Promoting Reviewer-Related Attribution: Moderately Complex Presentation of Mixed Opinions Activates the Analytic Process
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background and Research Hypotheses
2.1. The Negative Influence of WOM Dispersion
2.2. Mechanism to Promote Reviewer-Related Attribution Choices
2.2.1. The Important Role of Reviewer-Related Attribution Choices
2.2.2. Dual Processing and the Promotion of Reviewer-Related Attribution Choices
2.3. The Way to Promote Reviewer-Related Attribution Choices
2.3.1. The Influence of the Presentation Type of Mixed Opinions on Attribution Choices
2.3.2. The Presentation of Types of Mixed Opinions and Cognitive Load
2.4. Visual Processing Research in Cognition
3. Methodology and Results
- (1)
- Skill. Potential consumers’ responses to reviews can differ depending on how skilled they are at online shopping [59]. Therefore, participants were asked to report their online shopping frequencies on a seven-point scale ranging from “rarely” to “very frequently” in both studies.
- (2)
- Average ratings. Although average ratings are positive for the vast majority of products on real-world platforms and consumers tend to avoid low-rated options, a range of average ratings were manipulated directly to examine the validity of our predictions for both studies [4]. All participants need to report the minimum average ratings (1–10) and the ratio of good-level ratings (1%–100%) required for products in their consideration set. As described below, stimuli in the main studies were constructed with average ratings (and ratios of good-level ratings) above this level.
- (3)
- S-standard. Behavioral results do not reflect the existence of the dual systems, except that the rule-based (analysis) and the automatic (heuristic) processes lead to different responses [41,58]. This means different systems will result in different attribution choices only when WOM is high in dispersion. Therefore, the target distribution depicted high dispersion in both studies.
- (4)
- Samples. Considering that most active online shoppers are young adults under the age of thirty [60], choosing undergraduates as samples seems suitable for achieving the purpose of our study. The participants were all Chinese. Therefore, the language in all stimuli was Chinese. Since participants had to look at picture stimuli, a qualified participant needed to have normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
- (5)
- Equipment. An SR Research Eyelink 2000 eye tracker with a sampling rate of 500 Hz was used to collect participants’ eye movement data. Eye movement data were only recorded from the left eye. When participants arrived in the research lab, they were seated in front of a 27-inch monitor with a resolution of 1024 768 pixels, and then the pupils’ locations were calibrated.
3.1. Study 1
3.1.1. Method
3.1.2. Results
Manipulation Check
Causal Attribution Choices
Visual Attention Analysis
3.1.3. Discussion
3.2. Study 2
3.2.1. Pretest
3.2.2. Method
3.2.3. Results
Manipulation Check
Causal Attribution Choices
Visual Attention Analysis
3.2.4. Discussion
4. Conclusions
4.1. Theoretical Contributions
4.2. Managerial Implications
4.3. Limitations and Future Research
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- I would choose an envelope from the first set of envelopes.
- (b)
- I would choose an envelope from the second set of envelopes.
- (a)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to deliver all the promotional work to the larger publicity agency.
- (b)
- The best possibility of starting the promotion sooner would be to divide the promotional work between the two smaller agencies.
- (a)
- Mike is one of the humanities students.
- (b)
- Mike is one of the science students.
References
- China Online Shopping Market Research Report 2015. Available online: http://www.cnnic.cn/gywm/xwzx/rdxw/2016/201606/P020160721576734671513.pdf (accessed on 19 December 2020).
- Sun, A.M. How does the variance of product ratings matter? Manag. Sci. 2012, 58, 696–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hu, N.; Pavlou, P.A.; Zhang, J. On self-selection biases in online products reviews. MIS Q. 2017, 41, 449–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- He, S.X.; Bond, S.D. Why is the crowd divided? Attribution for dispersion in online word of mouth. J. Consum. Res. 2015, 41, 1509–1527. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, F.; Liu, X.; Fang, E. User reviews variance, critic reviews variance, and product sales: An exploration of customer breadth and depth effects. J. Retail. 2015, 91, 372–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, Y.M.; Liu, M.M.; Zeng, X.H.; Huang, P. The effects of prior reviews on perceived review helpfulness: A configuration perspective. J. Bus. Res. 2020, 110, 484–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Y. More is less: Only moderate polarized online product reviews can affect sales. Int. J. Bus. Manag. 2018, 13, 192–200. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, N.; Zhang, J.; Pavlou, P.A. Overcoming the J-shaped distribution of product reviews. Commun. ACM 2009, 52, 144–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, C.; Cui, G.; Peng, L. Tailoring management response to negative reviews: The effectiveness of accommodative versus defensive responses. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 84, 272–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guo, B.; Zhou, S. Understanding the impact of prior reviews on subsequent reviews: The role of rating volume, variance, and reviewer characteristics. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2016, 20, 147–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bi, S.; Liu, Z.; Usman, K. The influence of online information on investing decisions of reward-based crowdfunding. J. Bus. Res. 2017, 71, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sridhar, S.; Srinivasan, R. Social influence effects in online product ratings. J. Mark. 2012, 76, 70–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langan, R.; Besharat, A.; Varki, S. The effect of review valence and variance on product evaluations: An examination of intrinsic and extrinsic cues. Int. J. Res. Mark. 2017, 34, 414–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, F.; Zhang, X. Impact of online consumer reviews on sales: The moderating role of product and consumer characteristics. J. Mark. 2010, 74, 133–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moe, W.W.; Trusov, M. The value of social dynamics in online product ratings forums. J. Market. Res. 2011, 48, 444–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stone, E.R.; Sieck, W.R.; Bull, B.E.; Yates, J.F.; Parks, S.C.; Rush, C.J. Foreground: Background salience: Explaining the effects of graphical displays on risk avoidance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 2003, 90, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newman, G.E.; Scholl, B.J. Bar graphs depicting averages are perceptually misinterpreted: The within-the-bar bias. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 601–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leboeuf, R.A.; Norton, M.I. Consequence-cause matching: Looking to the consequences of events to infer their causes. J. Consum. Res. 2012, 39, 128–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- He, S.X. Consumer Judgment and Forecasting Using Online Word-of-Mouth. Ph.D. Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Laczniak, R.N.; Decarlo, T.E.; Ramaswami, S.N. Consumers’ responses to negative word-of-mouth communication: An attribution theory perspective. J. Consum. Psychol. 2001, 11, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Z.; Berger, J. When, why, and how controversy causes conversation. J. Consum. Res. 2013, 40, 580–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evans, J.S.; Stanovich, K.E. Dual-process theories of higher cognition. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 223–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bago, B.; De, N.W. Fast logic? Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition 2017, 158, 90–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alter, A.L.; Oppenheimer, D.M.; Epley, N.; Eyre, R.N. Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 569–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seufert, T.; Wagner, F.; Westphal, J. The effects of different levels of disfluency on learning outcomes and cognitive load. Instr. Sci. 2017, 45, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K.A. Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self-evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pieger, E.; Mengelkamp, C.; Bannert, M. Metacognitive judgments and disfluency: Does disfluency lead to more accurate judgments, better control, and better performance. Learn. Instr. 2016, 44, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simmons, J.P.; Nelson, L.D. Intuitive confidence: Choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 409–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pieger, E.; Mengelkamp, C.; Bannert, M. Fostering analytic metacognitive processes and reducing overconfidence by disfluency: The role of contrast effects. Appl. Cogn. Psych. 2017, 31, 291–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clemons, E.K.; Gao, G.; Hitt, L.M. When online reviews meet hyperdifferentiation: A study of craft beer industry. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 2006, 23, 149–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Khare, A.; Labrecque, L.I.; Asare, A.K. The assimilative and contrastive effects of word-of-mouth volume: An experimental examination of online consumer ratings. J. Retail. 2011, 87, 111–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mudambi, S.M.; Schuff, D. What makes a helpful review? A study of customer reviews on Amazon. com. MIS Q. 2010, 34, 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moon, S.; Bergey, P.K.; Iacobucci, D. Dynamic effects among movie ratings, movie revenues, and viewer satisfaction. J. Mark. 2010, 74, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Martino, B.; Kumaran, D.; Seymour, B.; Dolan, R.J. Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science 2006, 313, 684–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tsiros, M.; Mittal, V.; Ross, W.T. The role of attributions in customer satisfaction: A reexamination. J. Consum. Res. 2004, 31, 476–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiner, B. Attributional thoughts about consumer behavior. J. Consum. Res. 2000, 27, 382–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Z.; Lurie, N.H. Temporal contiguity and negativity bias in the impact of online word of mouth. J. Mark. Res. 2013, 50, 463–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoeffler, S.; Ariely, D.; West, P.; Duclos, R. Preference exploration and learning: The role of intensiveness and extensiveness of experience. J. Consum. Psychol. 2013, 23, 330–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratner, R.K.; Kahn, B.E. The impact of private versus public consumption on variety-seeking behavior. J. Consum. Res. 2002, 29, 246–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Frederick, S. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment; Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., Kahneman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 49–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; Toplak, M.E. Defining features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type 2 processing. Mind Soc. 2012, 11, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Advancing the rationality debate. Behav. Brain. Sci. 2000, 23, 701–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stephens, R.G.; Dunn, J.C.; Hayes, B.K.; Kalish, M.L. A test of two processes: The effect of training on deductive and inductive reasoning. Cognition 2020, 199, 104–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Petty, R.E.; Wegener, D.T. The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology; Chaiken, S., Trope, Y., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 41–72. [Google Scholar]
- Koriat, A. The relationships between monitoring, regulation and performance. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 296–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, J.H. Cognitive monitoring. In Children’s Oral Communication Skill; Dickson, W.P., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 35–60. [Google Scholar]
- Yue, C.L.; Castel, A.D.; Bjork, R.A. When disfluency is—And is not—A desirable difficulty: The influence of typeface clarity on metacognitive judgments and memory. Mem. Cognition 2013, 41, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 1981, 211, 453–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lipkus, I.M.; Hollands, J.G. The visual communication of risk. JNCI Monogr. 1999, 25, 149–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kao, D.T. Original paper framing in healthcare advertising: The moderating effects of regulatory focus and product category on advertising attitudes. J. Bus. Theory Pract. 2013, 1, 94–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ilic, U.; Akbulut, Y. Effect of Disfluency on Learning Outcomes, Metacognitive Judgments and Cognitive Load in Computer Assisted Learning Environments. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 99, 310–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Merriënboer, J.J.G.V.; Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 10, 251–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J. Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 22, 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eitel, A.; Kühl, T. Effects of disfluency and test expectancy on learning with text. Metacogn. Learn. 2016, 11, 107–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehman, M.; Karpicke, J.D. Elaborative retrieval: Do semantic mediators improve memory? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2016, 42, 1573–1591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lim, J.H.; Christianson, K. Second language sensitivity to agreement errors: Evidence from eye movements during comprehension and translation. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2014, 36, 1283–1315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Q.; Huang, Z.; Christianson, K. Visual attention toward tourism photographs with text: An eye-tracking study. Tour. Manag. 2016, 54, 243–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, M.B.; Garcia-Marques, L.; Sherman, S.J.; Sherman, J.W. Automatic and controlled components of judgment and decision making. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 91, 797–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Goel, L.; Prokopec, S. If you build it will they come? An empirical investigation of consumer perceptions and strategy in virtual worlds. Electron. Commer. Res. 2009, 9, 115–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mack, R.W.; Blose, J.E.; Pan, B. Believe it or not: Credibility of blogs in tourism. J. Vacat. Mark. 2008, 2, 133–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rayner, K. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2009, 62, 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franke, G.R.; Huhmann, B.A.; Mothersbaugh, D.L. Information content and consumer readership of print ads: A comparison of search and experience products. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2004, 32, 20–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiménez, F.R.; Mendoza, N.A. Too popular to ignore: The influence of online reviews on purchase intentions of search and experience products. J. Interact. Mark. 2013, 27, 226–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duan, W.; Gu, B.; Whinston, A.B. The dynamics of online word-of-mouth and product sales—An empirical investigation of the movie industry. J. Retail. 2008, 84, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bae, S.; Lee, T. Product type and consumers’ perception of online consumer reviews. Electron. Mark. 2011, 21, 255–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Purnawirawan, N.; De Pelsmacker, P.; Dens, N. Balance and sequence in online reviews: How perceived usefulness affects attitudes and intentions. J. Interact. Mark. 2012, 26, 244–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, L.R.; Goldsmith, R.E. A short, reliable measure of subjective knowledge. J. Bus. Res. 1999, 46, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, D.H.; Lee, J.; Han, I. The effect of on-line consumer reviews on consumer purchasing intention: The moderating role of involvement. Int. J. Electron. Comm. 2007, 11, 125–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Primi, C.; Morsanyi, K.; Chiesi, F.; Donati, M.A.; Hamilton, J. The development and testing of a new version of the cognitive reflection test applying item response theory (IRT). J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 29, 453–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pass, S.; Ronen, B. Management by the market constraint in the hi-tech industry. Int. J. Prod. Res. 2003, 41, 713–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Type | Main Online Platforms in China | The Presentation of Mixed Opinions |
---|---|---|
① | Amazon.cn | A five-star horizontal bar chart |
② | Tmall.com Jumei.com | A set of reviewers’ impressions of the product |
③ | Jingdong.com | A three-dimension horizontal bar chart and a set of reviewers’ impressions of the product |
Taobao.com | ||
Dangdang.com |
Word of Mouth (WOM) Dispersion (N = 40) | |
---|---|
Average Ratings | High Dispersion |
5 stars | |
(Variance = 8.9) | |
6 stars | |
(Variance = 8.9) | |
7 stars | |
(Variance = 10.9) |
Problem Version | Estimate | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Condition | Inclusion | Exclusion | H | RB |
Intuitive | 0.75 | 0.68 | 0.74 | 0.05 |
Rational | 0.78 | 0.44 | 0.71 | 0.33 |
Decision Conditions | |||
---|---|---|---|
WOM Average | High Dispersion | Rational | Intuitive |
Rating = 5 | Variance = 8.9 | 3.75 (0.31) | 1.89 (0.21) |
Rating = 6 | Variance = 8.9 | 4.46 (0.28) | 2.43 (0.24) |
Rating = 7 | Variance = 10.9 | 5.82 (0.30) | 3.96 (0.34) |
Product | Reviewers’ Impressions of the Product |
---|---|
Smartphone | Good camera quality; Smooth system performance; Decent call quality; High resolution; Metal-made; Bad sound quality; Poorly-designed; Slow. |
The Golden Era (Movie) | Good plot; Heartwarming story; Creative storytelling; Good movie; Good cast; Bad script; Incoherent storyline; Terrible soundtrack. |
Type ①: Simple Condition WOM Dispersion (N = 40) | |
---|---|
Search Product | Experience Product |
Type ②: Moderately Complex Condition WOM Dispersion (N = 40) | |
---|---|
Search Product | Experience Product |
Type ③: Very Complex Condition WOM Dispersion (N = 40) | |
---|---|
Search Product | Experience Product |
Product Attribute | |||
---|---|---|---|
WOM Average | Presentation Type of Mixed Opinions | Search Product (Smartphone) | Experience Product (Movie) |
Simple condition | 2.59 (0.20) | 3.41 (0.19) | |
Rating = 5 | Moderately complex condition | 3.52 (0.19) | 4.73 (0.16) |
Very complex condition | 2.61 (0.16) | 3.45 (0.18) | |
Simple condition | 2.71 (0.25) | 4.48 (0.22) | |
Rating = 6 | Moderately complex condition | 3.80 (0.24) | 5.30 (0.25) |
Very complex condition | 2.60 (0.20) | 4.29 (0.20) | |
Simple condition | 2.95 (0.21) | 4.69 (0.19) | |
Rating = 7 | Moderately complex condition | 4.14 (0.18) | 5.60 (0.20) |
Very complex condition | 2.98 (0.22) | 4.70 (0.24) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Xie, G.; Du, W.; Yuan, H.; Jiang, Y. Promoting Reviewer-Related Attribution: Moderately Complex Presentation of Mixed Opinions Activates the Analytic Process. Sustainability 2021, 13, 441. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13020441
Xie G, Du W, Yuan H, Jiang Y. Promoting Reviewer-Related Attribution: Moderately Complex Presentation of Mixed Opinions Activates the Analytic Process. Sustainability. 2021; 13(2):441. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13020441
Chicago/Turabian StyleXie, Guangming, Wenbo Du, Hongping Yuan, and Yushi Jiang. 2021. "Promoting Reviewer-Related Attribution: Moderately Complex Presentation of Mixed Opinions Activates the Analytic Process" Sustainability 13, no. 2: 441. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13020441
APA StyleXie, G., Du, W., Yuan, H., & Jiang, Y. (2021). Promoting Reviewer-Related Attribution: Moderately Complex Presentation of Mixed Opinions Activates the Analytic Process. Sustainability, 13(2), 441. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13020441