Next Article in Journal
Simulation of Triaxial Tests for Unsaturated Soils under a Tension–Shear State by the Discrete Element Method
Previous Article in Journal
Big Data Privacy in Smart Farming: A Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of Aposematic Signals of Plants on Students’ Perception and Willingness to Protect Them

by
Jana Fančovičová
1,
Pavol Prokop
2,3,* and
Markéta Kubíčková
4
1
Department of Biology, Faculty of Education, Trnava University, 918 43 Trnava, Slovakia
2
Department of Environmental Ecology and Landscape Management, Faculty of Natural Sciences, Comenius University, Ilkovičova 6, 842 15 Bratislava, Slovakia
3
Institute of Zoology, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Dúbravská cesta 9, 845 06 Bratislava, Slovakia
4
Primary School with Kindergarten, Námestie 1. Mája 3, 922 05 Chtelnica, Slovakia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(15), 9121; https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159121
Submission received: 6 May 2022 / Revised: 19 July 2022 / Accepted: 22 July 2022 / Published: 25 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Sustainable Education and Approaches)

Abstract

:
Degradation of biodiversity is one of the current problems of today, and scientists are increasingly concerned with identifying the key factors influencing people’s willingness to protect (WTP) wild organisms. Using a within-subject design, we investigated the influence of aposematic signals along with the presence or absence of flowers on perceived danger, attractiveness and WTP plants with lower secondary school students (mean age = 13 yrs) in Slovakia (n = 423). Aposematic plants received a higher dangerousness score (mean = 2.62 vs. 2.27), higher attractiveness score (mean = 3.45 vs. 3.32) and lower WTP plants than plants without aposematic signals (mean = 3.27 vs. 3.37). Interaction terms showed that males perceived the aposematic species as more dangerous than females and were more willing to protect species lacking aposematic signals. Females rated aposematic plants as more attractive than non-aposematic plants (mean = 3.82 vs. 3.0). The presence of flowers increased the perceived attractiveness of plants (mean = 3.75 vs. 3.02) and WTP plants (mean = 3.59 vs. 3.05) and decreased perceived dangerousness (mean = 2.70 vs. 2.20). Perceived attractiveness and WTP plants decreased with students’ age. Students with a higher interest in plants rated the attractiveness of the species more positively and were also more willing to protect them regardless of the presence of aposematic signals. We conclude that the presence of aposematic signals does not directly contribute to WTP plants, but conspicuous traits with high aesthetic value, such as flowers, positively enhance WTP in Slovak students.

1. Introduction

Aesthetic judgment in humans is likely to play an important role in everyday decision-making [1,2]. One attributes positive qualities to attractive subjects and negatively evaluates unattractive subjects. This mechanism is referred to as “what is beautiful is good” [3,4].
Aesthetic judgment plays a role in conservation science because it significantly influences the willingness to protect (WTP) animals [5,6] by means of emotional processes [7]. Species with low aesthetic value generally activate the emotion of fear or disgust and receive low conservation support [8,9,10,11]. In contrast, species with high aesthetic value receive high conservation support from the public [12,13,14] and are more frequently kept in zoos than less attractive species [15,16]. In addition, there are considerable cross-cultural similarities in aesthetic preferences for animals [17,18], suggesting that the psychological processes underlying aesthetic preference are universal [19] and findings from one culture can be generalized to other cultures. Unfortunately, however, these findings are based on research on animals, whereas similar research on plants is scarce.
Humans are interested in plants much less than in animals [20,21,22,23]. Consequently, plants receive lower conservation support than animals [24], although it should be noted that the factors underlying human’s WTP plants are rarely investigated [25,26]. Human neglect and ignorance of plants are also known as plant blindness [27,28] or plant awareness disparity [29]. One of the major proposed causes of plant blindness, that is, the human natural tendency to overlook plants, is their homogeneity and the absence of apparent movement, which leads (among other things) to insensitivity to the aesthetic characteristics of plants [28]. Females are generally more attentive to plants than males [30,31,32]. It is therefore recommended (1) to draw attention to individual plants and species rather than to plant communities and (2) to promote positive emotional connections with plants [24,28,33].
The aim of the present study was to examine whether the human perception of plants is influenced by certain morphological plant characteristics. In particular, the present study investigated how the presence of spines as examples of aposematic signals [34,35,36,37] influences perceived danger, attractiveness and WTP plants. Previous research indicated that certain plant colours and harmful characteristics enhance retention [38,39], and certain flower traits increase the aesthetic evaluation of plants [40,41,42,43]. We therefore also included the presence of flowers among predictors of perceived danger, attractiveness and WTP plants. We hypothesised that (1) the presence of spines negatively influences perceived attractiveness and WTP plants and positively influences the perceived danger of plants, (2) the presence of flowers positively influences perceived attractiveness and WTP plants but decreases the perceived danger of plants. We also hypothesised that (3) the low perceived danger of plants and high perceived attractiveness of plants are positively associated with WTP plants and that (4) students interested in plants show greater WTP plants than those with a low interest in plants. With respect to gender differences, we hypothesised that (5) females are more sensitive to the presence of spines than males, (6) females perceive plants as more attractive than males and (7) WTP plants is greater for females than for males.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 423 students from the 5th to the 9th grades of three lower secondary schools in the Trnava region (Western Slovakia) participated to this research. All these schools are typical middle-sized schools with 363–667 students taught by 25–56 teachers. The age of the students ranged between 10 and 16 years (mean age = 13 years, SE = 0.07, n = 423). The selection of participants was not intentional but was based on teachers’ willingness to participate in the research in the accessible schools. Teachers were selected according to personal contacts with researchers. If the teacher agreed, the questionnaire was administered to all the participants in the selected classes irrespective of the students’ attitudes towards plants. All the participants were unpaid volunteers who were blind to our research hypotheses, and the research was anonymous.

2.2. Selection of Plants

We used colour pictures of 18 plant species freely downloaded from Google. Half of the plants possess aposematic signals (spines). We selected two types of conspicuous spines and spines, which are typical of more than a thousand plant species belonging to the Agavaceae, Cactaceae, Asphodelaceae, Crassulaceae and Euphorbiaceae families. The second group consisted of nine species without aposematic signals (Table 1, Figure 1). Almost all the selected plants were non-indigenous species to avoid possible biases caused by personal experiences with these species in nature. The presented pictures were modified in the Adobe Photoshop graphics editor to make them standardized, cropped and placed on a uniform white background without disturbing elements.

2.3. Measuring Perceived Dangerousness, Attractiveness and Willingness to Protect (WTP) Plants

During the presentation of the plants, the respondents answered three questions similar to those which Prokop and Fančovičová [10] used to examine aposematic animals: (1) To what extent do you think the displayed species is dangerous? (2) To what extent do you think the displayed species is attractive? (3) Do you think that the species shown should be protected by law? All responses were Likert-scaled (1 = absolutely disagree, 5 = absolutely agree).

2.4. Measuring Interest in Plants

Students’ interest and enjoyment of plants was assessed with the 10-Likert scale items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) of the Interest in plants subscale derived from the Plant Attitude Questionnaire [44]. High scores indicate high interest in plants. Example items are “I am interested in reading books about plants”, “I would like to cultivate plants”, “I like watching films about plants”. The subscale showed high reliability (Cronbach alpha = 0.82), and the mean score was used to form an overall measure of interest in plants. Students were finally asked to provide information about their age, grade and gender.

2.5. Procedure

Students were tested as whole classes using paper-and-pencil questionnaires. First, each student was asked to write demographic variables, followed by ten Interest in plant items. We then projected 18 colour pictures of plants in lecture halls. The pictures were presented in random order. Each picture was presented for 1 min. During this time, participants rated perceived dangerousness, attractiveness and WTP each species.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) was used to test whether subjectively perceived danger, attractiveness and WTP plant scores (dependent variables) were influenced by gender differences, the presence of spines and flowers (categorical predictors) and interest in plants or age (continuous predictors). School and students’ ID were treated as random effects. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI). GLMM was performed in SPSS ver. 26. A power analysis in GPower revealed that our sample size would provide requested power at 0.80 to detect a medium effect (d = 0.50) with α two-tailed = 0.05 [45].

3. Results

3.1. Subjectively Perceived Danger

Plants with spines (mean = 2.62, 95% CI [2.56–2.69]) were subjectively perceived as more dangerous than plants without spines (mean = 2.27, 95% CI [2.21–2.33]) (F(1,1687) = 67.1; p < 0.001) (Figure 2). Plants with flowers (mean = 2.20, 95% CI [2.13–2.37]) were perceived as less dangerous than plants without flowers (mean = 2.70, 95% CI [2.64–2.75]) (F(1,1687) = 137.2; p < 0.001). Students more interested in plants perceived plants as less dangerous than those less interested in plants (β = −0.08, F(1,1687) = 6.9; p = 0.009). The effect of gender (males: mean = 2.47, 95% CI [2.41–2.53], females: mean = 2.42, 95% CI [2.36–2.48]) and age was not significant (F(1,1687) = 0.52 and 0.0, p = 0.47 and 0.99, respectively). A significant interaction term gender × spines emerged (F(1,1687) = 13.06; p < 0.001), suggesting that males perceived plants with spines as more dangerous (mean = 2.72, 95% CI [2.63–2.82]) than without spines (mean = 2.22, 95% CI [2.13–2.32]), and the same but weaker trend was observed for females (plants with spines: mean = 2.52, 95% CI [2.42–2.61] and plants without spines: mean = 2.32, 95% CI [2.22–2.42]). The interaction terms gender × flowers and spines × flowers were not statistically significant (F(1,1687) = 0.77 and 1.34, p = 0.38 and 0.29).

3.2. Subjectively Perceived Attractiveness

Plants with spines (mean = 3.45, 95% CI [3.39–3.50]) were subjectively perceived as more attractive than plants without spines (mean = 3.32, 95% CI [3.26–3.39]) (F(1,1687) = 9.65; p = 0.002) (Figure 3). Plants with flowers (mean = 3.75, 95% CI [3.68–3.81]) were perceived as more attractive than plants without flowers (mean = 3.02, 95% CI [2.98–3.07]) (F(1,1687) = 330.41; p < 0.001). Students’ interest in plants as well as students’ age were significantly associated with perceived attractiveness of plants (β = 0.17 and −0.05, F(1,1687) = 38.37 and 13.85; both p < 0.001). The effect of gender (males: mean = 3.33, 95% CI [3.27–3.39], females: mean = 3.44, 95% CI [3.38–3.51]) was not significant (F(1,1687) = 0.0, p = 0.99). The interaction term between gender × spines was not significant (F(1,1687) = 3.31, p = 0.07). The interaction term between gender × flowers was statistically significant (F(1,1687) = 8.25, p = 0.004). Females perceived plants with flowers as more attractive (mean = 3.82, 95% CI [3.70–4.0]) than plants without flowers (mean = 3.0, 95% CI [2.85–3.11]), but this difference was not so much pronounced in males. A significant interaction term spines × flowers emerged (F(1,1687) = 5.92; p = 0.015), suggesting that plants with flowers but without spines were perceived similarly attractive as plants having both flowers and spines. In contrast, the absence of flowers resulted in higher ratings of plants having spines than plants without spines (Figure 3).

3.3. Subjective Perception of Willingness to Protect Plants (WTP)

Students showed stronger preference for the protection of plants without spines (mean = 3.37, 95% CI [3.30–3.43]) than for plants with spines (mean = 3.27, 95% CI [3.21–3.33]) or (F(1,1687) = 4.80; p = 0.03) (Figure 4). Plants with flowers (mean = 3.59, 95% CI [3.53– 3.66]) received a significantly greater perceived WTP score than plants without flowers (mean = 3.05, 95% CI [3.0–3.10]) (F(1,1687) = 175.94; p < 0.001). Students’ interest in plants positively influenced WTP (β = 0.16, F(1,1687) = 30.44, p < 0.001). Students’ age negatively influenced willingness to protect plants (β = −0.04, F(1,1687) = 5.93, p = 0.02). The effect of gender (males: mean = 3.21, 95% CI [3.15–3.27], females: mean = 3.44, 95% CI [3.38–3.50]) was statistically significant (F(1,1687) = 9.72, p = 0.002); females were more willing to protect plants than males. Two interaction terms (gender × spines, flowers × spines) were not significant (F(1,1687) = 2.86, 0.003, p = 0.09 and 0.95, respectively). The interaction term gender × flowers (F(1,1687) = 6.07, p = 0.014) suggests that females were more willing to protect plants with flowers (mean = 3.72, 95% CI [3.63–3.80]) than plants without flowers (mean = 3.06, 95% CI [3.0–3.15]), but this difference was not so much pronounced in males.

3.4. Relationships between Perceived Danger, Attractiveness and Willingness to Protect Plants

Simple correlations showed that WTP plants were moderately and negatively correlated with perceived danger (r = −0.26, p < 0.001) and strongly and positively correlated with perceived attractiveness (r = 0.60, p < 0.001). Perceived attractiveness was moderately and negatively correlated with perceived danger (r = −0.36, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

This study examined the influence of the presence of spines as examples of aposematic signals and the presence of flowers on perceived danger, attractiveness and willingness to protect (WTP) plants with a sample of Slovak students. Our hypotheses were built largely on research on animals because research in this field is more abundant than research on WTP plants. As far as we are aware, this is the first study investigating the perception of plant aposematism by humans.
We hypothesised that (1) the presence of spines negatively influences perceived attractiveness and WTP plants and positively influences perceived danger of plants. This hypothesis was statistically confirmed. Spines have sharp-angled features, and such features enhance a feeling of threat because sharp objects such as teeth, claws or horns have been harmful to humans throughout our evolutionary history [46]. Not surprisingly, peacefulness is associated with round shapes, whereas angular shapes are associated with anger and aggression [47]. Our research shows that sharp features such as spines, which evolved as a defence against herbivores [48], also evoke a feeling of danger in humans. Interestingly, Hareli et al. [49] similarly found that sharp leaves were rated by participants as less friendly, uglier, less comforting, colder and more dangerous than round leaves. Ultimately, avoidance of aposematic plants with spines is caused by the risk of injuries which often cause septic inflammation that can be life-threatening due to transmission of pathogenic bacteria and fungi for both humans and non-human animals [50,51]. Perceived danger and fear, however, may boost memory, similar to other survival-relevant scenarios [38,39,52]. Thus, in theory, aposematic signals can be advantageous in learning, but this question requires further research.
Prokop & Fančovičová [10] showed that aposematic signals increased WTP animals, which can be seen as a contrast to the present study. Their research was focused, however, exclusively on aposematic colouration, meaning that aposematic animals were more conspicuous than non-aposematic animals. Our present research on plants is very distinct in this sense because we did not manipulate colours but exclusively the presence of sharp-angled features.
Our second hypothesis suggested that (2) the presence of flowers positively influences perceived attractiveness and WTP plants but decreases the perceived danger of plants. In line with previous reasoning, natural preferences for curved shapes positively influenced the perceived attractiveness of plants [46]. The aesthetic value of flowers [40,42,43] substantially contributed to the decreased perceived danger of plants. In turn, the low perceived danger of plants and particularly high perceived attractiveness of plants positively correlated with WTP plants (Hypothesis 3). These results agree with research on animals where dangerously looking individuals [11] and animals with low aesthetic value [8,9] received significantly lower WTP scores than neutral-looking and/or aesthetically pleasant animals.
In line with Hypothesis 4, students displaying greater interest in plants also showed greater WTP plants than students with intrinsically low interest in plants. It seems that interest in plants correlates with curiosity and knowledge about plants [32], and the present results extend these associations to WTP plants. More research about how students behave toward plants in real-life settings is required. Notably, interest in plants decreased with the age of students, which corroborates with similar findings focused on students’ attitudes [44] and knowledge about plants [53,54]. We are of the opinion that since these negative trends are attributed to puberty [54] when students’ interest in sexuality increases [55], it seems reasonable to heighten interest in plants by introducing topics of plant sexuality.
Contrary to Hypothesis 5, males seemed to be more sensitive to the presence of spines than females. Females did not consider plants more attractive than males (contrary to Hypothesis 6) and females showed greater WTP plants than males (Hypothesis 7). With respect to Hypothesis 5, perhaps because males behave riskier than females both actually and historically [56], their greater sensitivity to spines may help them avoid injury. The absence of greater aesthetic preferences for plants by females (Hypothesis 6) could mirror the selection of plant species in this study, which was based on the presence/absence of spines rather than on their aesthetic value. Indeed, plants with flowers were significantly more preferred by females than plants without flowers. We suggest that high aesthetic value of selected plants contributed to the occurrence of gender differences in WTP plants. Interestingly, however, Prokop & Fančovičová [10] did not find gender differences in WTP animals. More research with contrasted differences of selected plant species is required for more conclusive results.

5. Limitations

This study has one shortcoming: we did not experimentally manipulate the examined traits (spines, flowers), but we presented plant species with these traits and without them. We acknowledge that this approach does not allow for removal of potentially confounding effects due to differences in other characteristics among plant species. It is difficult to manipulate spines using Adobe Photoshop, but perhaps manipulation with similar traits such as thorns, which are less dense than spines, can be easily performed in future research.

6. Conclusions

Because of the lack of research in this area, we view our research as a first attempt to examine the essential characteristics of plants necessary to improve the willingness of people to protect them. The presence of aposematic signals, such as spines, seems to increase perceived danger (p < 0.001) and slightly increase the aesthetic value of plants (p = 0.002) but does not positively influence WTP plants (p = 0.03). In contrast, the aesthetic value of plants can easily be enhanced by the mere presence of colourful flowers (p < 0.001), which promote WTP plants (r = 0.60, p < 0.001). Students with high intrinsic interest in plants showed greater WTP plants (β = 0.16, p < 0.001), perceived plants as less dangerous (β = −0.08, p = 0.009) and perceived their aesthetic value more higher than less interested students (β = 0.17, p < 0.001). Interest in plants was compromised by the higher age of students (perceived attractiveness and WTP plants, both p < 0.05). We hope that these results can be used in conservation campaigns, where correctly selected flagship species may effectively promote conservation activities. For teaching botany, the use of aesthetically appealing species may contribute to increased interest in plants and WTP. For biology education research, we recommend examining whether (1) aposematic signals in plants increase retention similar to other survival-relevant scenarios, (2) perception of plants influences students’ behaviour toward them and (3) the focus on sexual reproduction in plants improves interest in plants in older students.

7. Educational Implications

Students are largely unaware of the ultimate function of aposematic signals in plants. Explaining the role of spines and thorns to them in defence against herbivores may naturally increase interest in plant topics because plants primed with animals enhance the participant’s recall of plant targets [57].
Increased attention to life-threatening injuries caused by plant appendages and associated bacteria may help to change the traditional view that plants are helpless organisms. Plants can instead be viewed as active components of natural selection.
Aesthetically appealing plant species may work as an umbrella for unappealing species. Explaining that aesthetically appealing flowers evolved because of insect attraction allows for explanations of why other wind-pollinated plants (e.g., grass) do not produce attractive flowers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, funding acquisition (J.F.), conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing—original draft, visualisation, writing—review and editing (P.P.), conceptualisation, validation, data collection (M.K.). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Trnava University, grant number B12/2022 and Scientific Grant Agency VEGA No. 1/0007/21.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Trnava University (204-17, 3 April 2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Raw data and visual material available upon request.

Acknowledgments

Three anonymous referees made valuable comments on an earlier draft. We thank David Livingstone for improving the English of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Correction Statement

This article has been republished with a minor correction to the Funding statement. This change does not affect the scientific content of the article.

References

  1. Voland, E.; Grammer, K. (Eds.) Evolutionary Aesthetics; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  2. Hoyer, W.D.; Stokburger-Sauer, N.E. The role of aesthetic taste in consumer behavior. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2012, 40, 167–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dion, K.; Berscheid, E.; Walster, E. What is beautiful is good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1972, 24, 285–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Langlois, J.H.; Kalakanis, L.; Rubenstein, A.J.; Larson, A.; Hallam, M.; Smoot, M. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 390–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Landová, E.; Poláková, P.; Rádlová, S.; Janovcová, M.; Bobek, M.; Frynta, D. Beauty ranking of mammalian species kept in the Prague Zoo: Does beauty of animals increase the respondents’ willingness to protect them? Sci. Nat. 2000, 105, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Prokop, P.; Randler, C. Biological predispositions and individual differences in human attitudes toward animals. In Ethnozoology: Animals in Our Lives; Alves, R.R.N., Albuquerque, U.P., Eds.; Academic Press: London, UK, 2018; pp. 447–466. [Google Scholar]
  7. Castillo-Huitrón, N.M.; Naranjo, E.J.; Santos-Fita, D.; Estrada-Lugo, E. The importance of human emotions for wildlife conservation. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gunnthorsdottir, A. Physical attractiveness of an animal species as a decision factor for its preservation. Anthrozoös 2001, 14, 204–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Knight, A.J. “Bats, snakes and spiders, Oh my!” How aesthetic and negativistic attitudes, and other concepts predict support for species protection. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 94–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Prokop, P.; Fančovičová, J. Does colour matter? The influence of animal warning coloration on human emotions and willingness to protect them. Anim. Cons. 2013, 16, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Prokop, P.; Fančovičová, J. Animals in dangerous postures enhance learning, but decrease willingness to protect animals. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2017, 13, 6069–6077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Small, E. The new Noah’s Ark: Beautiful and useful species only. Part 2. The chosen species. Biodiversity 2012, 13, 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Clucas, B.; McHugh, K.; Caro, T. Flagship species on covers of US conservation and nature magazines. Biodivers. Conserv. 2008, 17, 1517–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Sitas, N.; Baillie, J.E.M.; Isaac, N.J.B. What are we saving? Developing a standardized approach for conservation action. Anim. Cons. 2009, 12, 231–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Marešová, J.; Frynta, D. Noah’s Ark is full of common species attractive to humans: The case of boid snakes in zoos. Ecol. Econ. 2008, 64, 554–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Frynta, D.; Šimková, O.; Lišková, S.; Landová, E. Mammalian collection on Noah’s ark: The effects of beauty, brain and body size. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e63110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Marešová, J.; Antonín, K.; Frynta, D. We all appreciate the same animals: Cross-cultural comparison of human aesthetic preferences for snake species in Papua New Guinea and Europe. Ethology 2009, 115, 297–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Frynta, D.; Marešová, J.; Řeháková-Petrů, M.; Šklíba, J.; Šumbera, R.; Krása, A. Cross-cultural agreement in perception of animal beauty: Boid snakes viewed by people from five continents. Hum. Ecol. 2011, 39, 829–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Che, J.; Sun, X.; Gallardo, V.; Nadal, M. Cross-cultural empirical aesthetics. Prog. Brain Res. 2018, 237, 77–103. [Google Scholar]
  20. Baird, J.H.; Lazarowitz, R.; Allman, V. Science choices and preferences of middle and secondary school students in Utah. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1984, 21, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wandersee, J.H. Plants or animals–which do junior high school students prefer to study? J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1986, 23, 415–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kinchin, I.M. Investigating secondary-school girls’ preferences for animals or plants: A simple ‘head-to-head’ comparison using two unfamiliar organisms. J. Biol. Educ. 1999, 33, 95–99. [Google Scholar]
  23. Balas, B.; Momsen, J.L. Attention “blinks” differently for plants and animals. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 2014, 13, 437–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Balding, M.; Williams, K.J. Plant blindness and the implications for plant conservation. Cons. Biol. 2016, 30, 1192–1199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Martín-López, B.; Montes, C.; Benayas, J. The non-economic motives behind the willingness to pay for biodiversity conservation. Biol. Cons. 2007, 139, 67–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Laurentino, M.; de Lima Araújo, E.; Ramos, M.A.; Cavalcanti, M.C.B.T.; Gonçalves, P.H.S.; Albuquerque, U.P. Socioeconomic and ecological indicators in willingness to accept compensation for the conservation of medicinal plants in a tropical dry forest. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2022, 24, 4471–4489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wandersee, J.H.; Schussler, E.E. Preventing plant blindness. Amer. Biol. Teach. 1999, 61, 82–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wandersee, J.H.; Schussler, E.E. Toward a theory of plant blindness. Plant Sci. Bull. 2001, 47, 2–7. [Google Scholar]
  29. Parsley, K.M. Plant awareness disparity: A case for renaming plant blindness. Plants People Planet 2020, 2, 598–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gatt, S.; Tunnicliffe, S.D.; Borg, K.; Lautier, K. Young Maltese children’s ideas about plants. J. Biol. Educ. 2007, 41, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Schussler, E.E.; Olzak, L.A. It’s not easy being green: Student recall of plant and animal images. J. Biol. Educ. 2008, 42, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kubiatko, M.; Fančovičová, J.; Prokop, P. Factual knowledge of students about plants is associated with attitudes and interest in botany. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2021, 43, 1426–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Curtis, D.J. Using the arts to raise awareness and communicate environmental information in the extension context. J. Agric. Educ. Ext. 2011, 17, 181–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lev-Yadun, S.; Ne’Eman, G. When may green plants be aposematic? Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2004, 81, 413–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Inbar, M.; Lev-Yadun, S. Conspicuous and aposematic spines in the animal kingdom. Naturwissenschaften 2005, 92, 170–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lev-Yadun, S. Weapon (thorn) automimicry and mimicry of aposematic colorful thorns in plants. J. Theor. Biol. 2003, 224, 183–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lev-Yadun, S. Aposematic (warning) coloration in plants. In Plant-Environment Interactions. From Sensory Plant Biology to Active Plant Behavior; Baluška, F., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 167–202. [Google Scholar]
  38. Prokop, P.; Fančovičová, J. Seeing coloured fruits: Utilisation of the theory of adaptive memory in teaching botany. J. Biol. Educ. 2014, 48, 127–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Prokop, P.; Fančovičová, J. The perception of toxic and non-toxic plants by children and adolescents with regard to gender: Implications for teaching botany. J. Biol. Educ. 2019, 53, 463–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lindemann-Matthies, P.; Junge, X.; Matthies, D. The influence of plant diversity on people’s perception and aesthetic appreciation of grassland vegetation. Biol. Cons. 2010, 143, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Palmberg, I.; Berg, I.; Jeronen, E.; Kärkkäinen, S.; Norrgård-Sillanpää, P.; Persson, C.; Vilkonis, R.; Yli-Panula, E. Nordic–Baltic student teachers’ identification of and interest in plant and animal species: The importance of species identification and biodiversity for sustainable development. J. Sci. Teach. Educ. 2015, 26, 549–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hůla, M.; Flegr, J. What flowers do we like? The influence of shape and color on the rating of flower beauty. PeerJ 2016, 4, e2106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hůla, M.; Flegr, J. Habitat selection and human aesthetic responses to flowers. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2021, 3, e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Fančovičová, J.; Prokop, P. Development and initial psychometric assessment of the plant attitude questionnaire. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2010, 19, 415–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Meth. 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Bar, M.; Neta, M. Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 645–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Stefanowitsch, A. Words and their metaphors: A corpus-based approach. In Corpus-Based Approaches to Metaphor (Trends in Linguistics); Stefanowitsch, A., Gries, S.T., Eds.; Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany; New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 61–105. [Google Scholar]
  48. Grubb, P.J. A positive distrust in simplicity--lessons from plant defences and from competition among plants and among animals. J. Ecol. 1992, 80, 585–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hareli, S.; David, S.; Lev-Yadun, S.; Katzir, G. Money in your palm: Sharp shaped vegetation in the surroundings increase the subjective value of houses. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 46, 176–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Halpern, M.; Raats, D.; Lev-Yadun, S. Plant biological warfare: Thorns inject pathogenic bacteria into herbivores. Environ. Microbiol. 2007, 9, 584–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lev-Yadun, S.; Halpern, M. External and internal spines in plants insert pathogenic microorganisms into herbivore’s tissues for defense. In Microbial Ecology Research Trends; Van Dijk, T., Ed.; Nova Scientific Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 155–168. [Google Scholar]
  52. Bonin, P.; Thiebaut, G.; Prokop, P.; Méot, A. “In your head, zombie”: Zombies, predation and memory. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2019, 31, 635–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lindemann-Matthies, P. Investigating nature on the way to school: Responses to an educational programme by teachers and their pupils. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2006, 28, 895–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Lückmann, K.; Menzel, S. Herbs versus trees: Influences on teenagers’ knowledge of plant species. J. Biol. Educ. 2014, 48, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Fortenberry, J.D. Puberty and adolescent sexuality. Horm. Behav. 2013, 64, 280–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wilson, M.; Daly, M. Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1985, 6, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Zani, G.; Low, J. Botanical priming helps overcome plant blindness on a memory task. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 81, 101808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Examples of plants (a) with apparent spines (Melocactus oreas) and (b) without apparent spines (Turbinicarpus pseudopectinatus) used in the experiment.
Figure 1. Examples of plants (a) with apparent spines (Melocactus oreas) and (b) without apparent spines (Turbinicarpus pseudopectinatus) used in the experiment.
Sustainability 14 09121 g001
Figure 2. Comparison of perceived danger from plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Figure 2. Comparison of perceived danger from plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Sustainability 14 09121 g002
Figure 3. Comparison of perceived attractiveness of plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Figure 3. Comparison of perceived attractiveness of plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Sustainability 14 09121 g003
Figure 4. Comparison of willingness to protect (WTP) plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Figure 4. Comparison of willingness to protect (WTP) plants with respect to spines and flower occurrence. Means are shown with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Sustainability 14 09121 g004
Table 1. List of plants used in the research with percentage (%) of students who considered these plants dangerous, attractive and were willing to protect them *.
Table 1. List of plants used in the research with percentage (%) of students who considered these plants dangerous, attractive and were willing to protect them *.
Common English NameScientific NamePresence of Apparent SpinesFlower PresenceDanger (%)WTP (%)Attractiveness (%)
-Echinofossulocactus albatusyesyes19.74464.5
-Oreocereus fossulatusyesno65.738.530.8
Melon cactusMelocactus oreasyesno29.628.457
Moon cactusGymnocalycium mihanovichiiyesyes28.27370.7
Gray ghost organ pipeStenocereus pruinosusyesno43.424.931
MandacaruCereus jamacaruyesno6131.523.5
Parry’s agaveHuachuca agaveyesno2067.879.3
Mexican giant cardonPachycereus pringleiyesno50.528.433.6
Shaw’s agaveAgave shawiiyesno22.523.269.7
San Pedro cactusEchinopsis pachanoinono34.529.321.4
-Notocactus uebelmannianusnoyes12.469.774.9
-Turbinicarpus pseudopectinatusnono47.746.727.9
Sea urchinEuphorbia obesanono41.348.831.5
-Mammillaria pectiniferanono24.664.865.3
Common houseleekSempervivum tectorumnono6.343.767.8
Baseball plantEuphorbia obesanono20.751.642.5
-Matucana madisoniorumnoyes21.656.158.5
San Pedro cactusEchinopsis pachanoinono23.724.630.5
* Calculations based on “agree” and “strongly agree” responses.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fančovičová, J.; Prokop, P.; Kubíčková, M. The Effect of Aposematic Signals of Plants on Students’ Perception and Willingness to Protect Them. Sustainability 2022, 14, 9121. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159121

AMA Style

Fančovičová J, Prokop P, Kubíčková M. The Effect of Aposematic Signals of Plants on Students’ Perception and Willingness to Protect Them. Sustainability. 2022; 14(15):9121. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159121

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fančovičová, Jana, Pavol Prokop, and Markéta Kubíčková. 2022. "The Effect of Aposematic Signals of Plants on Students’ Perception and Willingness to Protect Them" Sustainability 14, no. 15: 9121. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159121

APA Style

Fančovičová, J., Prokop, P., & Kubíčková, M. (2022). The Effect of Aposematic Signals of Plants on Students’ Perception and Willingness to Protect Them. Sustainability, 14(15), 9121. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159121

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop