‘I Do It for Others’! Prosocial Reasons for Complying with Anti-COVID Measures and Pro-Environmental Behaviours: The Mediating Role of the Psychological Distance of Climate Change
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Reasons for Engaging in Anti-COVID Measures
1.2. Perceived Interdependencies
1.3. Psychological Distance
1.4. Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Procedure and Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Independent Variables
2.2.2. Mediating Processes
2.2.3. Dependent Variables
2.2.4. Psychological Control Variables
- Human values are known to impact individuals’ tendency to cheat vs. follow rules [43,44], their tendency to worry for others [45], and the willingness to engage in PEB [25,26]. To avoid a too long questionnaire, we focused on values that were particularly relevant in our case: conservation values (i.e., conformity to rules, personal security, and society’s security), universalism people and nature, and benevolence. They were measured with 12 items adapted from Schwartz and colleagues’ scale [46]. A component factor analysis, with oblimin rotation, showed that three factors obtained an eigenvalue superior to 1 (see Supplementary Material S3). First, the six conservation items (i.e., confr1, confr2, secp1, secp2, secs1, and secs2) were loaded on a first factor (α = 0.88; αCH = 0.88, αFR = 0.86, αSP = 0.89, αUK = 0.90). Then, the four universalism items (related to people and nature together; i.e., unic1, unic2, unin1, and unin2) were loaded on the same factor (α = 0.82; αCH = 0.76, αFR = 0.85, αSP = 0.75, αUK = 0.85). Finally, the two benevolence items (i.e., benc1 and benc2) were loaded on a separate and third factor (r = 0.75, p < 0.001; rCH = 0.71, rFR = 0.78, rSP = 0.72, rUK = 0.78).
- Individuals may have reported that they were engaged in anti-COVID measures to help others and/or in PEB because they knew that these behaviours are generally expected. For this reason, we also controlled for both general and environmental social desirability. Participants’ general social desirability (i.e., desgen1 to desgen8) was measured with the eight items of the Self-deceptive Enhancement subscale from Bobbio and Manganelli [47]. The French version was adapted from D’Amour-Raymond [48], and the items were translated to Spanish for the present study. A principal component analysis (Supplementary Material S3) revealed only one factor with an eigenvalue superior to 1 (α = 0.78; αCH = 0.74, αFR = 0.81, αSP = 0.76, αUK = 0.82). Environmental social desirability was measured with five items selected and translated (French and Spanish) from the Self-deception−assertion of positives subscale of Ewert and Galloway [49] A principal component analysis, with an oblimin rotation (see Supplementary Material S3), revealed that one item was loaded on another factor (sdesenv5). Scale reliability (α = 0.61) was indeed sufficient only when this item was not taken into account (α = 0.74; αCH = 0.74, αFR = 0.71, αSP = 0.79, αUK = 0.70).
- To partially account for the potential impact of individuals’ perception of the COVID-19 crisis on their perceptions of climate change, we asked participants to estimate whether they felt less or more worried about the latter because of the former, on a scale from 1 (much less worried) to 6 (much more worried).
3. Results
3.1. Mediating Variables
3.2. Predicting Membership to the Five Profiles
3.3. Indirect Paths
- Personal reasons to comply with anti-COVID measures were negatively related to the ‘strongly committed’ profile (β = −0.02, p = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.04, −0.00]), and positively related to the ‘strongly disengaged’ profile (β = 0.02, p = 0.03, 95% CI [0.00, 0.05]), through the psychological distance of climate change.
- A reduced personal psychological distance of COVID was positively related to the ‘strongly committed’ profile (β = 0.03, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.02, 0.05]), and negatively to the ‘strongly disengaged’ profile (β = −0.04, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.06, −0.03]), through the psychological distance of climate change. It was also negatively related to the ‘well-meaning’ (β = −0.01, p = 0.03, 95% CI [−0.02, −0.00]), and positively to the ‘committed to private PEB’ profile (β = 0.01, p = 0.03, 95% CI [0.00, 0.02]), through perceived interdependencies between the self and others.
- Finally, a reduced general psychological distance of COVID-19 was positively related to the ‘strongly committed’ profile (β = 0.02, p = 0.002, 95% CI [0.01, 0.04]), and negatively to the ‘strongly disengaged’ profile (β = −0.03, p = 0.001, 95% CI [−0.05, −0.01]), through a reduced psychological distance of climate change.
4. Discussion
4.1. Congruent vs. Incongruent Environmental Profiles
4.2. The Psychological Distance of the Two Crises
4.3. Perceived Interdependences between the Self and Others
4.4. Limitations of the Present Study
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Georgieva, I.; Lantta, T.; Lickiewicz, J.; Pekara, J.; Wikman, S.; Loseviča, M.; Lepping, P. Perceived effectiveness, restrictiveness, and compliance with containment measures against the COVID-19 pandemic: An international comparative study in 11 countries. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouman, T.; Steg, L.; Dietz, T. Insights from early COVID-19 responses about promoting sustainable action. Nat. Sustain. 2021, 4, 194–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masson-Delmotte, V.; Zhai, P.; Pirani, A.; Connors, S.L.; Péan, C.; Berger, S.; Caud, N.; Chen, Y.; Goldfarb, L.; Gomis, M.I.; et al. (Eds.) Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Summary for Policymakers. In Climate Change 2021: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Hornsey, M.J.; Fielding, K.S. Understanding (and reducing) inaction on climate change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2020, 14, 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geiger, N.; Gore, A.; Squire, C.V.; Attari, S.Z. Investigating similarities and differences in individual reactions to the COVID-19 pandemic and the climate crisis. Clim. Change 2021, 167, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manzanedo, R.D.; Manning, P. COVID-19: Lessons for the climate change emergency. Sci. Total Environ. 2020, 742, 140563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization (WHO). WHO Director-General’s Opening Remarks at the Media Briefing on COVID-19—11 March 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/director-general/speeches/detail/who-director-general-s-opening-remarks-at-the-media-briefing-on-COVID-19—11-March-2020 (accessed on 17 July 2023).
- Atkinson-Clement, C.; Pigalle, E. What can we learn from COVID-19 pandemic’s impact on human behaviour? The case of France’s lockdown. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021, 8, 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pagliaro, S.; Sacchi, S.; Pacilli, M.G.; Brambilla, M.; Lionetti, F.; Bettache, K.; Zubieta, E. Trust predicts COVID-19 prescribed and discretionary behavioral intentions in 23 countries. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0248334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lalot, F.; Abrams, D.; Ahvenharju, S.; Minkkinen, M. Being future-conscious during a global crisis: The protective effect of heightened Futures Consciousness in the COVID-19 pandemic. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021, 178, 110862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bargain, O.; Aminjonov, U. Trust and compliance to public health policies in times of COVID-19. J. Public Econ. 2020, 192, 104316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borgonovi, F.; Andrieu, E. Bowling together by bowling alone: Social capital and COVID-19. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 265, 113501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coroiu, A.; Moran, C.; Campbell, T.; Geller, A.C. Barriers and facilitators of adherence to social distancing recommendations during COVID-19 among a large international sample of adults. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Graham, J.; Nosek, B.A.; Haidt, J.; Iyer, R.; Koleva, S.; Ditto, P.H. Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 366–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lange, F.; Dewitte, S. Measuring pro-environmental behavior: Review and recommendations. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 63, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, S.E. Self-Construal. In The Wiley Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences: Personality Processes and Individual Differences; Carducci, B.J., Nave, C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022; pp. 381–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 224–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brewer, M.B.; Chen, Y.R. Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 133–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardner, W.L.; Gabriel, S.; Lee, A.Y. “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 10, 321–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bertozzi, A.L.; Franco, E.; Mohler, G.; Short, M.B.; Sledge, D. The challenges of modeling and forecasting the spread of COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 16732–16738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, P.C. New environmental theories: Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reese, G.; Jacob, L. Principles of environmental justice and pro-environmental action: A two-step process model of moral anger and responsibility to act. Environ. Sci. Policy 2015, 51, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reese, G. Common human identity and the path to global climate justice. Clim. Change 2016, 134, 521–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S.H. An overview of the Schwartz Theory of Basic Values. Online Read. Psychol. Cult. 2012, 2, 1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.I.; Steg, L. Value orientations to explain beliefs related to environmental significant behavior: How to measure egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric value orientations. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 330–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung, W.Y.; Luke, M.A.; Maio, G.R. On attitudes towards humanity and climate change: The effects of humanity esteem and self-transcendence values on environmental concerns. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 44, 496–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liberman, N.; Trope, Y. The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science 2008, 322, 1201–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 440–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDonald, R.I.; Chai, H.Y.; Newell, B.R. Personal experience and the ‘psychological distance’ of climate change: An integrative review. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 44, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maiella, R.; La Malva, P.; Marchetti, D.; Pomarico, E.; Di Crosta, A.; Palumbo, R.; Verrocchio, M.C. The psychological distance and climate change: A systematic review on the mitigation and adaptation behaviors. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 568899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, E.; Marsh, J.E.; Richardson, B.H.; Ball, L.J. A systematic review of the psychological distance of climate change: Towards the development of an evidence-based construct. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 81, 101822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Lange, P.A.; Huckelba, A.L. Psychological distance: How to make climate change less abstract and closer to the self. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 42, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guo, Y.; Hou, Y. COVID-19 pandemic as an opportunity or challenge: Applying psychological distance theory and the co-benefit frame to promote public support for climate change mitigation on social media. Environ. Comm. 2023; advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochachka, G. Unearthing insights for climate change response in the midst of the COVD.19 pandemic. Glob. Sustain. 2020, 3, e33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botzen, W.; Duijndam, S.; van Beukering, P. Lessons for climate policy from behavioral biases toward COVID-19 and climate change risks. World Dev. 2021, 137, 105214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Capstick, S.; Whitmarsh, L.; Poortinga, W.; Pidgeon, N.; Upham, P. International trends in public perceptions of climate change over the past quarter century. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. 2015, 6, 35–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evensen, D.; Whitmarsh, L.; Bartie, P.; Devine-Wright, P.; Dickie, J.; Varley, A.; Mayer, A. Effect of “finite pool of worry” and COVID-19 on UK climate change perceptions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2018936118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osborne, D.; Sibley, C.G. Identifying "types" of ideologies and intergroup biases: Advancing a person-centred approach to social psychology. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 28, 288–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hine, D.W.; Reser, J.P.; Morrison, M.; Phillips, W.J.; Nunn, P.; Cooksey, R. Audience segmentation and climate change communication: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. 2014, 5, 441–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoemann, A.M.; Boulton, A.J.; Short, S.D. Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Rev 2017, 8, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, S.; Hurlstone, M.J.; Leviston, Z.; Walker, I.; Lawrence, C. Climate change from a distance: An analysis of construal level and psychological distance from climate change. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tein, J.Y.; Coxe, S.; Cham, H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct. Equ. Model. 2013, 20, 640–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pulfrey, C.; Butera, F. Why neoliberal values of self-enhancement lead to cheating in higher education: A motivational account. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2153–2162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pulfrey, C.; Durussel, K.; Butera, F. The good cheat: Benevolence and the justification of collective cheating. J. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 110, 764–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S.H.; Sagiv, L.; Boehnke, K. Worries and values. J. Personal. 2020, 68, 309–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwartz, S.H.; Cieciuch, J.; Vecchione, M.; Davidov, E.; Fischer, R.; Beierlein, C.; Konty, M. Refining the theory of basic individual values. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 663–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bobbio, A.; Manganelli, A.M. Measuring social desirability responding. A short version of Paulhus’ BIDR 6. TPM Test. Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 2011, 18, 117–135. Available online: http://www.tpmap.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/11/18.2.4.pdf (accessed on 18 July 2023).
- D’Amours-Raymond, J. Version Abrégée Transculturelle du Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR). Master’s Thesis, Faculté des Études Supérieures de l’Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, 2011. Available online: https://dam-oclc.bac-lac.gc.ca/download?is_thesis=1&oclc_number=1033091278&id=6188d607-7906-4e82-ab24-1ed2ce37e2f4&fileName=28117.pdf (accessed on 21 July 2023).
- Ewert, A.; Galloway, G. Socially desirable responding in an environmental context: Development of a domain specific scale. Environ. Educ. Res. 2009, 15, 55–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamberg, S.; Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klöckner, C.A. A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour—A meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 2013, 23, 1028–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lou, X.; Li, L.M.W. The relationship of environmental concern with public and private pro-environmental behaviours: A pre-registered meta-analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 53, 2879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graton, A.; Mailliez, M.; Hahnel, U.J. Does reference to COVID-19 improve climate change communication? Investigating the influence of emotions and uncertainty in persuasion messages. Compr. Results Soc. Psychol. 2020, 4, 267–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Valkengoed, A.M.; Steg, L. Meta-analyses of factors motivating climate change adaptation behaviour. Nat. Clim. Change 2019, 9, 158–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
General | Switzerland | France | Spain | UK | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | |
Male participants % | 26.16% | 20.57 | 27.98% | 31.37% | 27.45% |
Age | 45.29 (16.48) | 36.12 (14.08) | 49.81 (13.30) | 44.99 (16.86) | 54.42 (15.83) |
Occupation | |||||
Employed | 59.21% | 61.81% | 68.37% | 53.16% | 49.48% |
Studying | 15.26% | 29.82% | 5.10% | 9.49% | 8.85% |
Access to outside | 86.14% | 85.76% | 84.44% | 80.00% | 94.47% |
General | Switzerland | France | Spain | UK | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | M SD/% | |
Reasons to comply with anti-COVID measures | |||||
Prosocial | 5.20 (1.00) | 5.25 (0.80) | 5.08 (1.14) | 5.23 (1.04) | 5.27 (1.05) |
Personal | 4.85 (1.36) | 4.79 (1.29) | 4.82 (1.44) | 4.79 (1.36) | 5.03 (1.37) |
Perceived interdependencies | |||||
Other-related self | 4.34 (0.79) | 4.47 (0.72) | 4.15 (0.85) | 4.35 (0.76) | 4.37 (0.84) |
PD: climate change | 4.70 (1.00) | 4.66 (0.86) | 4.59 (1.18) | 4.85 (0.89) | 4.73 (1.04) |
PD: COVID-19 | |||||
Personal | 4.33 (1.16) | 4.11 (1.07) | 4.22 (1.27) | 4.44 (1.14) | 4.70 (1.08) |
General | 5.27 (1.06) | 5.24 (0.99) | 5.09 (1.15) | 5.30 (1.06) | 5.50 (1.01) |
Symptoms (% yes) | |||||
Personal | 12.88% | 10.79% | 10.74% | 17.16% | 14.36% |
Close others | 50.41% | 55.87% | 41.15% | 52.94% | 50.49% |
COVID worry | 4.07 (1.30) | 3.96 (1.19) | 4.05 (1.42) | 4.58 (1.21) | 3.74 (1.29) |
Human values | |||||
Conservation | 4.45 (1.07) | 4.48 (0.91) | 4.36 (1.19) | 4.52 (1.12) | 4.47 (1.11) |
Universalism | 5.49 (0.69) | 5.52 (0.59) | 5.40 (0.84) | 5.62 (0.52) | 5.45 (0.77) |
Benevolence | 5.59 (0.69) | 5.62 (0.61) | 5.48 (0.82) | 5.69 (0.59) | 5.58 (0.71) |
Social desirability | |||||
General | 3.90 (0.81) | 3.85 (0.74) | 4.15 (0.87) | 3.86 (0.74) | 3.75 (0.84) |
Environmental | 4.51 (0.94) | 4.45 (0.91) | 4.61 (0.93) | 4.54 (1.03) | 4.47 (0.91) |
Self-rated impact of COVID crisis on concern for climate change | |||||
4.24 (1.30) | 4.17 (1.13) | 4.34 (1.50) | 4.55 (1.24) | 3.92 (1.25) | |
Self-reported PEB 1 | |||||
Educating oneself | 53.12% | 53.35% | 57.02% | 56.72% | 44.61% |
Signing petition | 47.64% | 47.92% | 48.33% | 50.25% | 43.84% |
Talking | 49.95% | 51.12% | 51.24% | 54.77% | 41.87% |
Lowering heating | 68.30% | 53.50% | 79.34% | 79.31% | 67.00% |
Pro-environmental intentions 2 | |||||
Only local products | 4.56 (1.63) | 5.07 (1.24) | 5.02 (1.50) | 4.34 (1.66) | 3.46 (1.71) |
Giving up plane | 3.87 (1.89) | 4.02 (1.74) | 4.36 (1.90) | 3.48 (1.90) | 3.45 (1.92) |
Volunteering | 3.48 (1.81) | 3.37 (1.71) | 3.31 (1.89) | 3.84 (1.79) | 3.50 (1.82) |
Ecological bank | 4.05 (1.75) | 4.16 (1.66) | 4.00 (1.80) | 4.10 (1.76) | 3.89 (1.80) |
No wrapped plastic | 4.67 (1.44) | 4.79 (1.23) | 4.74 (1.53) | 4.79 (1.43) | 4.26 (1.56) |
Bike use | 3.35 (1.82) | 3.31 (1.78) | 3.33 (1.88) | 3.57 (1.75) | 3.22 (1.86) |
Env. friendly mat. | 4.93 (1.56) | 4.61 (1.43) | 4.71 (1.64) | 4.18 (1.59) | 4.35 (1.58) |
Protesting | 3.68 (1.96) | 3.57 (2.00) | 3.42 (2.04) | 4.13 (1.81) | 3.72 (1.87) |
N of Classes | VLRM | LRM | AIC | BIC | Entropy |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
4 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 31,928.22 | 32,215.74 | 0.85 |
5 | 0.027 | 0.028 | 31,743.23 | 32,094.09 | 0.84 |
6 | 0.201 | 0.204 | 29,133.05 | 29,542.40 | 0.85 |
Strongly Committed | Disengaged | Strongly Disengaged | Well-Meaning | Committed to Private PEB | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | |||||
Age | + | ||||
National context (CH) | + | - | |||
FR | + | - | |||
SP | + | - | |||
UK | - | ||||
Access to outdoor | |||||
Personal reasons | |||||
PD: COVID-19 | |||||
Personal | |||||
General | |||||
Symptoms (% yes) | |||||
Personal | + | ||||
People they knew | |||||
COVID-19 worry | |||||
Human values | |||||
Universalism | + | - | - | + | |
Benevolence | |||||
Conservation | - | + | + | ||
Social desirability | |||||
General | - | ||||
Environmental | + | - | |||
Self-rated impact of COVID-19 crisis on concern for climate change | + | - | - |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sarrasin, O.; Zanetti, C.; Rudmann, O.; Avery, R.A.T.; Graton, A. ‘I Do It for Others’! Prosocial Reasons for Complying with Anti-COVID Measures and Pro-Environmental Behaviours: The Mediating Role of the Psychological Distance of Climate Change. Sustainability 2023, 15, 13194. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151713194
Sarrasin O, Zanetti C, Rudmann O, Avery RAT, Graton A. ‘I Do It for Others’! Prosocial Reasons for Complying with Anti-COVID Measures and Pro-Environmental Behaviours: The Mediating Role of the Psychological Distance of Climate Change. Sustainability. 2023; 15(17):13194. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151713194
Chicago/Turabian StyleSarrasin, Oriane, Cinzia Zanetti, Ocyna Rudmann, Robert A. T. Avery, and Aurélien Graton. 2023. "‘I Do It for Others’! Prosocial Reasons for Complying with Anti-COVID Measures and Pro-Environmental Behaviours: The Mediating Role of the Psychological Distance of Climate Change" Sustainability 15, no. 17: 13194. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151713194