA Style

Wu Y-T, Lee A-Y, Choi N-Y, Park S-A. Psychophysiological Responses of Cut Flower Fragrances as an Olfactory Stimulation by Measurement of Electroencephalogram in Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(18):11639. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wu, Yu-Tong, A-Young Lee, Na-Yoon Choi, and Sin-Ae Park. 2022. "Psychophysiological Responses of Cut Flower Fragrances as an Olfactory Stimulation by Measurement of Electroencephalogram in Adults" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 18: 11639. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop