Next Article in Journal
Optimal Selection of Seed-Trees Using the Multi-Objective NSGA-II Algorithm and a Seed Dispersal Model
Previous Article in Journal
How Does Carbon Trading Impact China’s Forest Carbon Sequestration Potential and Carbon Leakage?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Influence of Different Forest Landscapes on Physiological and Psychological Recovery

1
Department of Forest Therapy, Chungbuk National University, Cheongju 28644, Republic of Korea
2
School of Journalism and Communications, Henan University of Technology, Zhengzhou 450001, China
3
Faculty of Forestry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada
4
Department of Forest Science, Chungbuk National University, Cheongju 28644, Republic of Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Forests 2024, 15(3), 498; https://doi.org/10.3390/f15030498
Submission received: 11 January 2024 / Revised: 5 March 2024 / Accepted: 6 March 2024 / Published: 8 March 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Forest Economics, Policy, and Social Science)

Abstract

:
Previous studies have reported that exposure to forest landscapes has many benefits on human physiological and psychological health, as well as effectiveness in reducing stress and improving mood depending on different types of landscape. This study examined the effects of different types of forest landscapes for indirect visual experiences on the physical and mental health of college students (N = 33). Three types of landscape images were selected, in which forest landscapes included vegetated landscapes and water features, and as a control, we set up images of urban landscapes without natural elements. Physiological and psychological assessment was performed before the experiment for each student, followed by each student being exposed consecutively to nine landscape images for 3 min (each type) and assessed after each exposure. The results showed that both forest landscapes decreased stress (p < 0.05 for all) and improved mood and self-esteem (p < 0.01 for all). In contrast, water landscapes showed a slightly higher impact on physical and mental health than vegetated landscapes, but there was no significant difference. Conversely, only for self-esteem, the response after viewing vegetated landscapes (VL, SD = 29.06 ± 3.38) was better than after water views (WL, SD = 28.21 ± 2.48). Despite significant differences between the two types of forest landscapes not being found in our findings, the benefits of forest landscapes were observed through understanding the health-promoting capacities of different forest landscapes.

1. Introduction

Since the previous century, there has been a global shift toward economic reform and expansion, paralleled by rapid industrial growth and extensive urbanization. The World Bank estimated in 2023 that 56% of the world’s population lived in urban areas, a figure projected to increase by 1.5 times to 6 billion by 2045 [1]. The swift pace of urban development has been a catalyst for economic and social advancement, yet it has also given rise to a host of environmental challenges and changes in lifestyle, such as local climate alteration [2,3,4,5], increased air and water pollution [6,7,8], a major reduction in natural vegetation production [9,10,11,12], and decreased ecosystem services [13]. These problems are eroding human physical and mental health and increasing exposure to environmental stressors. Urban environments today, characterized by their unceasing and diverse distractions, may hinder individuals’ ability to concentrate on vital matters or to attain states of relaxation [14,15]. This crisis was particularly pronounced among college students, who face multiple health risk factors [16,17,18,19,20,21]. Previous studies have shown that approximately half of the college student population reports significant levels of stress, anxiety, depression, or their combination [22], underscoring the growing need for effective stress management and healthy living approaches [23,24]. The restorative effect of natural environments is increasingly recognized in environmental psychology and public health domains [25,26,27]. A report in 1974 by M. Lalonde [28] introduced the “Health Field Concept”, which broadly considered environmental factors and identified them as one of the determinants of health promotion. The theory of therapeutic landscapes [29] explored why certain environments contribute to a healing sense of place. And building on this work, the contemplative landscape model by Olszewska-Guizzo et al. [30] focuses on how certain landscapes, such as forests, can be designed or identified to promote mental and physical recovery by providing a space for contemplation and relaxation. Moreover, the Attention Restoration Theory suggested that depleted directed attention caused mental fatigue and that exposure to natural environments facilitated the restoration of the capacity for directed attention [31]. Stress Reduction Theory suggests that the non-threatening natural setting as a restorative environment could evoke positive emotions and block negative emotions [32,33]. More and more studies have corroborated the restorative effects of the natural environment as a medium for people’s physical and mental health [25,26,34,35]. Restorative primarily translates into physiological and psychological advantages of exposure to the natural environment. That is, positive physiological and psychological changes resulting from exposure to forest landscapes have a restorative effect on people’s physical and mental health [36].
Forests have been studied a lot of times as typical restorative environments [37,38,39]. These studies directly proved or inferred that the forest environment was effective in relieving stress and depression, and individuals felt more relaxation, dynamism, and rest when exposed to the forest [31,40,41,42]. Studies focusing on the physiological effects of relaxation exposure to the forest space showed that exposure to the forest could decrease pre-frontal cortex cerebral blood flow, lower blood pressure and heart rate [39,40], enhance parasympathetic activity, inhibit sympathetic activity [42,43,44], decrease salivary cortisol stress hormone concentrations [42,43], and enhance anticancer proteins and natural killer cells activities [45,46], thereby enhancing immune performance. This was mainly due to various environmental elements in the forest, such as sufficient oxygen, phytoncides, sunlight, sound, etc. Additionally, studies have demonstrated that forest settings, as opposed to urban ones, enhance positive mood states, alleviate negative emotions like depression and anxiety [47,48,49], and improve specific psychological responses such as attention recovery, self-esteem, and life quality [50,51,52].
However, the capacity of forest environments to improve human health was influenced by landscape type and characteristics [31,53,54], which meant that some types or characteristics were more effective for health recovery. Green plants were more favored by people than other colored plants [55,56,57]. Nordh et al. [58] applied choice-based conjoint analysis to assess the restorative value of small urban parks, identifying landscape features such as grass, trees, and shrubs as key factors in restoration likelihood. A study of landscape types and landscape elements in urban parks showed that different landscape types elicit varied physiological and emotional reactions; landscapes mimicking natural mountain forests showed the highest restorative impacts [59], coniferous forests provided relaxation for the subjects, and broadleaved forests produced the most stable mood [60]. Furthermore, various specific landscape features, including “open view” [61], “stand density” [60,62], “planting with flower cover” [63], etc., have been identified as significant in creating restorative environments within each study’s specific context.
Similar to green landscapes, blue landscapes are also increasingly recognized for their therapeutic and public health benefits [31]. Bell et al. discovered that physical activity, social interaction, and psychological benefits were evident in people visiting blue spaces [64]. The research by Tang et al. [65] used fMRI to compare the rejuvenating effects of different landscapes (urban, mountain, forest, water), noting that water landscapes, unlike urban ones, increased neural activity in brain regions associated with attention, implying a stimulatory effect on the attention system. They found that in contrast to urban landscapes, viewing water landscapes was associated with increased brain neural activity in the attention area, suggesting that viewing water landscapes might stimulate the rest of the attention system. In addition, a study on seven different forest landscape types for stress relief showed that exposure to forest–water spaces, especially dynamic water landscapes, was more effective for psychological stress relief [66]. Moreover, a study of Han [67] on landscape preferences showed that the highest-ranked forest scenes contained water.
However, there are also differences in the effects of green and blue landscapes on physical and mental health. A study by Li et al. investigated the restorative effects of exposure to water, lawn, and topography landscape environments and showed significant differences between the three landscapes on participants’ blood pressure, brain activity, and mood states [59]. In addition, the effect of urban blue space on users’ health enhancement was prominent [68]. On the contrary, Zhao et al. [69] reported in their study that water features reduced the quality of restoration of vegetated pathway landscapes.
As a result of these studies, various types of landscapes have been found to affect the perceived degree of physical and mental health improvement. However, little evidence has been collected regarding specific landscape types in relation to the sympathetic nervous system and emotional states. In this study, we sought to discover the different restorative effects between vegetated landscapes in forests and water landscapes with vegetated elements. Therefore, diverse landscape settings including broadleaved forests, coniferous forests, mixed forests, topography forest landscapes, flower, open nature landscapes, and a variety of water features with vegetated elements were applied as experimental stimuli. This was carried out to improve our understanding of landscape types and elements using scientific and evidence-based research, aiming to identify the effects of these more resource-intensive natural environments in promoting health.
In recent years, virtual reality (VR) has been used a lot in improving health; nevertheless, VR offers a more realistic experience, but it also faces technical and logistical challenges. Numerous studies have explored and validated the differences between direct interviews and indirect exposures, finding that viewing pictures might, to some extent, substitute for on-site surveys [70,71,72]. Therefore, in this study, we used pictures as experimental stimuli, set the urban landscapes as opposed to restorative environments as negative stimuli, and used images to evaluate the physical and mental restorative effects during exposure to different types of landscape.
The aim of this study is to evaluate the differential impacts of purely vegetated forest landscapes and water landscapes with vegetated elements on stress reduction and mood improvement. We specifically address the following research questions: (1) Do purely vegetated landscapes (VL, included broadleaved, coniferous, mixed forests, and topography-influenced forest landscapes) and water landscapes (WL) featuring vegetative elements differ in their restorative effects on individuals? (2) Among these landscapes, do water features integrated with vegetation more effectively promote health than purely vegetated landscapes? This investigation seeks to enhance understanding of the health-promoting capacities of diverse natural environments through scientific and evidence-based research to inform decisions about landscape design and healthcare interventions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample size was determined using G*Power 3.1 (University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany), considering the effect size was set to 0.25 (large effect size: 0.4, medium effect size: 0.25, small effect size: 0.1), the significance level was set to 0.05, and the β power was set to 0.8, with an anticipated dropout rate of 20%, resulting in a total requirement of 35 participants. Previous research indicates that college students experience a lot of stress, anxiety, and depression, which could lead to insomnia [16], alcoholism [17,18], and substance abuse [19,20]. Therefore, 33 (male 15, female 18, age: 19.82 ± 0.88) college students from Henan Agriculture University and Henan University of Animal Husbandry and Economy participated in the experiment. Each participant consented to the study by signing a research informed consent form prior to involvement. All participants were healthy adults aged 18 and above and had no major cardiovascular diseases or mental disorders. Participants were told to abstain from consuming beverages, such as tea, coffee, or alcohol that may lead to mental excitement for 12 h before the experiment, and were prohibited from strenuous exercise for 24 h before the experiment.

2.2. Experimental Materials

Based on the analysis of the landscape types obtained from the previous studies [55,56,57,58,59,60], we collected photographs of each type of landscape through web portals such as Google, sohu, and photography. Two typical landscape types of forest environment were selected: vegetated landscape and water landscape (contains vegetated elements). This study categorized vegetated landscapes into coniferous, broadleaved, and mixed forest landscapes and other elements like trails, overlooks, and understory based on vegetation type and restorative quality. The water landscape was subdivided into waterfall, river, lake, creek, etc., according to the water flow pattern. Images of the urban landscape without natural elements were selected to be the control landscape. Finally, as shown in Appendix A, a total of three types of landscapes were selected: urban image (buildings, alleyways, dwellings, overpasses, traffic, underground, shopping malls, markets, industrial areas); forest landscape (broadleaf forest, coniferous forest, mixed forest, flowers, forest deck road, forest trail, overlook, understory, grassland); and water landscape (mountain waterfall, forest waterfall, river waterfall, canal, river, lake, brook, stream, creek).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Physiological Indices

In this investigation, we aimed to assess the effects of visual exposure to natural environments on two key outcomes: stress levels and autonomic nervous system activity. The outcomes and their respective measures are detailed below:
Stress Levels: To evaluate stress levels, an outcome indicative of the sympathetic nervous system’s response to psychological stress, we measured salivary alpha-amylase (SAA) activity. Increases in SAA activity are directly correlated with heightened sympathetic nervous system activity, serving as a biomarker for stress to quantify stress levels [73,74]. For this purpose, salivary samples were collected using sublingual reagent paper. Participants were instructed to moisten their mouths and place the reagent paper under their tongues for 60 s before it was placed into the monitoring device for analysis. The soaked test paper was then inserted into an SAA monitor (Nipro, Osaka, Japan) to record the SAA activity.
Autonomic Nervous System Activity: Heart rate variability (HRV) was assessed as a physiological marker to measure the activity of the autonomic nervous system, encompassing both its sympathetic and parasympathetic components. High-frequency (HF) power, indicative of parasympathetic nervous system activity, is typically elevated during states of relaxation. Conversely, the Low Frequency-to-High Frequency (LF/HF) ratio reflects the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system functions. To measure these indicators, the uBio Macpa (Biosense Creative, Seoul, Republic of Korea), a portable HRV-measuring instrument, was utilized. This device allows for the detection of the autonomic nervous system’s response to various environments through 2 min and 30 s of uninterrupted measurements.
By focusing on these outcomes (stress levels and autonomic nervous system activity), this study underscores the physiological responses to environmental stimuli. The use of SAA activity and HRV as outcome measures provides a nuanced understanding of how visual exposure to natural landscapes can influence psychological and physiological states.

2.3.2. Psychological Indices

In this study, we focused on evaluating several key psychological indices: mood, perceived stress, and self-esteem. To measure these indices, we employed various validated tools.
Mood: The primary index of mood was assessed using the Profile of Mood States (POMS), developed by Grove et al. in 1992 [75] and translated into a Chinese version [76]. POMS is a comprehensive tool that evaluates mood across seven sub-scales: Tension (T), Anxiety (A), Depression (D), Fatigue (F), Confusion (C), Vigor (V), and Esteem-Related Affect (E), encompassing a total of 40 questions [75]. To analyze the overall mood state changes in participants, we used the Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) score, calculated as follows:
TMD = T + A + D + F + C − (V + E) + 100
Perceived Stress: As a secondary outcome, we assessed participants’ perceived stress levels using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). This self-report tool, originally developed by Cohen et al. [77] and later translated into Chinese [78], consists of 10 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale from ‘never’ to ‘very often’ with total scores ranging from 0 to 40, with higher scores indicating greater perceived stress. Adequate reliability and validity were demonstrated [77,79].
Self-Esteem: Finally, self-esteem was measured using the Self-Esteem Scale (SES), developed by Rosenberg and translated into Chinese by Yu and Ji [80]. This scale is used to rate an individual’s overall feelings about self-worth and self-acceptance [81]. This assessment comprises 10 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale, featuring both positive and negative statements.

2.4. Experimental Design

The participants received information in a waiting room about the purpose and procedures of the experiment before the experiment. After obtaining informed consent for the experiment, each participant was subsequently sent to the experimental room. To acclimate participants to the experimental setting, they were asked to observe a gray background for 60 s in a calm state. Then, we collected participants’ demographic characteristics and they performed tests for heart rate variability (HRV), salivary α amylase (SAA), and psychological indicators as baseline measurements. Attention Restoration Theory (ART) proposes that exposure to forest landscape images for 3 to 5 min can yield comparable mental and physical restoration effects to direct forest visits, suggesting that even brief visual engagement with nature imagery can facilitate recovery from mental fatigue and stress. Therefore, after a 1 min rest following baseline measurements, each participant was separately exposed to urban landscape images, vegetated landscape images, or water landscape images for 180 s each, while maintaining a sitting position. Instantly following the completion of each viewing, the participants were asked to respond to HRV and SAA measurements and answer the POMS, SES, and PSS scales.
Additionally, salivary amylase shows diurnal variation, with a relatively steady period between 10:30 a.m. and 5 p.m. To mitigate the influence of food intake, participants were instructed not to consume any food one hour prior to testing. The experiment was therefore conducted between 10:30 a.m. and 5:00 p.m. The experimental design and measurement procedures are outlined in Figure 1.

2.5. Analysis and Statistics

Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS 26.0 (IBM corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Initially, participants’ demographic characteristics were examined through a descriptive analysis. Second, normality testing was performed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Then, for variables that satisfied normality, a paired t-test was used to compare baseline and post-tests of each landscape, while the variables not satisfying normality were examined using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Third, physiological and psychological differences between the three types of landscapes were further discerned by a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA with a post hoc (Bonferroni) test. As a result, the threshold for statistical significance was established at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Physiological Measurement

3.1.1. Salivary α Amylase (SAA)

The results between baseline and post-tests of salivary α amylase concentration for each landscape type are presented in Table 1. As shown, there was a significant decrease after viewing vegetated landscapes (VL, baseline: 21.85 ± 11.46, post: 18.06 ± 8.59, t = 2.491, p = 0.018, paired t-test) and water landscapes (WL, baseline: 21.85 ± 11.46, post: 17.55 ± 9.70, t = 2.211, p = 0.034, paired t-test). However, no significant differences were shown before and after viewing urban landscapes (UL, baseline: 21.85 ± 11.46, post: 23.42 ± 13.45, Z = −0.44, p = 0.661, Wilcoxon signed-rank).
Also, one-way repeated-measures ANOVA of salivary α amylase concentration showed that there was a significant difference in salivary α amylase levels across the three types of landscape. The main effect was F = 3.391, p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.202. Mauchly’s test indicated that χ2 = 11.414, p = 0.003. As illustrated in Table 2, post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant increase in salivary amylase levels after viewing vegetated landscapes (p = 0.027) and water landscapes (p = 0.039) compared to urban landscapes, while between vegetated (M ± SD = 18.06 ± 8.59) and water (M ± SD = 17.55 ± 9.70), there was a small and non-significant decrease (p = 1.000).

3.1.2. HRV

The results of High-frequency (HF) for each landscape by paired t-tests between baseline and post-tests are presented in Table 3. As shown, there was a significant increase after viewing vegetated landscapes (baseline: 6.90 ± 0.43, post: 7.03 ± 0.53, t = −2.293, p = 0.029) and water landscapes (baseline: 6.90 ± 0.43, post: 7.08 ± 0.49, t = −2.914, p = 0.006). However, no significant differences were shown before and after viewing urban landscapes (baseline: 6.90 ± 0.43, post: 6.89 ± 0.49, t = 0.374, p = 0.711).
Also, one-way repeated-measures ANOVA of HF showed that there was a significant difference across the three types of landscape. The main effect was F = 4.451, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.122. Mauchly’s test indicated that χ2 = 2.251, p = 0.324. As illustrated in Table 4, post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant increase in HF after viewing water landscapes compared to urban landscapes (p = 0.044), while between vegetated (M ± SD = 7.03 ± 0.53) and water (M ± SD = 7.08 ± 0.49), there was a small and non-significant increase (p = 1.000), as well as between vegetated and urban landscapes (p = 0.054).
The results of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test between baseline and post-tests of Low Frequency-to-High Frequency (LF/HF) for each landscape type are presented in Table 5. As shown, there was a significant decrease after viewing water landscapes (Z = −2.353, p = 0.019). However, no significant differences were shown before and after viewing urban landscapes (Z = −1.213, p = 0.225) and vegetated landscapes (Z = −0.269, p = 0.204).
Also, one-way repeated-measures ANOVA of LF/HF showed that there was a significant difference across the three types of landscape. The main effect was F = 6.559, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.170. Mauchly’s test indicated that χ2 = 5.479, p = 0.065. As illustrated in Table 6, post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant increase in LF/HF after viewing vegetated landscapes (p = 0.028) and water landscapes (p = 0.011) compared to urban landscapes, while between vegetated (M ± SD = 1.15 ± 0.10) and water (M ± SD = 1.12 ± 0.08), there was a small and non-significant decrease (p = 0.546).

3.2. Psychological Measurement

3.2.1. Profile of Mood Scale (POMS)

The differences between baseline and post-tests of Total Mood Disturbance scores of POMS for each landscape type are presented in Table 7. As shown, there was a significant decrease after viewing VL (t = 6.508, p < 0.001) and WL (t = 6.505, p < 0.001) than UL. And we can see in the sub-scales of POMS, significant positive changes were also demonstrated. The Tension (VL: t = 3.690, p = 0.001; WL: t = 5.066, p < 0.001), Anger (VL: Z = −3.888, p < 0.001; WL: Z = −3.932, p < 0.001), Confusion (VL: t = 3.516, p = 0.001; WL: t = 4.656, p < 0.001), Depression (VL: Z = −3.058, p = 0.002; WL: Z = −3.699, p < 0.001), Fatigue (VL: Z = −3.118, p = 0.002; WL: Z = −3.683, p < 0.001), and Vigor (VL: t= −2.082, p = 0.45; WL: t = −2.080, p = 0.046) were decreased after viewing VL and WL. And the Esteem (VL: Z = −2.289, p = 0.022) was increased after viewing VL. However, there were no significant differences between baseline and post-test of POMS for viewing ULs.
Also, repeated-measures ANOVA of POMS showed that there were significant differences in all POMS scores across the three types of landscape. The main effect of TMD was F = 13.725, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.470; Tension was F = 8.094, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.343; Confusion was F = 8.078, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.343; Vigor was F = 5.836, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.274; Anger was F = 12.929, p = 0.002; Depression was F = 17.5709, p < 0.001; Fatigue was F = 11.574, p = 0.003; Esteem was F = 7.582, p = 0.023. Post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant difference after viewing VL (TMD: p < 0.001; Tension: p = 0.020; Confusion: p = 0.004; Vigor: p =0.005; Anger: p =0.003; Depression: p = 0.026; Esteem: p = 0.006) and WL (TMD: p < 0.001; Tension: p = 0.001; Confusion: p = 0.001; Vigor: p = 0.012; Anger: p = 0.002; Depression: p = 0.003; Fatigue: p = 0.002; Esteem: p = 0.027) than UL. However, all of the POMS scores were different between vegetated and water landscapes, but there was no significance (Table 8).

3.2.2. SES

The results of SES for each landscape by paired t-tests between baseline and post-tests are presented in Table 9. As shown, there was a significant increase after viewing vegetated landscapes (t = −3.695, p = 0.001). However, no significant differences were shown before and after viewing urban landscapes (t = −1.428, p = 0.163) and water landscapes (t = −1.382, p = 0.177).
Also, one-way repeated-measures ANOVA of SES showed that there was a significant difference in SES across the three types of landscape. The main effect was F = 3.922, p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.202. Mauchly’s test indicated that χ2 = 0.843, p = 0.656. As illustrated in Table 10, post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant increase in SES after viewing VL (p = 0.036) compared to UL.

3.2.3. PSS

The results of HF for each landscape by paired t-tests between baseline and post-tests are presented in Table 11. As shown, there was a significant increase after viewing vegetated landscapes (t = 3.115, p = 0.004) and water landscapes (t = 2.487, p = 0.018). However, no significant differences were shown before and after viewing urban landscapes (t = −0.437, p = 0.665).
Also, one-way repeated-measures ANOVA of PSS showed that there was a significant difference in PSS across the three types of landscape. The main effect was F = 7.283, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.320. Mauchly’s test indicated that χ2 = 0.806, p = 0.668. As illustrated in Table 12, post hoc analyses using Bonferroni correction revealed a significant decrease in PSS after viewing VL (p = 0.004) and WL(p = 0.007) compared to UL, while this decrease was shown between vegetated (M ± SD = 16.73 ± 6.01) and water (M ± SD = 16.61 ± 5.55) landscapes, but there was no significance (p = 1.000).

4. Discussion

4.1. Physiological Effects

In this study, salivary α amylase (SAA) and heart rate variability (HRV) were employed as biomarkers to evaluate the influence of viewing forest images on the autonomic nervous system of college students. Under experimental conditions, compared to baseline measurements, both paired-sample t-tests and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests revealed that exposure to landscapes featuring forest vegetation and water scenarios, a notable decrease in salivary α amylase and a significant increase in parasympathetic activity post-exposure to these environmental settings. However, there was no significant difference between the vegetated and water landscapes.
Notably, LF/HF ratios declined significantly only after viewing water landscapes, while changes associated with vegetated landscapes were minor and not statistically significant. These outcomes are consistent with extensive prior research [35,54,67,82,83,84,85], suggesting a beneficial influence of forest landscapes on autonomic nervous system function. Furthermore, our study explored the comparative restorative impacts of vegetated versus water views. Water views demonstrated a marginally greater influence on physiological recovery than vegetated landscapes, though the disparity between the two was not statistically notable. This finding echoes other studies [56,86] indicating minimal variance in restorative effects among different natural environments. Patrik and Ulrika’s research [87] supports the notion that not only visual but also other sensory experiences shape the psychological response to green environments. In accordance with Stress Reduction Theory (SRT), humans are innately drawn to safe, natural settings abundant with trees, water, and varied vegetation, eliciting immediate positive responses [32,88]. Additional studies [89,90] have emphasized the stress-relieving properties of water features, which might be potentially linked to a preference for blue spaces [67].

4.2. Psychological Effects

Our study indicated that viewing both vegetated and water landscapes significantly ameliorated negative emotions, improved activity, and reduced stress. However, comparing the three types of landscape, the impact of water landscapes on emotion improvement and stress relief was marginally greater than that of vegetated landscapes, but the difference was not significant. In addition, self-esteem was significantly boosted only after exposure to the vegetated landscapes.
Tyrväinen et al. [85] noted that environments with distinct characteristics, such as densely vegetated forests and natural reserves, can alleviate stress and fatigue. This could be attributed to negative emotions being more responsive to environmental changes, which may swiftly and instinctively mitigate unpleasant emotions [90]. Blue spaces, in particular, are considered superior for mental restoration [91,92], possibly reflecting a general preference for such environments. Notably, only the ‘fatigue’ dimension showed a significant reduction post-exposure to water features. We hypothesize that the influence of water on self-perception may surpass that of green environments [93,94] and that aquatic environments like beaches and rivers are effective in diminishing negative emotions [95]. This observation was corroborated by PSS measurements.
Moreover, improvements in positive mood were significant in both vegetated and water landscapes. An increase in ‘Vigor’ was noted after exposure to water landscapes compared to vegetated ones, although this change was not significant. This could be attributed to the fact that the monotonous green of vegetation might appear unengaging and visually unappealing [96], whereas the diversity in water landscapes may evoke more pleasant feelings [27,31]. Conversely, vegetated landscapes were more effective than water landscapes in enhancing ‘self-esteem’, a finding exclusively noted in SES measurements. Zhang’s research [97] suggested that visual stimuli from forest landscapes, particularly the green color and tree shapes, are influential in boosting self-esteem. This finding underscores the unique contribution of greenery in forest landscapes to self-esteem restoration.

4.3. Limitations

This study has several limitations. Firstly, while it explored the restorative effects of in-forest vegetated and water landscapes, it did not examine the nine landscape elements within each type individually. In real-world settings, different types of environments may vary in their effectiveness for physical and mental health restoration. Not just visual elements but auditory elements and even olfactory elements may also affect recovery. Future research should expand on this aspect to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the restorative effects of forest environments and explore these nuances in more detail.
Secondly, the absence of a control group in this study may affect its internal validity and increase the potential for bias. Future studies would benefit from employing a randomized controlled trial design to mitigate these concerns and enhance the reliability of findings.
Thirdly, despite college students typically being a stressed population, in reality, other populations also experience a variety of stressors. In the future, the long-term effects of such indirect nature exposure on various populations such as seniors, manual laborers, stay-at-home moms, etc., could provide valuable insights into its efficacy as a sustainable intervention.
Lastly, images were selected as visual stimuli for this study, and although many previous studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of forest landscape images for stress relief, results may vary depending on the image accuracy, subject preference, and exposure order.
Future research could build on our findings, exploring the optimal types and qualities of images that most effectively convey the restorative properties of natural environments. This could include investigating aspects such as image resolution, realism, the inclusion of auditory elements, or even the use of emerging technologies like virtual reality (VR) to provide a more immersive and realistic comparison between vegetated landscapes and water-containing forest landscapes. Such investigations are crucial for identifying the visual and sensory characteristics that most significantly contribute to the restorative experience. Moreover, examining the long-term effects of such indirect nature exposure on various populations could provide valuable insights into its efficacy as a sustainable mental health intervention. Simulating realistic nature experiences could revolutionize approaches to mental health care, providing accessible, effective solutions for stress and mood management.

5. Conclusions

In this research, aiming to provide foundational insights into the benefits of visual exposure to different types of landscapes, we explored the physical and psychological effects resulting from visual exposure to different landscape environments. We specifically investigated whether predominantly vegetated landscapes and water features with vegetation elements exhibit distinct restorative effects on individuals and whether there are differences between the two in their capacity to foster physical and psychological recovery. Our findings illuminate the nuanced benefits of visual exposure to diverse landscape environments, particularly emphasizing the restorative potential of forest landscapes that integrate both vegetation and water elements. We observed that both types of landscapes (vegetated and water features with vegetation elements) contribute positively to human health, enhancing both physiological and psychological well-being. Notably, while water features presented a marginally higher restorative advantage, the difference in recovery effects between purely vegetated landscapes and those including water was not statistically significant. This outcome suggests that forest landscapes, regardless of the inclusion of water, are inherently beneficial, though the presence of water elements tends to augment emotional improvement and stress alleviation slightly more.
In the context of forest therapy, our research underscores the potential of forest landscapes, both with and without water features, as therapeutic environments for enhancing human well-being. This insight can guide the management and utilization of forest landscape resources, suggesting that the inclusion of diverse landscape features, including both vegetation and water, can be beneficial in landscape design and healthcare interventions. Urban planners, landscape architects, and public health policymakers can leverage these findings to design green spaces that optimize the health benefits for the community, particularly for stress-prone populations such as college students. Furthermore, this research could inspire the development of more targeted forest therapy programs that capitalize on the specific elements of forest landscapes that are most effective in promoting physical and mental recovery.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.S.; methodology, H.S., H.L. and Y.W.; investigation, H.L.; data curation, H.S. and H.L.; writing-original draft, H.S.; writing-review and editing, H.S., Y.W.; project administration and supervision, W.-S.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants in the experiment, and also thank Xiangnan Lin from Chungbuk National University for his donation of materials for experiment and assistance during the preparation of this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Urban landscape images.
Figure A1. Urban landscape images.
Forests 15 00498 g0a1
Figure A2. Vegetated landscape images.
Figure A2. Vegetated landscape images.
Forests 15 00498 g0a2
Figure A3. Water landscape images.
Figure A3. Water landscape images.
Forests 15 00498 g0a3

References

  1. The World Bank. Urban Development. Available online: https://www.worldbank.org/en/topic/urbandevelopment/overview (accessed on 22 October 2023).
  2. Zhou, L.M.; Dickinson, R.E.; Tian, Y.H.; Fang, J.; Li, Q.; Kaufmann, R.K.; Tucker, C.J.; Myneni, R.B. Evidence for a significant urbanization effect on climate in China. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 9540–9544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Yang, X.; Hou, Y.; Chen, B. Observed surface warming induced by urbanization in east China. J. Geophys. Res. 2011, 116, D14113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Oke, T.R. City size and the urban heat island. Atmos. Environ. 1973, 7, 769–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Oke, T. The energetic basis of the urban heat island. Q. J. Roy. Meteorol. Soc. 1982, 108, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Liu, J.; Diamond, J. China’s environment in a globalizing world. Nature 2005, 435, 1179–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Shao, M.; Tang, X.; Zhang, Y.; Li, W. City clusters in China: Air and surface water pollution. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2006, 4, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bayraktar, H.; Turalioğlu, F.S.; Tuncel, G. Average mass concentrations of TSP, PM10 and PM2.5 in Erzurum urban atmosphere, Turkey. Stoch. Environ. Res. Risk Assess 2010, 24, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Fang, J.; Piao, S.; Field, C.B.; Pan, Y.; Guo, Q.; Zhou, L.; Peng, C.; Tao, S. Increasing net primary production in China from 1982 to 1999. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2003, 1, 293–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Imhoff, M.L.; Bounoua, L.; DeFries, R.; Lawrence, W.T.; Stutzer, D.; Tucker, C.J.; Ricketts, T. The consequences of urban land transformation on net primary productivity in the United States. Remote Sens. Environ. 2004, 89, 434–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Milesi, C.; Elvidge, C.D.; Nemani, R.R.; Running, S.W. Assessing the impact of urban land development on net primary productivity in the southeastern United States. Remote Sens. Environ. 2003, 86, 401–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pei, F.; Li, X.; Liu, X.; Wang, S.; He, Z. Assessing the differences in net primary productivity between pre- and post-urban land development in China. Agric. For. Meteorol. 2013, 171–172, 174–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Haas, J.; Ban, Y. Urban growth and environmental impacts in Jing-Jin-Ji, the Yangtze, River Delta and the Pearl River Delta. Int. J. Appl. Earth Obs. Geoinf. 2014, 30, 42–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kaplan, R. The nature of the view from home psychological benefits. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 507–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kim, J.; Kil, N.; Holland, S.; Middleton, W.K. The effect of visual and auditory coherence on perceptions of tranquility after simulated nature experiences. Ecopsychology 2017, 9, 182–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Eisenberg, D.; Gollust, S.E.; Golberstein, E.; Hefner, J.L. Prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and suicidality among university students. Am. J. Orthopsychiatr. 2007, 77, 534–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Geisner, I.; Mallett, K.; Kilmer, J.R. An examination of depressive symptoms and drinking patterns in first year college students. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 2012, 33, 280–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Pedrelli, P.; Borsari, B.; Lipson, S.K.; Heinze, J.E.; Eisenberg, D. Gender differences in the relationships among major depressive disorder, heavy alcohol use, and mental health treatment engagement among college students. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2016, 77, 620–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Halperin, A.C.; Smith, S.S.; Heiligenstein, E.; Brown, D.; Fleming, M.F. Cigarette smoking and associated health risks among students at five universities. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2010, 12, 96–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Keith, D.; Hart, C.; McNeil, M.; Silver, R.; Goodwin, R. Frequent marijuana use, binge drinking and mental health problems among undergraduates. Am. J. Addict. 2015, 24, 499–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Keyes, C.L.; Eisenberg, D.; Perry, G.S.; Dube, S.R.; Kroenke, K.; Dhingra, S.S. The relationship of level of positive mental health with current mental disorders in predicting suicidal behavior and academic impairment in college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2012, 60, 126–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Regehr, C.; Glancy, D.; Pitts, A. Interventions to reduce stress in university students: A review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 148, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Frumkin, H. Beyond toxicity human health and the natural environment. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2001, 20, 234–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. UN-Habitat. State of the World’s Cities: Bridging the Urban Divide. Available online: http://www.unhabitat.org/documents/SOWC10/R7.pdf (accessed on 20 October 2023).
  25. James, P.; Banay, R.F.; Hart, J.E.; Laden, F. A Review of the Health Benefits of Greenness. Curr. Epidemiol. Rep. 2015, 2, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ulrich, R. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 1984, 224, 420–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Government of Canada. A New Perspective on the Health of Canadians. Available online: https://www.canada.ca/en/public-health/services/health-promotion/population-health/a-new-perspective-on-health-canadians.html (accessed on 24 February 2024).
  29. Gesler, W.M. Therapeutic Landscapes: Theory and a Case Study of Epidauros, Greece. Environ. Plan. D Soc. Space 1993, 11, 171–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Agnieszka, O.G.; Angelia, S.; Nicolas, E. Revised Contemplative Landscape Model (CLM): A reliable and valid evaluation tool for mental health-promoting urban green spaces. Urban For. Urban Green. 2023, 86, 128016. [Google Scholar]
  31. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In Behavior and the Natural Environment; Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F., Eds.; Human Behavior and Environment (Advances in Theory and Research); Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1983; Volume 6, pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  33. Markevych, I.; Schoierer, J.; Hartig, T.; Chudnovsky, A.; Hystad, P.; Dzhambov, A.M.; de Vries, S.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Brauer, M.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; et al. Exploring pathways linking greenspace to health: Theoretical and methodological guidance. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 301–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; de Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and Health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Miles, M.A. Effects of environmental simulations and television on blood donor stress. J. Arch. Plan. Res. 2003, 20, 38–47. [Google Scholar]
  36. Joye, Y.; van den Berg, A.E. Restorative environments. In Environmental Psychology: An Introduction; Van den Berg, A.E., De Groot, J., Steg, L., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 57–66. [Google Scholar]
  37. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Sato, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological Effects of Shinrin-yoku (Taking in the Atmosphere of the Forest)—Using Salivary Cortisol and Cerebral Activity as Indicators&mdash. J. Physiol. Anthr. 2007, 26, 123–128. [Google Scholar]
  38. Li, Q.; Morimoto, K.; Kobayashi, M.; Inagaki, H.; Katsumata, M.; Hirata, Y.; Hirata, K.; Shimizu, T.; Li, Y.J.; Wakayama, Y.; et al. A forest bathing trip increases human natural killer activity and expression of anti-cancer proteins in female subjects. J. Biol. Regul. Homeost. Agents 2008, 22, 45. [Google Scholar]
  39. Sung, J.; Woo, J.; Kim, W.; Lim, S.; Chung, E.J. The effect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest therapy” program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive patients. Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 2012, 34, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Morita, E.; Fukuda, S.; Nagano, J.; Hamajima, N.; Yamamoto, H.; Iwai, Y.; Nakashima, T.; Ohira, H.; Shirakawa, T.J.P.H. Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health 2007, 121, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Shin, W.S. The influence of forest view through a window on job satisfaction and job stress. Source? Scand. J. For. Res. 2007, 22, 248–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia: The Human Bond with Other Species; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  43. Juyoung, L.; Bumjin, P.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scand. J. For. Res. 2009, 24, 227–234. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lee, J.; Park, B.-J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of forest bathing on physiological and psychological responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health 2011, 125, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Zhang, Z.; Wang, P.; Gao, Y.; Ye, B. Current Development Status of Forest Therapy in China. Healthcare 2020, 8, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Li, Q.; Morimoto, K.; Kobayashi, M.; Inagaki, H.; Katsumata, M.; Hirata, Y.; Hirata, K.; Suzuki, H.; Li, Y.J.; Wakayama, Y.; et al. Visiting a Forest, but Not a City, Increases Human Natural Killer Activity and Expression of Anti-Cancer Proteins. Int. J. Immunopathol. Pharmacol. 2008, 21, 117–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Yu, C.P.; Lin, C.M.; Tsai, M.J.; Tsai, Y.C.; Chen, C.Y. Effects of short forest bathing program on autonomic nervous system activity and mood states in middle-aged and elderly individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Park, B.J.; Kasetani, T.; Morikawa, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of forest recreation in a young conifer forest in Hinokage Town, Japan. Silva. Fenn. 2009, 43, 291–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of a walk in urban parks in fall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 14216–14228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Herzog, T.R.; Black, A.M.; Fountaine, K.A.; Knotts, D.J. Reflection and attentional recovery as two distinctive benefits of restorative environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1997, 17, 165–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jang, C.; Koo, C. Effects of after-school forest healing program activities on infant’s pro-social behavior and self-efficacy. Korean J. Environ. Ecol. 2017, 31, 595–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Yu, Y.; Lee, Y.J.; Kim, J.Y.; Yoon, S.B.; Shin, C.S. Effects of forest therapy camp on quality of life and stress in postmenopausal women. For. Sci. Technol. 2016, 12, 125–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Laumann, K.; Gärling, T.; Stormark, K.M. Rating scale measures of restorative components of environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hartig, T.; Evans, G.W.; Jamner, L.D.; Davis, D.S.; Gärling, T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Balling, J.D.; Falk, J.H. Development of Visual Preference for Natural Environments. Environ. Behav. 2016, 14, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Heerwagen, J.H.; Orians, G.H. Humans, Habitats, and Aesthetics; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  57. Van den Berg, A.E.; Koole, S.L.; Van der Wulp, N.Y. Environmental preference and restoration: (How) are they related? J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Nordh, H.; Alalouch, C.; Hartig, T. Assessing restorative components of small urban parks using conjoint methodology. Urban For. Urban Gree. 2011, 10, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Deng, L.; Li, X.; Luo, H.; Fu, E.K.; Ma, J.; Sun, L.X.; Huang, Z.; Cai, S.Z.; Jia, Y. Empirical Study of Landscape Types, Landscape Elements and Landscape Components of the Urban Park Promoting Physiological and Psychological Restoration. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 48, 126488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. An, K.W.; Kim, E.I.; Joen, K.S.; Setsu, T. Effects of Forest Stand Density on Human’s Physiophychological Changes. J. Fac. Agric. Kyushu Univ. 2004, 49, 283–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Stigsdotter, U.K.; Corazon, S.S.; Sidenius, U.; Refshauge, A.D.; Grahn, P. Forest design for mental health promotion: Using perceived sensory dimensions to elicit restorative responses. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 160, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Chiang, Y.C.; Li, D.; Jane, H.A. Wild or Tended Nature? The Effects of Landscape Location and Vegetation Density on Physiological and Psychological Responses. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 167, 72–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hoyle, H.; Hitchmough, J.; Jorgensen, A. All about the “wow factor”? The relationships between aesthetics, restorative effect and perceived biodiversity in designed urban planting. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 164, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. de Bell, S.; Hilary, G.; Stuart, J.; Piran, W. The importance of nature in mediating social and psychological benefits associated with visits to freshwater blue space. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 167, 118–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Tang, I.C.; Tsai, Y.P.; Lin, Y.J.; Chen, J.H.; Hsieh, C.H.; Hung, S.H.; Sullivan, W.C.; Tang, H.F.; Chang, C.Y. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to analyze brain region activity when viewing landscape. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 162, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wang, X.; Shi, Y.; Zhang, B.; Chiang, Y. The influence of forest resting environments on stress using virtual reality. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Han, K.T. Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and restorativeness. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 529–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Völker, S.; Kistemann, T. Developing the urban blue: Comparative health responses to blue and green urban open spaces in Germany. Health Place 2015, 35, 196–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Zhao, J.W.; Wang, X.X. Effects of Landscape Types and Complexity Along Path on Mental Restoration. Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2022, 15, 391–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Xiang, Y.; Liang, H.; Fang, X.; Chen, Y.; Xu, N.; Hu, M.; Chen, Q.; Mu, S.; Hedblom, M.; Qiu, L. The comparisons of on-site and off-site applications in surveys on perception of and preference for urban green spaces: Which approach is more reliable? Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 58, 126961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Dupont, L.; Ooms, K.; Duchowski, A.T.; Antrop, M.; Van Eetvelde, V. Investigating the visual exploration of the rural-urban gradient using eye-tracking. Spat. Cogn. Comput. 2016, 17, 65–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Dupont, L.; Ooms, K.; Antrop, M.; Van Eetvelde, V. Testing the validity of a saliency-based method for visual assessment of constructions in the landscape. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 167, 325–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Yamaguchi, M.; Deguchi, M.; Miyazaki, Y. The effects of exercise in forest and urban environments on sympathetic nervous activity of normal young adults. J. Int. Med. Res. 2006, 34, 152–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Nater, U.M.; Rohleder, N.; Gaab, J.; Berger, S.; Jud, A.; Kirschbaum, C.; Ehlert, U. Human salivary alpha-amylase reactivity in a psychosocial stress paradigm. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2005, 55, 333–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Grove, J.R.; Prapaessis, H. Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of an abbreviated Profile of Mood States. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1992, 23, 93–109. [Google Scholar]
  76. Zhu, B.L. Brief Introduction of POMS Scale and its Model for China. J. Tianjin Inst. Phys. Educ. 1995, 10, 35–37. [Google Scholar]
  77. Cohen, S.; Kamarch, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Wang, Z.; Chen, J.; Boyd, J.E.; Zhang, H.Y.; Jia, X.Z.; Qiu, J.; Xiao, Z.P. Psychometric Properties of the Chinese Version of the Perceived Stress Scale in Policewomen. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, 28610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image, 1st ed.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  80. Yu, X.; Ji, F.Y. The Self Esteem Scale. Chin. Ment. Health J. 1999, Z1, 318–320. [Google Scholar]
  81. Komori, T.; Mitsui, M.; Togashi, K.; Matsui, J.; Kato, T.; Uei, D.; Shibayama, A.; Yamato, K.; Okumura, H.; Kinoshita, F. Relaxation effect of a 2-Hour walk in Kumano-Kodo Forest. J. Neurol. Neurosci. 2017, 8, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Park, B.-J.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurements. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 113, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Birenboim, A.; Dijst, M.; Ettema, D.; De Kruijf, J.; De Leeuw, G.; Dogterom, N. The utilization of immersive virtual environments for the investigation of environmental preferences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2019, 189, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Mohamed, E.; Shao, Y.H.; Liu, B.Y. Benefits of Indirect Contact with Nature on the Physiopsychological Well-Being of Elderly People. Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2021, 14, 227–241. [Google Scholar]
  85. Tyrväinen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Ulrich, R.S. Visual Landscapes and Psychological Well-Being. Landsc. Res. 1979, 4, 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Patrik, K.; Ulrika, K.S. The relation between perceived sensory dimensions of urban green space and stress restoration. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2010, 94, 264–275. [Google Scholar]
  88. Chen, R.; Gao, Y.; Zhang, R.X.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, W.K.; Meng, H.; Zhang, T. How Does the Experience of Forest Recreation Spaces in Different Seasons Affect the Physical and Mental Recovery of Users? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 2357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Wu, M.Y.; Gao, Y.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, B.; Meng, H.; Zhang, W.K.; Zhang, T. How Do Repeated Viewings in Forest Landscapes Influence Young People’s Visual Behaviors and Cognitive Evaluations? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 4753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Russell, J.A. A circumplex model of affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 1161–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. White, M.; Smith, A.; Humphryes, K.; Pahl, S.; Snelling, D.; Depledge, M. Blue space: The importance of water for preference, affect, and restorativeness ratings of natural and built scenes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 482–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Berman, M.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The cognitive benefits for interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Wheeler, B.W.; White, M.; Stahl-Timmins, W.; Depledge, M.H. Does living by the coast improve health and wellbeing? Health Place 2012, 18, 1198–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. White, M.P.; Alcock, I.; Wheeler, B.W.; Depledge, M.H. Coastal proximity, health and well-being: Results from a longitudinal panel survey. Health Place 2013, 23, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Hinds, J.; Sparks, P. The affective quality of human-natural environment relationships. Evol. Psychol. 2011, 9, 451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Wandersee, J.H.; Schussler, E.E. Preventing Plant Blindness. Am. Biol. Teach. 1999, 61, 82–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Zhang, D.X.; Jin, X.H.; Wang, L.N.; Jin, Y.H. Y.H. Form and color visual perception in green exercise: Positive effects on attention, mood, and self-esteem. J. Environ. Psychol. 2023, 88, 102028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental process chart. (A): experimental timeline; (B): exposure to visual stimuli; (C): measurement of HRV; (D): measurement of SAA; (E): completion of psychological questionnaires.
Figure 1. Experimental process chart. (A): experimental timeline; (B): exposure to visual stimuli; (C): measurement of HRV; (D): measurement of SAA; (E): completion of psychological questionnaires.
Forests 15 00498 g001
Table 1. Effect of viewing landscapes on salivary α amylase concentration.
Table 1. Effect of viewing landscapes on salivary α amylase concentration.
MeanSDtp
VLbaseline21.8511.462.4910.018 *
post18.068.59
WLbaseline21.8511.462.2110.034 *
post17.559.70
MeanSDZp
ULbaseline21.8511.46−0.44b0.661
post23.4213.45
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape; b: the value based on negative rank. N = 33. * p < 0.05.
Table 2. A comparison of three landscape types in salivary α amylase concentration.
Table 2. A comparison of three landscape types in salivary α amylase concentration.
M ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
UL23.42 ± 13.453.931b0.030 *0.202UL > VL,WL
VL17.45 ± 8.27
WL17.09 ± 9.60
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape; b: exact statistic. N = 33. * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Effect of viewing landscapes on HF.
Table 3. Effect of viewing landscapes on HF.
MeanSDtp
ULbaseline6.900.430.3740.711
post6.890.49
VLbaseline6.900.43−2.2930.029 *
post7.030.53
WLbaseline6.900.43−2.9140.006 **
post7.080.49
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. A comparison of three landscape types in HF.
Table 4. A comparison of three landscape types in HF.
M ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
UL6.89 ± 0.494.4510.015 *0.122UL < WL
VL7.03 ± 0.53
WL7.08 ± 0.49
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33. * p < 0.05.
Table 5. Effect of viewing landscapes on LF/HF.
Table 5. Effect of viewing landscapes on LF/HF.
MeanSDZp
ULbaseline1.160.09−1.213b0.225
post1.180.09
VLbaseline1.160.09−0.269c0.204
post1.150.10
WLbaseline1.160.09−2.353b0.019 *
post1.120.08
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33; b: the value based on negative rank; c: the value based on positive rank. * p < 0.05.
Table 6. A comparison of three landscape types in LF/HF.
Table 6. A comparison of three landscape types in LF/HF.
M ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
UL1.18 ± 0.096.5590.003 **0.170UL > VL,WL
VL1.15 ± 0.10
WL1.12 ± 0.08
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33. ** p < 0.01.
Table 7. Effect of viewing landscapes on POMS.
Table 7. Effect of viewing landscapes on POMS.
MeanSDtp
TMDULbaseline111.0615.120.8220.417
post109.3314.58
VLbaseline111.0615.126.508<0.001 ***
post100.2115.38
WLbaseline111.0615.126.505<0.001 ***
post98.1514.70
TensionULbaseline6.672.821.2260.229
post6.062.96
VLbaseline6.672.823.6900.001 **
post4.703.72
WLbaseline6.672.825.066<0.001 ***
post4.153.05
VigorULbaseline10.093.010.7630.451
post9.484.14
VLbaseline10.093.01−2.0820.045 *
post11.362.99
WLbaseline10.093.01−2.0800.046 *
post11.583.50
ConfusionULbaseline6.303.010.8090.425
post5.972.51
VLbaseline6.303.013.5160.001 **
post4.732.88
WLbaseline6.303.014.656<0.001 ***
post4.333.05
MeanSDZp
AngerULbaseline4.633.28−1.090b0.056
post4.003.39
VLbaseline4.633.28−3.888b<0.001 ***
post2.673.27
WLbaseline4.633.283.932b<0.001 ***
post2.452.56
FighterULbaseline6.063.39−1.163b0.245
post5.553.20
VLbaseline6.063.39−3.118b0.002 **
post4.583.32
WLbaseline6.063.39−3.683b<0.001 ***
post3.912.84
DepressionULbaseline4.243.83−0.640b0.522
post3.823.09
VLbaseline4.243.83−3.058b0.002 **
post2.703.04
WLbaseline4.243.83−3.699b<0.001 ***
post2.372.85
EsteemULbaseline6.762.24−0.680b0.497
post5.972.51
VLbaseline6.762.24−2.289c0.022 *
post4.732.88
WLbaseline6.762.24−1.953c0.051
post4.333.05
TMD: Total Mood Disturbance. UL= urban landscape; VL= vegetated landscape; WL= water landscape; b: the value based on negative rank; c: the value based on positive rank. N = 33. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Table 8. A comparison of three landscape types in POMS scores.
Table 8. A comparison of three landscape types in POMS scores.
Sub-ScaleTypeM ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
TMDUL109.33 ± 14.5813.725b<0.001 ***0.470UL > VL,
WL
VL100.21 ± 15.38
WL98.15 ± 14.70
TensionUL6.06 ± 2.968.094b0.001 **0.343UL > VL,
WL
VL4.70 ± 3.72
WL4.15 ± 3.05
ConfusionUL5.97 ± 2.518.078b0.002 **0.343UL > VL,
WL
VL4.73 ± 2.88
WL4.33 ± 3.05
VigorUL9.48 ± 4.415.836b0.007 **0.274UL < VL,
WL
VL11.36 ± 2.99
WL11.58 ± 3.56
AngerUL4.00 ± 3.397.368b0.002 **0.322UL > VL,
WL
VL2.67 ± 3.28
WL2.45 ± 2.56
DepressionUL3.82 ± 3.076.215b0.005 **0.286UL > VL,
WL
VL2.70 ± 3.04
WL2.36 ± 2.85
FatigueUL5.55 ± 3.207.420b0.002 **0.324UL > WL
VL4.58 ± 3.32
WL3.91 ± 2.84
EsteemUL6.58 ± 2.747.3890.001 **0.188UL < VL,
WL
VL7.79 ± 2.07
WL7.48 ± 2.15
UL = urban landscape; VL = vegetated landscape; WL = water landscape; b: exact statistic. ** p < 0.01 *** p < 0.001.
Table 9. Effect of viewing landscapes on SES score.
Table 9. Effect of viewing landscapes on SES score.
MeanSDtp
ULbaseline27.483.58−1.4280.163
post28.123.29
VLbaseline27.483.58−3.6950.001 **
post29.063.38
WLbaseline27.483.58−1.3820.177
post28.212.48
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33. ** p < 0.01.
Table 10. A comparison of three landscape types in SES score.
Table 10. A comparison of three landscape types in SES score.
M ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
UL28.12 ± 3.293.922b0.030 *0.202UL < VL
VL29.06 ± 3.38
WL28.21 ± 2.48
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape; b: exact statistic. N = 33. * p < 0.05.
Table 11. Effect of viewing landscapes on PSS score.
Table 11. Effect of viewing landscapes on PSS score.
MeanSDtp
ULbaseline18.185.80−0.4370.665
post18.365.95
VLbaseline18.185.803.1150.004 **
post16.736.01
WLbaseline18.185.802.4870.018 *
post16.615.55
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape. N = 33. ** p < 0.01. * p < 0.05.
Table 12. A comparison of three landscape types in PSS score.
Table 12. A comparison of three landscape types in PSS score.
M ± SDFpηp2Bonferroni
UL18.36 ± 5.957.283b0.003 **0.320UL > VL WL,
VL16.73 ± 6.01
WL16.61 ± 5.55
UL: urban landscape; VL: vegetated landscape; WL: water landscape; b: exact statistic. N = 33. ** p < 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shi, H.; Luo, H.; Wei, Y.; Shin, W.-S. The Influence of Different Forest Landscapes on Physiological and Psychological Recovery. Forests 2024, 15, 498. https://doi.org/10.3390/f15030498

AMA Style

Shi H, Luo H, Wei Y, Shin W-S. The Influence of Different Forest Landscapes on Physiological and Psychological Recovery. Forests. 2024; 15(3):498. https://doi.org/10.3390/f15030498

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shi, Hui, Han Luo, Yawei Wei, and Won-Sop Shin. 2024. "The Influence of Different Forest Landscapes on Physiological and Psychological Recovery" Forests 15, no. 3: 498. https://doi.org/10.3390/f15030498

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop