4.1. Results
Setting the gender as the grouping variable and the emotion activation efficiency as the test variable, an independent sample
t-test was carried out on the emotion activation efficacy of male and female participants. According to the
t-test results (
Table 4), there were significant differences in the activation efficiency of anger (
t = 2.953), fear (
t = −2.431), anxiety (
t = −2.320), and pleasure (
t = −3.188) between male and female participants. The differences in activation efficiency of other emotions between male and female participants were not significant.
The average activation efficiency (AAE) of anger, fear, anxiety, and pleasure in male and female participants was shown in
Figure 3. According to the statistics, the AAE of anger, fear, anxiety, and pleasure in male participants was 2.95 (SD = 1.36), 2.05 (SD = 1.53), 2.05 (SD = 1.41), and 2.20 (SD = 1.49), respectively, and the AAE of anger, fear, anxiety, and pleasure in female participants was 2.06 (SD = 1.20), 2.88 (SD = 1.36), 2.82 (SD = 1.42), and 3.21 (SD = 1.19), respectively.
One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was carried out in order to examine the role of participants’ age on the activation efficacy of different emotions. In the one-way ANOVA, the participants’ age was set as the factor variable, and the activation efficacy of different emotions was set as the dependent variable. The results (
Table 5) showed significant difference only in the activation efficiency of surprise among participants in different age groups (F = 4.399).
The AAE of the emotion surprise for participants in age groups 1, 2, 3, and 4 was 3.18 (SD = 1.43), 2.40 (SD = 1.43), 1.84 (SD = 1.21), and 1.38 (SD = 1.19), respectively. To further explore the impact of age on participants’ emotional activation efficacy, multiple comparison analysis (MCA) was performed on the activation efficacy of surprise for participants in different age groups. The MCA methods utilized in this study were Tukey B’s method and Waller–Duncan’s method. Multiple comparison results were considered valid only when the two methods achieved consistent results. The MCA results (
Figure 4 and
Table 6) showed no significant differences in the activation efficacy of surprise between participants in age groups 1 and 2, and no significant differences between participants in age groups 2, 3, and 4. The MCA results also showed significant differences in the activation efficacy of surprise between participants in age groups 1 and 3, and significant differences between participants in age groups 1 and 4.
Setting the driving competence as the factor variable and emotion activation efficiency as the dependent variable, one-way ANOVA was carried out in order to examine the effect of driving competence on the activation efficacy of different emotions. The results (
Table 7) showed significant differences in the activation efficacy of surprise (F = 5.146), helplessness (F = 3.223), contempt (F = 7.811), and relief (F = 4.491) between participants of different DC.
The AAE of surprise for participants with the driving competence levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 3.13 (SD = 1.73), 2.13 (SD = 1.46), 2.96 (SD = 1.13), 1.43 (SD = 1.22), and 1.44 (SD = 1.24), respectively. The MCA results (
Figure 5a and
Table 8) showed that there were significant differences in surprise activation efficiency between participants with DC levels 1 and 4, between participants with DC levels 1 and 5, between participants with DC levels 3 and 4, and between participants with DC levels 3 and 5. The MCA results also showed there was no significant difference in surprise activation efficiency between participants with DC levels 1, 2, and 3, and between participants with DC levels 2, 4 and 5.
The AAE of helplessness for participants with driving competence levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 2.13 (SD = 0.84), 2.63 (SD = 1.03), 2.22 (SD = 1.55), 1.93 (SD =1.33), and 0.78 (SD = 0.83), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 5b and
Table 8), there were significant differences in the activation efficacy of helplessness between participants of DC levels 1 and 5, between participants of DC levels 2 and 5, and between participants of DC levels 3 and 5. There was no significant difference in the activation efficacy of helplessness between participants of DC levels 1, 2, 3, and 4 and between participants of DC levels 4 and 5.
According to statistics, the AAE of contempt for participants with driving competence levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 0.88 (SD = 1.13), 1.38 (SD = 0.89), 1.89 (SD = 1.42), 2.86 (SD = 1.03), and 3.56 (SD = 1.59), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 5c and
Table 8), there were significant differences in the activation efficacy of contempt between participants of DC levels 1 and 4, between participants of DC levels 1 and 5, between participants of DC levels 2 and 4, between participants of DC levels 2 and 5, and between participants of DC levels 3 and 5. There was no significant difference in the activation efficacy of contempt between participants of DC levels 1, 2, and 3, between participants of DC levels 3 and 4, or between participants of DC levels 4 and 5.
The AAE of relief for participants with driving competence levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 2.25 (SD = 1.17), 2.00 (SD = 1.03), 3.11 (SD = 1.45), 3.21 (SD = 1.42), and 4.00 (SD = 1.00), respectively. The MCA results (
Figure 5d and
Table 8) showed significant differences in the activation efficacy of relief between participants of DC levels 1 and 5 and between participants of DC levels 2 and 5. The MCA results showed no significant difference in the activation efficacy of relief between participants of DC levels 1, 2, 3, and 4, and between participants of DC levels 3, 4, and 5.
In order to test the impacts of driving safety attitude on emotion activation efficiency, the DSA was set as the factor variable, and the emotion activation efficiency was set as the dependent variable for the carried-out one-way ANOVAs. The results (
Table 9) suggested that there were significant differences in the activation efficiency of anger (F = 3.038), contempt (F = 2.847), and pleasure (F = 4.069) between participants of different driving safety attitudes.
According to the statistical results, the AAE of anger for participants of DSA levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 3.13 (SD = 1.64), 3.29 (SD = 1.20), 2.54 (SD = 1.00), 2.12 (SD = 1.36), and 1.57 (SD = 1.81), respectively. To further test the impacts of DSA on emotion activation efficacy, an MCA was performed on the activation efficacy of anger for participants of DSA. According to the MCA results (
Figure 6a and
Table 10), there were significant differences in anger activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 1 and 5 and between participants of DSA levels 2 and 5. The MCA results also indicated that there was no significant difference in anger activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 1, 2, 3, and 4, and between participants of DSA levels 3, 4, and 5.
The AAE of contempt for participants of DSA levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 3.38 (SD = 1.51), 2.36 (SD = 1.28), 1.96 (SD = 1.37), 1.47 (SD = 1.55), and 1.71 (SD = 1.11), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 6b and
Table 10), there were significant differences in contempt activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 1 and 4 and between participants of DSA levels 1 and 5. There was no significant difference in contempt activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 2, 3, 4, and 5, or between participants of DSA levels 1, 2, and 3.
The average activation efficiency of pleasure for participants of DSA levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.38 (SD = 1.06), 2.36 (SD = 1.28), 2.50 (SD = 1.32), 3.35 (SD = 1.41), and 3.57 (SD = 1.62), respectively. The MCA results (
Figure 6c and
Table 10) indicate significant differences in pleasure activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 1 and 4 and between participants of DSA levels 1 and 5. There was no significant difference in pleasure activation efficacy between participants of DSA levels 1, 2, and 3 or participants of DSA levels 2, 3, 4, and 5.
With the aim of testing the impacts of driving anger tendency on emotion activation efficiency, one-way ANOVA was carried out by setting the DAT as the factor variable and the emotion activation efficiency as the dependent variable. The results (
Table 10) indicated that there were significant differences in the activation efficacy of anger (F = 4.834) and anxiety (F = 3.078) between participants of different DAT. However, there were no significant differences in the activation efficacy of surprise (F = 0.524), fear (F = 1.414), helplessness (F = 0.293), contempt (F = 0.360), relief (F = 0.681), or pleasure (F = 2.284) between participants of different DAT.
The average activation efficiency of anger for participants of DAT levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.44 (SD = 1.42), 2.11 (SD = 1.37), 2.54 (SD = 0.83), 3.27 (SD = 1.34), and 3.50 (SD = 1.51), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 7a and
Table 11), there were significant differences in anger activation efficacy between participants of DAT levels 1 and 4 and between participants of DAT levels 1 and 5 and between participants of DAT levels 2 and 5. Additionally, there was no significant difference in anger activation efficacy between participants of DAT levels 1, 2, and 3, between participants of DAT levels 2, 3, and 4, or between participants of DAT levels 3, 4, and 5.
The AAE of anxiety for participants of DAT levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.67 (SD = 1.23), 1.83 (SD = 1.58), 2.38 (SD = 1.35), 2.93 (SD = 1.39), and 3.50 (SD = 1.07), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 7a and
Table 11), there were significant differences in anxiety activation efficacy between participants of DAT levels 1 and 5 and between participants of DAT levels 2 and 5. In addition, there was no significant difference in anxiety activation efficacy between participants of DAT levels 1, 2, 3, and 4 or between participants of DAT levels 3, 4, and 5 (
Table 12).
Setting the stress state as the factor variable and the emotion activation efficiency as the dependent variable, one-way ANOVA was carried out in order to examine the impacts of stress state on emotion activation efficiency.
The results (
Table 13) suggested that there were significant differences in the activation efficiency of anger (F = 9.351), fear (F = 2.812), anxiety (F = 11.315), helplessness (F = 4.528), relief (F = 4.399), and pleasure (F = 9.073). The AAE of anger for participants of stress state levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.57 (SD = 1.22), 1.93 (SD = 1.39), 2.45 (SD = 0.83), 3.29 (SD = 1.14), and 3.91 (SD = 1.04), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 8a and
Table 14), there were significant differences in anger activation efficacy between the participants of SS levels 1 and 4, between the participants of SS levels 1 and 5, between the participants of SS levels 2 and 4, between the participants of SS levels 2 and 5, and between the participants of SS levels 3 and 5. In addition, there was no significant difference in anger activation efficacy between the participants of SS levels 1, 2 and 3 between the participants of SS levels 3 and 4, or between the participants of SS levels 4 and 5.
The AAE of fear for participants of stress state levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.71 (SD = 1.44), 1.93 (SD = 1.28), 2.60 (SD = 1.39), 2.57 (SD = 1.65), and 3.45 (SD = 1.44), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 8b and
Table 14), there were significant differences in fear activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1 and 5, and between participants of SS levels 2 and 5. There was no significant difference in fear activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1, 2, 3, and 4 or between participants of SS levels 3, 4, and 5.
The average activation efficiency of anxiety for participants of SS levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.21 (SD = 1.25), 2.07 (SD = 1.39), 2.60 (SD = 1.14), 2.14 (SD = 1.17), and 4.27 (SD = 0.65), respectively. The MCA results (
Figure 8c and
Table 14) showed significant differences in anxiety activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1 and 3, between participants of SS levels 1 and 5, between participants of SS levels 2 and 5, between participants of SS levels 3 and 5, and between participants of SS levels 4 and 5. There was no significant difference in anxiety activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1, 2, and 4 or between participants of SS levels 2, 3, and 4.
The AAE of helplessness for participants of SS levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 1.57 (SD = 1.22), 1.20 (SD = 0.94), 2.25 (SD = 1.12), 2.50 (SD = 1.35), and 3.00 (SD = 1.61), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 8d and
Table 14), there were significant differences in the activation efficacy of helplessness between participants of SS levels 1 and 5, between participants of SS level 2 and 4, and between participants of SS levels 2 and 5. Meanwhile, there was no significant difference in the activation efficacy of helplessness between participants of SS levels 1, 2, and 3, between participants of SS levels 1, 3, and 4, or between participants of SS levels 3, 4, and 5.
The AAE of relief for participants of SS levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 4.07 (SD = 0.83), 3.33 (SD = 1.18), 2.75 (SD = 1.52), 2.50 (SD = 1.09), and 1.64 (SD = 1.21), respectively. The MCA results (
Figure 8e and
Table 14) demonstrated that there were significant differences in relief activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1 and 3, between participants of SS levels 1 and 4, between participants of SS levels 1 and 5, and between participants of SS levels 2 and 5. There was no significant difference in relief activation efficacy between participants of SS levels 1 and 2, between participants of SS levels 2, 3, 4, and 3, or between participants of SS levels 4 and 5.
The average activation efficiency of pleasure for participants of SS levels 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 was 3.93 (SD = 1.00), 2.93 (SD = 1.53), 2.75 (SD = 1.16), 2.07 (SD = 1.21), and 1.18 (SD = 1.45), respectively. According to the MCA results (
Figure 8f and
Table 14), there were significant differences in the activation efficacy of pleasure between participants of SS levels 1 and 3, between participants of SS levels 1 and 4, between participants of SS levels 1 and 5, between participants of SS levels 2 and 5, and between participants of SS levels 3 and 5, while there was no significant difference in the activation efficacy of pleasure between participants of SS levels 1 and 2, participants of SS levels 4 and 5, or between participants of SS levels 2, 3, and 4.
In order to verify that the results obtained in the experiment were indeed caused by a single variable, we further conducted a test analysis to exclude the influence of other factors. The details are as follows:
To further examine the effect of gender on the activation efficacy of anger, fear, anxiety, and pleasure, the independent sample
t-test was carried out setting the gender as the grouping variable and the other individual characteristics as the test variable. The results (
Table 14) showed that there were significant differences in DSA (
t = −2.745) and DAT (
t = 2.201) between male and female participants, suggesting that the differences in emotion activation efficiency between male and female participants may also be caused by the DSA or DAT. By setting the DSA and DAT as the manipulated variable, the partial correlation between gender and the activation efficacy of anger, fear, anxiety, and pleasure was tested (
Table 15). The results showed that gender was only significantly associated with the activation efficacy of fear (pc = 0.304) and anxiety (pc = 0.417) after excluding the effects of DSA and DAT. These results suggested that female drivers are more prone to fear and anxiety, which was in agreement with the view that females are more likely to be disturbed by anxiety [
30].
One-way ANOVA was carried out setting the age as the factor variable and the other individual features as test variables. The results (
Table 16) showed no significant difference in individual features for participants of different ages, indicating that age was the factor leading to significant differences in surprise activation efficiency for participants of different ages. It could be deduced that the increase of age decreased the surprise activation efficiency based on MCA results (
Table 17).
A further one-way ANOVA was carried out setting the DC as the factor variable and the other individual characteristics as the test variable. The results (
Table 18) revealed significant differences in driving safety attitude (F = 2.958) for participants of different DC, which indicated that the difference of emotion activation efficacy for participants of different DC may also be caused by driving safety attitude. Setting the driving safety attitude as the manipulated variable, the partial correlation between driving competence and the emotional activation efficiencies of surprise, helplessness, contempt, and relief was tested (
Table 19). The partial correlation results showed that driving competence was significantly correlated with the activation efficiency of surprise (PC = −0.317), helplessness (PC = −0.311), contempt (PC = 0.577), and relief (PC = 0.425) after eliminating the influence of driving safety attitude, which indicated that driving competence was an influence on the activation efficacy of the above four emotions. It can be concluded that, with improvement of the DC, the activation efficiency of surprise and helplessness experienced a downward trend, while the activation efficiency of contempt and relief showed an upward trend.
To further test the effect of DSA on participants’ emotion activation efficacy, one-way ANOVA was carried out setting the DSA as the factor variable and the other individual characteristics as the test variable. The results (
Table 20) showed that there was no significant difference in the individual characteristics of participants of different DSA, which indicated that driving safety attitude was the key influencing factor that led to the significant difference in the activation efficiency of anger, contempt, and pleasure. According to the MCA results, it can be deduced that the better the driving safety attitude was, the lower the activation efficiency of anger and contempt was and the higher the activation efficiency of pleasure was.
Setting the DAT as the factor variable and the other individual characteristics as the test variable, one-way ANOVA was carried out to further verify the effects of driving anger tendency on emotion activation efficacy. The results (
Table 21) suggested that DAT was significantly correlated with anger (PC = 0.313) and anxiety (PC = 0.244) after excluding the influence of stress state, which indicated that DAT was the influencing factor for the activation efficacy of anger and anxiety. The results (
Table 22) showed that there were significant differences in the stress state (F = 3.817) of participants of different driving anger tendencies, which suggested that it is also possible that the difference of emotion activation efficacy for participants of different DAT was caused by stress state. Setting the stress state as the manipulated variable, the partial correlation between DAT and the activation efficacy of anger and anxiety was tested. This led to the conclusion that the level of DAT was directly proportional to the activation efficiency of anger and anxiety based on the MCA results (
Table 21).
Setting the stress state as the factor variable and the other individual characteristics as test variables, a one-way ANOVA was carried out to further test the impacts of stress state on emotion activation efficiency. The results (
Table 23) showed that there were significant differences in anger tendency (F = 3.753) for participants of different stress states, which suggested that the difference of emotion activation efficacy between participants of different SS may also be caused by DAT. Setting the DAT as a manipulated variable, the partial correlation between SS and the activation efficacies of anger, fear, anxiety, helplessness, relief, and pleasure was tested. The results (
Table 24) suggested that SS was significantly correlated with anger (PC = 0.496), fear (PC = 0.330), anxiety (PC = 0.450), helplessness (PC = 0.399), ease (PC = −0.521), and pleasure (PC = −0.548) after eliminating the influence of DAT. The results indicated that the stress state was the influencing factor of the above six emotion activation efficacies. Therefore, it can be inferred that with the increase of stress level, the activation efficiencies of anger, fear, anxiety, and helplessness showed an upward trend, while the stress state was inversely proportional to the activation efficacies of relief and pleasure according to the MCA results.
4.2. Discussions
In this study, we analyzed the five influencing factors of the eight emotional activation efficacies, respectively. We determined by t-test or one-way ANOVA that the activation efficacy of each emotion was caused by the variable we selected and not related to other factors. However, it is sometimes impossible to exclude all of the interference factors only through the t-test or one-way ANOVA of a single factor on the emotional activation efficacy, and there may be other factors not selected by us in the results that also affect the emotional activation efficacy. Therefore, we further used the selected variables as grouping variables and other variables that may cause interference as test variables and conducted independent-sample t-test or one-way ANOVA. If this process was able to eliminate all interfering factors, then it meant that the influencing factors we selected were the only ones that had an impact on the efficacy of emotional activation. If there were still interfering factors that could not be excluded in this process, then we selected the variables that cannot be excluded as control variables in order to test their partial correlation with the subjects’ emotional activation efficacy. Interfering factors were excluded by partial correlation test. The conclusion shows that, except for the variables we selected, the activation efficacy of emotion is not related to other variables.
Although some scholars’ studies have revealed the relationship between emotions and different characteristics of people, there is no research on the relationship between emotions and drivers’ characteristics in the field of transportation. Our study comprehensively investigated the differences in the efficacy of emotional activation among drivers of different genders, ages, driving abilities, driving safety attitudes, driving anger tendencies, and stressful states.
Compared with male drivers, female drivers have higher anxiety activation efficacy, indicating that female drivers are more prone to anxiety. This is consistent with the conclusion drawn by Burani K. et al. [
57] that women are more likely to be disturbed by anxiety than men.
Age is an influential factor leading to differences in the activation efficacy of surprise. The activation of surprise decreased with increasing age. However, the activation efficacy of surprise only showed significant difference when the age difference is large. This is due to the increased driving experience as the driver gets older, resulting in a more traffic-experienced drivers who are less likely to be surprised. However, the accumulation of experience takes a long time to complete, so the difference in the activation efficacy of surprise only occurs when the age difference is large.
Drivers with different driving abilities have different activation effects of surprise, helplessness, contempt, and relief. With the improvement of driving competence, the driver’s emotion activation efficacy for surprise and helplessness decreases, while the activation efficacy for contempt and relief increases. The greater the difference in driving competence, the greater the difference in the activation efficacy of helplessness and contempt. The higher the driver’s driving competence, the more confident he will be in the process of driving, and more he will be able to deal with various driving scenarios calmly. This results in the driver being less likely to feel surprised and helpless. When the driver has a high driving competence, he becomes too confident in himself, which makes him more prone to contempt. The activation efficacy of relief is only significantly different when the driving competence gap is too large. For example, the relief of driving is very different between a novice driver and an experienced driver. Novice drivers tend to have low driving competence and drive cautiously, while experienced drivers usually have high driving competence and can drive easily.
Drivers with different driving safety attitudes have different activation effects for anger, contempt, and pleasure. The higher the level of driving safety attitude, the lower the activation efficacy of the driver’s anger and contempt and the higher the activation efficacy of pleasure. However, the activation efficacy of the three emotions are significantly different only when the driving safety attitudes are quite different. Drivers who pay more attention to driving safety have higher willingness and ability to control their negative emotions during driving and are less susceptible to negative emotions. Therefore, people with higher levels of driving safety attitude are less likely to experience anger and contempt, but more likely to be pleasant.
Drivers with different driving anger tendencies have different activation efficacies for anger and anxiety. This conclusion is consistent with research on road rage. The greater the driver’s tendency to anger, the more likely he is to be angry, which is accompanied by anxiety. However, the differences in the activation efficacy of anxiety among people with a small differences in anger tendencies are not large.
Drivers in different stress states have different activation efficacies for anger, fear, anxiety, helplessness, relief, and pleasure. When the driver’s stress state level increases, the activation efficacies of anger, fear, anxiety, and helplessness are higher, and the activation efficacies of relief and pleasure are lower. However, the activation efficacies of fear and relief are significantly different only when the difference in stress state is large. The reason for this is that as the level of the driver’s stress state increases, negative emotions are more likely to be activated, and positive emotions are less likely to be activated. Slight changes in stress state level have little effect on fear and relief. Once the stress state is too high, the driver will easily appear fearful and will rarely appear relaxed.
The research method for emotional activation efficacy proposed in this paper is comprehensive. The factors influencing the activation efficacy of the eight emotions have all undergone detailed and in-depth research, through which the relationship between the activation efficacy of each emotion and the characteristics of drivers is revealed. This provides an effective reference method for accurately measuring drivers’ emotions in driving experiments, and is of great significance for advancing research on driving emotions. However, we only analyzed the relationship between emotion activation efficacy and drivers with different characteristics in a qualitative manner, and we do not further propose the quantitative relationship between emotion activation efficacy and driver characteristics. Subsequent research can further carry out driving experiments in order to study the quantitative relationship between emotion and driver characteristics. It can be improved by measuring emotion and eliminating interfering factors in driving emotion experiments.