Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Previous Research on the Use of Persuasive Messages to Promote PEB
1.2. Potential Moderator Variables
1.3. The Present Study
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design
2.3. Materials
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Analyses
3.2. Analysis of the Main Hypotheses
3.3. Analysis of the Research Questions
4. Discussion
4.1. Implications
4.2. Limitations and Future Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Message | Mego | Meco | p |
---|---|---|---|
It is getting hot! In the winter of 2016 there were a number of days when it was 15 to 21 degrees Celsius warmer than normal. As a result, the North Pole was warmed. This also thawed the permafrost that surrounds the seed vault in Svalbard, Norway. Seeds from all over the world are stored in this safe. Nicknamed the ′Last Judgment′, it is designed to ensure that the planet′s agriculture could survive any disaster. Due to the partial thawing of the frozen ground, water ran into the seed stores—only ten years after it was set up, it was threatened by climate change′ (Wallache-Wells, 2017). Think about it: how big do you estimate the collective loss would be if the seed vault was destroyed by climate change? | 3.71 | 6.00 | <0.001 |
The proportion of carbon dioxide in the air will increase due to climate change. One consequence is as follows: The oceans are acidifying: about one quarter of CO2 emitted is absorbed by the oceans, and their pH value is changed as a result. So far, this has already reduced the pH value of the oceans by 0.1. If the acidity of the oceans changes, this affects the ability of crustaceans to form shells or of corals to form reefs (see WMO, 2019, State of the Climate 2018). The sea is acidifying. The conditions are becoming more hostile to life (Global2000—https://www.global2000.at/haben-des-klimawandels). Imagine the seas without shellfish and coral. How will this influence the ecosystem? | 3.71 | 6.64 | <0.001 |
Harmful insects can become a nuisance due to climate change. ′If you include harm of the bark beetle in the assessment of climate change effects on the spruce, there is a clear increase in very poorly suited forest areas. This is because the bark beetle favors a warmer climate, while at the same time, the spruce suffers from climatic stress and is therefore more prone to damage (WWF Klimastudie der Bundesforste AT, https://www.bundesforste.at/fileadmin/publikationen/studien/Klimastudie_WWF.pdf) | 4.11 | 6.32 | <0.001 |
Melting ice: all over the world, ice is melting on the poles and glaciers. This is most visible in the Arctic sea ice. Expansion at the summer minimum in September is about 28% lower than average. The inland glaciers in Greenland are also melting. Since 2002, they have lost 3.600 billion tons of ice. In the last 10 years, the Swiss glaciers have lost half of their volume (see WMO (2019): Declaration on the state of the global climate). (Global2000—https://www.global2000.at/haben-des-klimawandels). Think about the ecological consequences of the disappearance of glaciers. | 4.18 | 6.64 | <0.001 |
The ice is melting! Every ticket for flights from New York to London and back costs additional three-square meters of Arctic ice’ (Wallache-Wells, 2017; according to Vizcarra. Stroeve & Notz, 2016). One consequence of this is that ‘climate change could cause polar bears to become extinct by 2100. Due to rising temperatures, animals will find less food (Zdfheute/nature climate change, July 2020). Imagine this influence on the ecosystem and the consequences for polar bear. | 4.35 | 6.57 | <0.001 |
Sometimes, just do not buy anything. Why do minimalists have less money worries? They consume less. They who spend little, save a lot. “Thrift is a good income”, said Cicero over 2000 years ago. Owning things usually entails operating and maintenance costs. Disposal also has to be paid for at some point. That adds up in the end and is usually not considered when buying. (https://myfoodmyfuture.com/minimalismus/#Geld_sparen). Less is more—for your wallet! | 6.25 | 4.26 | <0.001 |
Water is our most important food. It is obvious that we need to spend money on it. However, watch out: many people spend way too much money on their daily water. The cheapest mineral waters cost just under 15 cents per liter. Branded mineral waters are already around 70 cents. The scale is open at the top. Designer waters such as Voss sometimes cost 1.80 euros for half a liter. A liter of tap water costs less than 0.2 cents. People who stop buying water in plastic bottles do not only stop having to lug boxes and return bottles (and are, by the way, also kind to the environment), but they also save a lot of money (https://utopia.de/ratgeber/nachhaltiger-bio-konsum-mit-wenig-geld/). If you only replace buying 1 L of drinking water per day (70 cents) by drinking tap water (0.2 cents), you save around 19 euros a month. That is a lot of money every year! | 6.21 | 4.64 | <0.001 |
The ice is melting! There are diseases in the Arctic ice that have not circulated in our air for millions of years—some of them were already around before humans existed. That means that our immune system would have no idea how it should ward off these prehistoric diseases, if they should be released again. In the Arctic, however, there are also terrifying germs from more recent times. In Alaska, researchers have found remains of the 1918 flu, which infected 500 million people and killed up to 100 million people—that’s five percent of the world’s population at the time and almost six times as many as were killed in the First World War (...). In May the BBC reported that scientists also suspected the presence of smallpox and the bubonic plague in Siberian ice’ (Wallache-Wells, 2017). Imagine what restrictions the release of the diseases in the Arctic ice could place on your daily life. | 6.00 | 5.14 | <0.05 |
Walking, taking the stairs instead of the elevator and cycling to work are not only good for the environment but primary pays off for you personally. ′The advantages are: prevention of back pain and cardiovascular diseases, diabetes, arthrosis, osteoporosis, depression and others; prevention of obesity or helping you lose weight; increases self-confidence and well-being; improves performance; promotes good sleep; saves money because cars are no longer needed (Https://myfoodmyfuture.com/minimalismus/#Tipps_Minimalismus_entruempeln). | 6.64 | 3.96 | <0.001 |
The proportion of carbon dioxide in the air will increase due to climate change. One consequence is Air that you can′t breathe—our lungs need oxygen. However, this is only a fraction of what we breathe. For example, the proportion of carbon dioxide in the air is increasing. It has just risen above 400 parts per million (ppm). Estimates based on current trends suggest that it will be at 1000 ppm by the end of the century. That concentration would, compared with the air that we breathe today, lead to a 21 percent decline in people′s cognitive abilities (Wallache-Wells, 2017). Think about how air that you can′t breathe would feel. | 5.96 | 5.54 | =0.09 |
Appendix B
“In the Last 5 days, I have…” (Items) | r pre | r post |
---|---|---|
used detergents in a smaller dose, than manufacturers recommended (e.g., while dishwashing, cleaning, …). | 0.59 | 0.64 |
saved water in my household for primary environmental reasons (e.g., short showering instead of taking a bath). | 0.58 | 0.58 |
taken recyclable materials to the adequate recycling stations (e.g., paper, glass, aluminum). | 0.40 | 0.39 |
forgone eating meat, for environmental reasons. | 0.52 | 0.54 |
preferred regionally grown food for primary environmental reasons. | 0.62 | 0.72 |
mainly chosen environmentally friendly transportation alternatives (e.g., train or bicycle instead of car). | 0.43 | 0.39 |
chosen stairs over elevators to save energy for environmental reasons. | 0.51 | 0.58 |
taken a bag with me for shopping to avoid taking a plastic bag in shop. | 0.43 | 0.48 |
avoided plastic that is used only once while shopping (e.g., food packages). | 0.60 | 0.63 |
avoided buying drinks in cans or plastic bottles for environmental reasons. | 0.67 | 0.65 |
payed attention to labels for ecological products (on food, clothes, …). | 0.62 | 0.69 |
turned off lights and power switches when they are not needed to avoid unnecessary energy use. | 0.43 | 0.38 |
References
- IPBES. Summary for Policymakers of the Global Assessment Report on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services; Díaz, S., Settele, J., Brondízio, E.S., Ngo, H.T., Guèze, M., Agard, J., Arneth, A., Balvanera, P., Brauman, K.A., Butchart, S.H.M., et al., Eds.; IPBES secretariat: Bonn, Germany, 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steg, L.; Vlek, C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abrahamse, W.; Steg, L.; Vlek, C.; Rothengatter, T. A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 273–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friese, S.; Reisch, L. Special Session Summary Portrayals of Environmental Myths and Images in European Adverts and the News Media. In E—European Advances in Consumer Research; Groeppel-Klien, A., Esch, F.R., Eds.; Association for Consumer Research: Provo, UT, USA, 2001; Volume 5, pp. 164–166. [Google Scholar]
- Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L.; Geller, E.S.; Lehman, P.K.; Postmes, T. Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2013, 3, 413–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Dominicis, S.; Schultz, P.; Bonaiuto, M. Protecting the environment for self-interested reasons: Altruism is not the only pathway to sustainability. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Herziger, A.; Berkessel, J.; Steinnes, K.K. Wean off green: On the (in) effectiveness of biospheric appeals for consumption curtailment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 101415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S.H. Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 19–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, W.P. Inclusion with nature: The Psychology of human-nature relations. In Psychology of Sustainable Development; Schmuck, P., Schultz, W.P., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2002; pp. 61–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carfora, V.; Catellani, P.; Caso, D.; Conner, M. How to reduce red and processed meat consumption by daily text messages targeting environment or health benefits. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 65, 101319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carfora, V.; Bertolotti, M.; Catellani, P. Informational and emotional daily messages to reduce red and processed meat consumption. Appetite 2019, 141, 104331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Lange, P.A.; Bekkers, R.; Schuyt, T.N.; Vugt, M.V. From games to giving: Social value orientation predicts donations to noble causes. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 29, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gärling, T.; Fujii, S.; Gärling, A.; Jakobsson, C. Moderating effects of social value orientation on determinants of proenvironmental behavior intention. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Corral-Verdugo, V.; Caso-Niebla, J.; Tapia-Fonllem, C.; Frías-Armenta, M. Consideration of immediate and future consequences in accepting and responding to anthropogenic climate change. Psychology 2017, 8, 1519–1531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montada, L.; Kals, E.; Becker, R. Umweltschützende Verzichte [Pro Environmental Sacrificing Actions]; Danner, D., Glöckner-Rist, A., Eds.; Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen [Compilation of social science items and scales]; GESIS: Mannheim, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherhorn, G.; Haas, H.; Hellenthal, F.; Seibold, S. Naturverträgliches Handeln [Environmental Compatible Action]; Danner, D., Glöckner-Rist, A., Eds.; Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen [Compilation of social science items and scales]; GESIS: Mannheim, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kormos, C.; Gifford, R. The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: A meta-analytic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, S. Factors Influencing Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors: A U.K. Case Study of Household Waste Management. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 435–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nolan, J.M.; Schultz, P.W. Prosocial behavior and environmental action. In The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior; Schroeder, D.A., Graziano, W.G., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 626–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schleyer-Lindenmann, A.; Ittner, H.; Dauvier, B.; Piolat, M. The NEP Scale-behind the (German) scenes of environmental concern. Diagnostica 2018, 64, 156–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCambridge, J.; Witton, J.; Elbourne, D.R. Systematic review of the Hawthorne effect: New concepts are needed to study research participation effects. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2014, 67, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miles, L.M.; Rodrigues, A.M.; Sniehotta, F.F.; French, D.P. Asking questions changes health-related behaviour: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilding, S.; Conner, M.; Sandberg, T.; Prestwich, A.; Lawton, R.; Wood, C.; Miles, E.; Godin, G.; Sheeran, P. The question-behaviour effect: A theoretical and methodological review and meta-analysis. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 27, 196–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lange, F.; Brick, C.; Dewitte, S. Green when seen? No support for an effect of observability on environmental conservation in the laboratory: A registered report. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 190189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bateson, M.; Callow, L.; Holmes, J.R.; Roche, M.L.R.; Nettle, D. Do images of ‘watching eyes’ induce behaviour that is more pro-social or more normative? A field experiment on littering. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e82055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vicente-Molina, M.A.; Fernández-Sainz, A.; Izagirre-Olaizola, J. Does gender make a difference in pro-environmental behavior? The case of the Basque Country University students. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 176, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, A. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 116, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hodges, H.; Kuehl, C.; Anderson, S.E.; Ehret, P.J.; Brick, C. How managers can reduce household water use through communication: A field experiment. J. Policy Anal. Manag. 2020, 39, 1076–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Name | Scale | Sample Item | Mean | SD | Cronbach’s α [CI95%] |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
NEP [22] | 1–5 (don’t agree at all–fully agree) | Humans are born to rule over nature. | 3.93 | 0.41 | 0.69 [0.63, 0.75] |
Consideration of distant future consequences [18] | 1–5 (don’t agree at all–fully agree) | Generally, my behavior is influenced by future consequences. | 3.81 | 0.49 | 0.57 [0.48, 0.65] |
Consideration of immediate consequences [18] | 1–5 (don’t agree at al –fully agree) | I only act to satisfy immediate concerns, figuring the future will take care of itself. | 2.00 | 0.63 | 0.65 [0.58, 0.72] |
Self-reported PEB pre | 1–7 (never–always) | In the last 5 days, I have payed attention to labels for ecological products (e.g., on food, clothes). | 5.86 | 1.14 | 0.77 [0.72, 0.81] |
Self-reported PEB post | 1–7 (never–always) | In the last 5 days, I have payed attention to labels for ecological products (e.g., on food, clothes). | 6.00 | 1.11 | 0.80 [0.76, 0.84] |
Age | Gender | Political Attitude | NEP | Cons. of Future Cons. | Cons. of Immediate Cons. | Self-Reported PEB Pre | Self-Reported PEP Post | Willingness to Donate (Prosocial) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | 0.13 [−0.00, 0.26] | ||||||||
Political attitude | 0.02 [−0.11, 0.16] | 0.04 [−0.09, 0.17] | |||||||
NEP (pro-environmental attitude) | −0.06 [−0.19, 0.08] | −0.17 * [−0.31, −0.03] | −0.22 ** [−0.35, −0.08] | ||||||
Cons. of future cons. | −0.05 [−0.18, 0.09] | −0.10 [−0.24, 0.05] | −0.10 [−0.25, 0.03] | 0.21 ** [0.07, 0.34] | |||||
Cons. of immediate cons. | 0.03 [−0.12, 0.17] | 0.14 * [0.01, 0.27] | 0.22 ** [0.08, 0.35] | −0.25 *** [−0.38, −0.11] | −0.54 *** [−0.64, −0.43] | ||||
Self-reported PEB pre | 0.05 [−0.09, 0.19] | −0.12 [−0.25, 0.01] | −0.21 ** [−0.35, −0.06] | 0.11 [−0.03, 0.25] | 0.47 *** [0.36, 0.56] | −0.23 *** [−0.38, −0.08] | |||
Self-reported PEB post | 0.01 [−0.13, 0.15] | −0.17 * [−0.30, −0.04] | −0.20 ** [−0.33, −0.07] | 0.18 ** [0.03, 0.31] | 0.44 *** [0.32, 0.54] | −0.27 *** [−0.40, −0.14] | 0.84 *** [0.78, 0.88] | ||
Willingness to donate (prosocial) | −0.06 [−0.20, 0.07] | −.09 [−0.22, 0.05] | −0.00 [−0.15, 0.13] | −0.13 [−0.27, 0.00] | −0.09 [−0.23, 0.05] | 0.04 [−0.11, 0.18] | 0.13 [−0.00, 0.25] | 0.08 [−0.06, 0.22] | |
Willingness to donate (pro-environmental) | 0.09 [−0.05, 0.22] | −0.22 ** [−0.34, −0.09] | −0.10 [−0.23, 0.02] | 0.14 * [0.01, 0.29] | 0.23 *** [0.10, 0.35] | −0.21 ** [−0.34, −0.07] | 0.33 *** [0.20, 0.44] | 0.29 *** [0.16, 0.42] | 0.31 *** [0.17, 0.44] |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kesenheimer, J.S.; Greitemeyer, T. Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior. Sustainability 2020, 12, 10064. https://doi.org/10.3390/su122310064
Kesenheimer JS, Greitemeyer T. Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior. Sustainability. 2020; 12(23):10064. https://doi.org/10.3390/su122310064
Chicago/Turabian StyleKesenheimer, Jana Sophie, and Tobias Greitemeyer. 2020. "Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior" Sustainability 12, no. 23: 10064. https://doi.org/10.3390/su122310064
APA StyleKesenheimer, J. S., & Greitemeyer, T. (2020). Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior. Sustainability, 12(23), 10064. https://doi.org/10.3390/su122310064