Next Article in Journal
Landslide Susceptibility Mapping Based on Multitemporal Remote Sensing Image Change Detection and Multiexponential Band Math
Previous Article in Journal
The Steady Seepage Line Equation of Ionic Rare Earth Ore (IREO) In Situ Leached with Long Strip Bare Feet and Its Application
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Using a Unified Model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to Investigate the Energy-Saving Behaviour of Urban Residents in Vietnam: Moderation Role of Cultural Values

1
Centre for Analysis Forecasting and Sustainable Development, National Economics University, Hanoi 100000, Vietnam
2
Faculty of Finance, Banking Academy, Hanoi 100000, Vietnam
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2023, 15(3), 2225; https://doi.org/10.3390/su15032225
Submission received: 9 January 2023 / Revised: 21 January 2023 / Accepted: 23 January 2023 / Published: 25 January 2023

Abstract

:
This study integrated the theory of planned behaviour (TPB), the norm activation model (NAM), and the stimulus–organism–behaviour–consequences theory (SOBC) to determine how external (subjective injunctive norm, subjective descriptive norm, and perceived behavioural control) and internal stimuli (ascription of responsibility, awareness of consequences) stimulate organisms (attitude towards energy saving and personal norms), which in turn drives behavioural responses (energy-saving intentions and behaviours) and their consequences (energy-saving habits). A sample of 1514 residents of five large cities in Vietnam and a multiple linear regression analysis were used to test the hypothesised model. The results show that external stimuli positively shaped a favourable energy-saving attitude, while internal stimuli aroused individuals’ personal norms. In addition, energy-saving intention, behaviours, and habits were serial mediators impacted by both internal and external stimuli. The results also indicate that a long-term orientation positively moderated the relationship between energy-saving intention, behaviours, and habits, but collectivism only moderated the nexus between energy-saving behaviours and habits. These findings imply that policymakers should focus on conveying information related to energy conservation among surrounding people, increasing citizens’ awareness of the consequences, personal responsibilities, moral obligations regarding saving energy, and should not neglect the informative role of cultural values in energy conservation practices.

1. Introduction

Current global warming is a great threat to human civilisation, and the main cause of this phenomenon is excessive energy consumption [1]. Despite this, the demand for energy is still increasing around the world. The sources of energy consumption are diverse; however, the residential sector is one of the most significant sources [2]. According to Nejat and Jomehzadeh [3], about 27% of the total energy consumed and 17% of the global CO2 emissions are due to the residential sector. Therefore, reducing energy consumption in the residential sector can help to minimise the harmful impacts on the natural environment and the global climate.
Due to the issues mentioned above, scholars have put effort into researching pro-environmental behaviours, such as green consumption [4], energy consumption [5], and waste separation behaviours [6]. Previous pro-environmental studies have been based on various theories, but the TPB and NAM theories have been widely used [6,7]. The main argument of the TPB is that an individual’s willingness to take a specific behaviour is due to their personal motives (i.e., rational cognition), whereas the NAM posits that this willingness is a result of their morality (i.e., moral motives) [8]. The TPB posits that attitudes toward behaviour, subjective norms, and perceived behaviour control are the three antecedents of behavioural intention, and behavioural intention with perceived behaviour control are the two factors that directly form an individual’s behaviour [9]. For instance, in a pro-environmental area, when an individual has a favourable attitude towards energy-saving behaviours, they will have a higher likelihood of enacting such behaviours [10]. In other words, the NAM proposes that an individual’s behaviour is motivated by the ascription of responsibility, awareness of consequences, and personal norms [11]. More specifically, an individual’s behaviour is formed by their personal norms, which are identified by their level of awareness of consequences and the ascription of responsibility. Since the TPB model neglects the role of non-rational, non-cognitive, emotional, and altruistic motives in forming behaviour [8], one study integrated the TPB model with the NAM model to enrich the extant knowledge of pro-environmental behaviours [6]. The pro-environmental literature has illustrated that the integration of the TPB and NAM has been effective in exploring the predictors of energy-saving behaviours along with many other pro-environmental and pro-social behaviours [7,10]. However, some scholars argued that this integration is still not sufficiently convincing to explain pro-environmental behaviour and its consequences [9]. Firstly, the merged TPB–NAM model overlooks the role of environmental and contextual factors in decision-making processes [7]. Secondly, the integrated model of the TPB and NAM it neglects the consequences of actual behaviours [12,13], such as behavioural habits that are enacted with little deliberation or conscious awareness [13].
The stimulus–organism–behaviour–consequence (SOBC) framework is an important theory to understand the antecedents and subsequent various behaviours in environmental psychology [14]. This theory is seen as an advancement of the stimulus–organism–response theory [15] and the antecedent–behaviour–consequence theory [16]. The SOBC proposes that environmental stimuli (S), such as emotions, characteristics, and motives, drive individuals’ internal states or organisms (O). In turn, these organisms influence their behaviour (B) responses and then drive overt consequences (C). The SOBC theory provides an effective framework to explain the complex relationship between individuals and their environment. This theory also provides a more appropriate model for examining the relationship between behaviours and the consequences of these behaviours [14,17]. Thus, a recent study applied this theory to investigate interactions in different contexts [17]. However, to the best of our knowledge, SOBC has never been used in the energy-saving context. Therefore, in this study, we used the SOBC framework to strengthen the TPB–NAM integration and to clearly explain the rational, moral, and contextual aspects of energy behaviours.
Moreover, although the role of cultural values in other pro-environmental behaviours, such as green consumption [18,19] and the consumption of recycled products [17], has been recently investigated, to the best of our knowledge, the moderating roles of cultural values that enhance the transformation of energy-saving intention into actual behaviours and energy-saving behaviours into habits have not been considered in previous studies. In addition, scholars have emphasised that we still do not fully understand how cultural values encourage pro-environmental behaviours and have called for further efforts to develop a consistent theory of culture and pro-social behaviours [20,21,22]. In the present study, through the lens of the SOBC theory by Hofstede [19,23], two key cultural dimensions (collectivism and long-term orientation) work as contextual influences that moderate the relationships among energy-saving intention, behaviours, and habits.
In this study, the three variables in the TPB (subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norm, and perceived behavioural control) are external stimuli, whereas the two factors in the NAM (ascription of responsibility and awareness of consequences) are internal stimuli [10]. In addition, the two most influential antecedents of behavioural intention in the TPB (attitude towards behaviour) and NAM (personal norms) serve as organisms. Behavioural responses include behavioural intentions and actual behaviours, while energy-saving habits are viewed as a consequence of energy-saving behaviours. Furthermore, based on the work by Hofstede [19], two cultural dimensions of collectivism and long-term orientation act as contextual moderators that can increase the transformation of behavioural intention into actual behaviours and actual behaviours into consequences.
In sum, the current study integrated Ajzen’s (1991) TPB and Schwartz’s (1977) NAM into the stimulus–organism–behaviour–consequences (SOBC) framework with the addition of moderators (i.e., the cultural values of collectivism and long-term orientation) to clearly explain the rational, moral, and contextual aspects of energy behaviours and habits. Specifically, this study aims to understand: (1) how external stimuli (subjective injunctive norm, subjective descriptive norm, and perceived behavioural control) stimulate the attitudes toward energy saving, which in turn influences energy-saving intention, and serially affects energy-saving behaviours and energy-saving habits; (2) how internal stimuli (ascription of responsibility and awareness of consequences) arouse personal norms, which in turn increase energy-saving intention, and serially impact energy-saving behaviours and energy-saving habits; (3) how cultural values (collectivism and long-term orientation) positively moderate the relationships among energy-saving intention, behaviours, and habits; and (4) how energy-saving intention results in energy-saving behaviours and how energy-saving behaviours lead to energy-saving habits.
This study is structured as follows. First, the existing literature on energy-saving behaviours is reviewed to formulate the hypotheses. Then, the research method used in this study is presented. Subsequently, the results from the multiple linear regression analysis are reported, followed by a discussion of the findings. Finally, the theoretical and practical implications, and limitations, are presented.

2. Literature Review

2.1. External Stimulus

Environmental factors, such as objective conditions and social and political environments that an individual cannot control, are called external stimuli [10]. In the TPB model, subjective norms reflect the social pressures for carrying out a specific behaviour [19]. Subjective norms can be divided into subjective injunctive norms (SINs) and subjective descriptive norms (SDNs) [24]. SINs refer to social approval or disapproval of a certain behaviour, while SDNs refer to the prevalence of that behaviour [20]. The TPB proposes that individuals tend to comply with the expectations and behaviours of their important persons [19]. When an individual perceives a positive expectation and approval about a behaviour from significant persons, they will have a favourable attitude towards that behaviour, and therefore increase the likelihood of performing the behaviour [25].
Similarly, perceived behavioural control (PBC) in the TPB model is also an external stimulus. PBC refers to the perception of how easy or difficult it is to adopt a specific behaviour [19]. This factor relates to some external conditions, such as the availability of facilities and time, which are free from individual control. Several prior studies have demonstrated that an individual’s perceived behaviour control positively affects their attitude towards behaviour [4,26]. When a person has perceived that performing a specific behaviour is easy, they will have a positive assessment of the outcome of that behaviour, and therefore have a positive attitude towards that behaviour.
From these above arguments, this study posits that subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norms, and perceived behavioural control act as external stimuli, which positively influence attitudes toward energy-saving behaviours. With stronger subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norms, and perceived behavioural control, individuals have a more favourable attitude towards energy saving. Therefore, the following hypotheses are proposed:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Subjective injunction norms positively stimulate attitudes toward energy-saving behaviours.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Subjective descriptive norms positively stimulate attitudes toward energy-saving behaviours.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Perceived behavioural control positively stimulates attitudes toward energy-saving behaviours.

2.2. Internal Stimulus

The two main factors of the NAM model—awareness of consequences and ascription of responsibility—can be seen as internal stimuli since the foundation of this model is an activation process for an individual’s inner moral and ethical dimensions [10]. The awareness of consequences refers to an individual’s awareness of the adverse and negative influences of not implementing pro-social actions on others or things, while the ascription of responsibility reflects a feeling of responsibility for the negative consequences of not acting pro-socially [27,28,29]. Rezaei and Safa [29] argued that when individuals are aware of the negative consequences of not adopting a behaviour for others and feel a personal responsibility because of these adverse consequences, they may feel a personal moral obligation to engage in a certain behaviour, which is known as personal norms [21]. In the energy-saving context, the higher one’s awareness of negative consequences and the higher one’s personal responsibility related to non-energy-saving behaviours is, the more likely that person will feel a higher moral obligation to engage in energy-saving behaviours. Therefore, the following hypotheses are proposed:
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Awareness of consequences positively affects personal norms.
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
Ascription of responsibility positively affects personal norms.

2.3. Organisms and Behavioural Responses

In this study, attitudes toward energy-saving behaviours and personal norms were used as the organisms in the SOBC paradigm. It is argued that internal and external stimuli may inspire these organisms, which can influence the behaviour responses, including energy-saving intentions and behaviours.
Attitude towards behaviour is seen as the best predictor of behaviour intention in the TPB model [30]. Individuals will have higher intentions to perform a specific behaviour when their attitude towards that behaviour is favourable [20]. Prior studies have shown that attitudes toward energy saving positively affects individuals’ pro-environmental intentions, such as the intention for energy-saving behaviours at the workplace, organic food consumption, or mask-saving [7,31]. Likewise, in the NAM model, personal norms are the crucial construct that drive an individual’s adoption of pro-social behaviours. Based on the integrated TPB–NAM model, scholars illustrated that personal norms play an important role in forming energy-saving intentions [10].
From these above arguments, the following hypotheses are proposed:
Hypothesis 6 (H6).
Attitude towards behaviour positively affects energy-saving intentions.
Hypothesis 7 (H7).
Personal norms positively affect energy-saving intentions.
Although there is a gap between intention and behaviour, it is widely recognised that intention is a critical predictor of behaviour in general [30] and pro-environmental behaviours in particular [31,32]. Behavioural intention is seen as a strong internal stimulus and is often recognised as a cause of behaviour [33,34]. The theory of planned behaviour demonstrates that intention plays an important role in predicting behaviour in various areas. Environmental behaviour intention is defined as individuals’ perception engaging in pro-environmental behaviours, reflecting their propensity to take part in a particular environmental behaviour [35,36]. The prediction role of intention in pro-environmental behaviours has been proven in previous studies [37,38,39]. In an energy-saving context, the results of the relationship between energy-saving intention and behaviour are still inconsistent. While one study illustrated that energy-saving intention had a positive impact on energy-saving behaviour [33], another study showed that there was an insignificant relationship between energy-saving intention and behaviour [34]. These inconsistent results imply that further studies on this relationship are needed. Thus, the current study argues that energy-saving intention has a positive effect on energy-saving behaviour. The following hypothesis is proposed:
Hypothesis 8 (H8).
Energy-saving intention positively affects energy-saving behaviour.

2.4. Behavioural Response and Consequence

The literature on psychology has increasingly suggested that the consequence of a repeated behaviour is a habit [35]. A habit, which is a routine action that is repeatedly automatic and tends to occur subconsciously, is easily shaped by past behaviours [36]. Contemporary theory considers a habit as a behavioural tendency that tends to be enacted with little deliberate awareness, and as a consequence, it occurs quickly, efficiently, and automatically, with little effort [12,37]. Haith and Krakauer [40] also argue that habits are conceptually similar to skills, in that sequences of behaviours become automatic and natural. The roles of various antecedents, such as policy environment [22], negative and positive perceptions [41], or group values, norms, and interactions [42], in developing pro-environmental behaviours have been explored by scholars. However, little is known about how energy-saving behaviours develop energy-saving habits or the underlying mechanisms that facilitate or restrain the translation of energy-saving behaviours into habits. In other words, little attention has been paid to the sequence of energy-saving behaviours, but this information is extremely necessary not only for environmentalists but also for policymakers to enable them to protect the environment and maintain our planet. Based on the structured lens of the SOBC theory, we conceptualise energy-saving habits as a consequence of energy-saving behaviours to test how and under which conditions urban residents’ energy-saving behaviours transform into energy-saving habits. The following hypothesis is proposed:
Hypothesis 9 (H9).
Energy-saving habits are positively associated with energy-saving behaviours.

2.5. Moderation Effects

Culture is known as “the collective programming of the mind that distinguishes the member of one group or category of people from others” [40]. Six key dimensions of culture include collectivism/individualism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity/femininity, long-term orientation, and indulgence/restraint [41]. These dimensions shape individuals’ beliefs, norms, and shared values, which in turn trigger particular behaviours [32,42,43]. Despite receiving some criticisms due to the assumption that the inhabitants of a particular country are representatives of a single national culture, scholars argued that Hofstede’s model still provides a useful conceptual foundation and roadmap for understanding human behaviour [43].
In recent studies, Hofstede’s cultural dimensions have been increasingly used to explore various pro-social behaviours, such as general environmental behaviour [44] and green consumption [45]. However, the employment of these dimensions in an energy-saving context is still limited. To the best of our knowledge, no prior studies have used these cultural dimensions as moderators that can strengthen the energy-saving intention–behaviour–habit path. Therefore, based on the integrated SOBC model, this study utilised the two most widely used cultural dimensions (collectivism and long-term orientation) [46,47] to explain pro-environmental behaviours. Specifically, this study tested whether cultural values could moderate the relationships among energy-saving intentions, behaviours, and habits.
Hofstede [42,43] defined collectivism as the degree to which individuals in a society are integrated into groups. Collectivism can also be thought of as the degree to which people feel proud, loyal, and engaged in their group [15,48,49]. In a collectivist society, individuals are often members of well-built and cohesive internal groups, such as extended families [50]. Out of unconditional loyalty in such a culture, individuals in the group are willing to assist and look after each of their members [51,52,53]. They are more likely to share their resources with other members of the same group and often show favourable attitudes towards behaviours that benefit their group and community [54]. Scholars argued that collectivism is a critical factor in sustainability because collectivist societies focus on the needs of the collective and pay attention to the effects of their actions on society and the community [15,55]. In addition, because of their values of cooperation and cohesion, collectivists show a strong connection to their social and environmental problems, and they aspire to live in harmony with the natural environment [56,57,58,59]. Therefore, under the moderation effect of collectivism, urban energy-saving intentions are more likely to be translated into actual behaviours. Moreover, the process that leads a behaviour to develop into a habit is likely to be facilitated or constrained by environmental and contextual cues [12,60], such as collectivism.
From these above arguments, the following hypothesis is proposed:
Hypothesis 10 (H10).
Collectivism positively moderates the relationships among energy-saving intention, energy-saving behaviours, and energy-saving habits. As such, these positive relationships will become stronger when the level of collectivism is high.
Long-term orientation reflects the temporal orientation of a society, showing the importance of the future in terms of values, beliefs, and attitudes towards that society [61,62]. It reflects how a society encourages and rewards future orientation behaviours, such as planning or delaying gratification, or even how a society makes sacrifices today for a better tomorrow [63]. Individuals with a long-term orientation are not inspired by temporary results but by future rewards [64]. In a long-term-oriented culture, individuals will be willing to take actions that they find inconvenient, such as turning off the lights and air-conditioner, for a better future for themselves and their community [65]. Furthermore, in the field of sustainability, previous studies have demonstrated that long-term orientation is related to attitudes toward pro-social behaviours [66], pro-environmental intentions and behaviours [67], and even promoting the translation from pro-social attitudes and intentions into actual pro-social behaviours [68].
From these above arguments, the following hypothesis is proposed:
Hypothesis 11 (H11).
Long-term orientation moderates the relationships among energy-saving intention, energy-saving behaviours, and energy-saving habits. As such, these positive relationships will become stronger when the level of long-term orientation is high. You can see the hypothesis model in Figure 1.

3. Methods

3.1. Research Context and Research Process

Currently, the amount of consumed energy in Vietnam is growing higher and higher to meet the demands of economic development and rapid population growth. The residential sector uses more energy than commercial and public services. Residential energy consumption grew rapidly by nearly 140% in 10 years from 2008 to 2018 and accounted for a third of the total energy consumption [69]. However, Vietnam is struggling with an energy shortage problem due to the lack of appropriate related policies and corresponding developed technology. The current energy demand has exceeded the domestic supply. Thus, the Vietnam government identified economical and efficient energy consumption as an important way to protect the environment and develop the national economy sustainably [70]. Based on these facts, examining the energy-saving behaviours of Vietnamese residents is meaningful.
The research process is shown in Figure 2 and is carried out through three stages: (1) building the research model and scales, (2) building the questionnaire and collecting data, and (3) analysing the data and discussion of the results.

3.2. Data Collection

This study surveyed adult residents (over 18 years old) in five cities with the largest populations in Vietnam (Hai Phong, Ha Noi, Da Nang, Ho Chi Minh City, and Can Tho). The data collection was carried out for 3 months (from May to July 2022). The process of data collection included several principles. Firstly, to decrease the selection bias, one out of every five persons who crossed central areas of the city, such as supermarkets, coffee houses, or office buildings, were asked to participate in the survey. Secondly, every participant was clearly informed that this survey only served research purposes. Thirdly, before answering the questionnaire, all the concepts included in the survey were fully explained to every participant. Finally, 1694 questionnaires were completed, but 180 responses were eliminated due to missing data. Therefore, our final sample included 1514 responses. The demographic profiles of the respondents are presented in Table 1.

3.3. Measure

This study adopted all scales that have been used in prior studies and modified them to be suitable for the research context. Some reversed items were added to decrease the response bias. The measurement items of energy-saving habits were adapted from Zhang and Wang [54,55], and Wang and Lin [55]. Items measuring energy-saving behaviours were adapted from the study of Zhang and Yu [54,55], including a reversed item, “I always turn on the electricity appliances when I do not use them or when I leave home”. The energy-saving intention measurement was adopted from Zhu and Alam [47], including a reversed question, “I do not have the intention to use energy-saving appliances”. Regarding the constructs in the TPB model, the measurement items of attitude towards energy saving, subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norms, and perceived behavioural control were derived from the research of Ru and Wang [28]. Finally, the measurement items of the three core constructs in the NAM model—awareness of consequences, attribution of responsibility, and personal norms—were adopted from Wang and Wang [1]. All items on the scales were scored from 1 to 5, corresponding with “strongly disagree or never” to “strongly agree or always”, respectively.

3.4. Data Analysis

This study validates the hypothetical model through a two-step process. First, the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was employed to test the measurement model. The structural validity which consists of convergence validity and discrimination validity was also examined in this step. After that, the multiple linear regression analysis was utilized to test the hypotheses. This analysis technique is appropriate for hypotheses with interaction impacts [56,63,64,65]. In addition, Model 4 and Model 6 of the PROCESS macro approach, with 5000 bias-corrected bootstrapping samples [57,58,66,67], were used to examine mediate and moderate effects, respectively. To ensure the reliability of the analysis results, several demographic variables, such as gender, age, educational level, and monthly income, were also controlled as covariates.

4. Results and Discussions

4.1. Reliability and Validity

Before testing the research hypotheses, CFA was performed using AMOS (Analysis of Moment Structures) software version 25.0 to examine the reliability and validity of the research constructs. Table 2 demonstrates the final results of the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).
First, the Cronbach’s Alpha results show that ESB6 and ESI4 (reversed items) had corrected item–total correlation values lower than the threshold value of 0.3 (0.080 and 0.095, respectively), and thus, these items were removed from the scales [52,68,69]. Next, CFA was performed to examine the reliability and validity of the scales. The initial results of the CFA illustrate acceptable fit indices: χ2 (695) = 3194.110; χ2/df = 4.596; p < 0.001; GFI = 0.886; CFI = 0.930; TLI = 0.921; and RMSEA = 0.049. However, item ESB3 (“When I am in a public place, I don’t like to use air conditioners”) had a standardised regression weight value of only 0.320; therefore, this item was eliminated from the ESB scale [53]. The CFA was then performed again. The final CFA results show the excellent fit indices of the measurement model: χ2 (1105) = 3763.505; χ2/df = 3.406 < 5; p < 0.001; GFI = 0.901 > 0.9; CFI = 0.943 > 0.9; TLI = 0.937 > 0.9; RMSEA = 0.040 < 0.05 [53]. Additionally, the standardised regression weights of all items were higher than 0.5 (λ > 0.5), and thus, all items were significant. Furthermore, both the reliability and discriminant validity of the constructs were confirmed since all constructs had AVE values higher than 0.5, and their CRs were higher than 0.7 (see Table 2). A heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) analysis was also used as an additional test to confirm the discriminant validity of the constructs. The analysis results show that the discriminant validity of the constructs was verified because all HTMT values were less than the threshold value of 0.9 [54].

4.2. Results of Hypotheses Testing

The results of the multiple linear regression analysis of the coefficients are illustrated in Table 3 and Table 4. This analysis technique is appropriate for hypotheses with interaction impacts [55]. To ensure the reliability of the analysis results, several demographic variables, such as gender, age, educational level, and monthly income, were controlled as covariates. Table 4 illustrates the analysis results for the direct and interaction effects, while Table 5 presents the mediation analysis results using Model 4 and Model 6 of the PROCESS macro approach, with 5000 bias-corrected bootstrapping samples [56]. The empirical results also presented in Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 in the follows.
The results of the analysis show that all the external stimuli (subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norms, and perceived behavioural control) were positively and significantly correlated with attitude towards energy saving (β = 0.099, 0.250, and 0.435, respectively, all with p < 0.001), while all the internal stimuli (ascription of responsibility and awareness of consequence) were positively and significantly correlated with personal norms (β = 0.321 and 0.324, respectively, all with p < 0.001). H1, H2, H3, H4, and H5 are thus supported. This finding is suitable with the high collectivist society context in Vietnam where individuals’ attitudes are strongly effected by others [44]. Additionally, it illustrates that Vietnamese urban residents are aware that not taking energy-saving behaviour may lead to bad consequences for others and they feel that they are responsible for these bad results.
In addition, the results show that two organisms (attitude towards energy-saving behaviour and personal norms) positively impacted energy-saving intention (β = 0.452 and 0.329, respectively, all with p < 0.001). Energy-saving intention also positively influenced energy-saving behaviours (β = 0.308; p < 0.001), and energy-saving behaviours positively shaped energy-saving habits (β = 0.153; p < 0.001). Thus, H6, H7, H8, and H9 are supported by the data.
More importantly, the analysis results illustrate that long-term orientation positively moderated the relationships among energy-saving intentions, behaviours, and habits (β = 0.294 and 0.265, respectively, all with p < 0.001), while collectivism only moderated the effect of energy-saving behaviours and energy-saving habits (β = 0.139, p-value < 0.01). Therefore, H10 is partially supported, while H11 is fully supported. Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4 present the plots of significant interactions. The slopes of the interaction plot in Figure 2 indicate that variations in energy-saving intentions are more strongly correlated with energy-saving behaviours under the conditions of a high degree of long-term orientation. Similarly, Figure 3 and Figure 4 suggest that variations in energy-saving behaviours are more strongly correlated with energy-saving habits under the conditions of a higher degree of collectivism and long-term orientation. In other words, when residents perceived a higher degree of long-term orientation or higher level of collectivism, the likelihood of transforming their energy-saving intentions into actual behaviours and their energy-saving behaviours into habits became stronger. Although this study is the first to examine the moderation role of cultural values on the relationships among energy-saving intentions, behaviours, and habits, this finding is consistent with some studies on other pro-social behaviours, such as environmental commitment or organic food consumption [32,46,57]. It also sheds light on the fact that in a collectivist society, such as Vietnam or China, where residents respect traditional values, pride, faithfulness, and a willingness to sacrifice temporary benefits for a better tomorrow, they tend to translate their pro-environmental intentions into actual behaviours, and pro-environmental behaviours into habits, including energy-saving practices, to protect the environment and develop sustainably [32,42,44,46].
Interestingly, our mediation analyses (see Table 5) show that all the external and internal stimuli had serial multiple mediation effects on energy-saving behaviours and energy-saving habits as all the mediation regression coefficients were statistically significant (all bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals do not contain zero). These findings suggest that the integration of the external and internal psychological constructs of a “rational” model (TPB) with an “emotional” model (NAM) in the SOBC paradigm are more than sufficient to explain individuals’ energy-saving practices. In this unified framework, the three psychological constructs of the TPB and the two psychological constructs of the NAM serve as stimuli, directly arousing organisms (attitudes toward energy saving and personal norms, respectively). This in turn affects behavioural responses (energy-saving intention and behaviours), and then develops the consequence of behaviours (energy-saving habits). In line with the integrated TPB–NAM model, previous studies showed that the approval and expectations perceived by surrounding others (i.e., family members and friends), perception of the ease of saving energy, and perception of moral obligation are directly related to energy-saving intentions [20] and/or energy-saving behaviours [22,38]. However, no prior studies have considered these constructs as stimuli that can drive the energy-saving behaviours and habits underlining the complex mediation mechanism of the SOBC model.

5. Conclusions

Energy-saving behaviour is seen as an effective way to address climate change issues [22,60,61]. Therefore, researching energy-saving behaviour or habits is attracting more attention. Energy-saving behaviour is relatively complex, depending on both individual and contextual factors [62]. However, scholars have emphasised that we still do not fully understand how cultural values encourage pro-environmental behaviours in general and energy-saving behaviour in particular, and have called for further efforts to develop a consistent theory of culture and pro-social behaviours [15,63]. In addition, previous studies mostly employed the TPB and NAM models to explain energy-saving behaviours, which overlooked the role of environmental and contextual factors on decision-making processes [7] and neglected the consequences of actual behaviours [14]. Therefore, this study used the SOBC framework to strengthen the integration of the TPB–NAM and to clearly explain the rational, moral, and contextual aspects of energy behaviours.
The findings of the current study make several significant contributions to both theoretical and empirical aspects. Regarding theoretical contributions, this study has five. Firstly, this study is the first to integrate the TPB and NAM models into the SOBC framework to explain energy-saving behaviours. Secondly, this study is the first to conceptualise the three antecedents of the TPB (subjective injunctive norms, subjective descriptive norms, and perceived behavioural control) and the two antecedents of the NAM (ascription of responsibility and awareness of consequences) as stimuli in the SOBC model. The findings confirm that these factors are effective stimuli which directly, positively, and significantly influence organisms (i.e., attitude and personal norms), and then indirectly drive behavioural responses (i.e., energy-saving intention and behaviour) and their consequences (i.e., energy-saving habits). Thirdly, besides examining the direct effect on the SOBC framework, this study also provides empirical evidence for the serial mediation mechanism of organisms and behavioural responses in the relationship between stimuli and consequences in an energy-saving context. Fourthly, the present study is the first to consider energy-saving habits as a consequence of energy-saving behaviours and examine this relationship. Finally, the moderation effects of cultural dimensions (i.e., collectivism and long-term orientation) on the energy-saving intention–behaviour relationship, as well as the relationship between energy-saving behaviours and habits, were first tested by this study.
Regarding the political aspect, the findings of this study provide some interventions to encourage residents’ energy-saving behaviours and energy-saving habits to achieve the Sustainable Development Goals. Firstly, the results of this study illustrate that both subjective injunctive and descriptive norms are important factors that not only shape a favourable attitude towards energy-saving but also indirectly encourage energy-saving intention, behaviours, and habits. Therefore, encouraging energy-saving practices by conveying information related to energy conservation among surrounding people can be an effective policy. This policy can be implemented through educational programs or mass media campaigns that are easily and freely accessible to everyone. Through these programmes, people can build a positive awareness of energy conservation and pro-social behaviours. Secondly, the results of this study also show that perceived behaviour control plays a crucial role in inspiring favourable energy-saving attitudes, intention, behaviours, and habits; therefore, policymakers should focus on ways to raise residents’ perceptions of the ease and convenience of energy-saving behaviours; for example, informing residents of some simple ways to save energy, such as turning off the lights or unplugging chargers when not in use. Thirdly, it is necessary to increase awareness of the consequences, personal responsibilities, and moral obligations regarding saving energy. Information about the environment and the global warming issue should be transmitted to everyone through conventional media channels, such as magazines, television, radio, and newspapers, along with the internet. Finally, policymakers should not neglect the informative role of cultural values in energy conservation practices since these values significantly increase the transformation from energy-saving intention into behaviours, and from behaviours into habits. Individuals should be encouraged to respect community values in their collective society and to contribute to the sustainable development of the community through educational programmes and communications in groups. The benefits to the community and society when implementing energy-saving behaviours could be transmitted via educational and communicative messages.
This study is not exempt from limitations. Firstly, even though this study is the first to test the moderate effect of cultural values on the relationships among energy-saving intentions, behaviours, and habits, data were only collected from residents in Vietnam. According to Chwialkowska and Bhatti [15], the nexus between sustainability and culture should be implemented in a cross-country context. Therefore, further studies should be implemented in different cultural contexts to clarify the role of cultural values in pro-social behaviours. Secondly, as a result of intercultural communication, culture is always changing over time, and therefore the influence of culture on individuals’ behaviours is also longitudinally shifted. Thus, further studies should employ a longitudinal method to expand our knowledge about the relationships among intention, behaviours, and habits, in addition to the effects of culture on sustainable settings, including energy-saving behaviours.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.M.H., V.N.X., L.T.L. and N.T.D.; data curation, N.T.D. and V.N.X.; formal analysis, N.T.D. and V.N.X.; funding acquisition, N.M.H., V.N.X. and L.T.L.; investigation, N.T.D., V.N.X. and L.T.L.; methodology, N.T.D., V.N.X. and L.T.L.; project administration, V.N.X. and N.T.D.; resources, V.N.X. and N.T.D.; supervision, V.N.X.; validation, N.T.D.; visualization, N.T.D.; writing—original draft, V.N.X.; writing—review and editing, V.N.X. and N.T.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study is funded by the National Economics University, Hanoi, Vietnam (number: 21/HD-CBQT1.2022.21).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Research Ethics Committee of Department of Research Management, National Economics University, Hanoi, Vietnam after reviewing the research proposal, titled “Using a Unified Model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to Investigate the Energy-Saving Behaviour of Urban Residents in Vietnam: Moderation Role of Cultural Values”. Proposal/ID: CBQT1.2022.21.

Informed Consent Statement

The current study was carried out based on the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical clearance for this research has been reviewed and approved by Department of Research Management, National Economics University, Hanoi, Vietnam. No: CBQT1.2022.21. Moreover, informed consent has been received from all participants who agreed to take part in the survey. Specifically, we informed that their participants are totally voluntary and they can withdraw from the survey at any time they want. Also, the research only serve for academic purposes and non-commerce.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wang, J.; Li, Y.; He, Z.; Gao, J.; Wang, J. Scale framing, benefit framing and their interaction effects on energy-saving behaviors: Evidence from urban residents of China. Energy Policy 2022, 166, 113005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Tran, L.N.; Xuan, J.; Nakagami, H.; Kuroki, S.; Ge, J.; Gao, W. Influence of household factors on energy use in Vietnam based on path analysis. J. Build. Eng. 2022, 57, 104834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Nejat, P.; Jomehzadeh, F.; Taheri, M.M.; Gohari, M.; Abd Majid, M.Z. A global review of energy consumption, CO2 emissions and policy in the residential sector (with an overview of the top ten CO2 emitting countries). Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2015, 43, 843–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Duong, C.D.; Doan, X.H.; Vu, D.M.; Ha, N.T.; Dam, K.V. The role of perceived environmental responsibility and environmental concern on shaping green purchase intention. Vis. J. Bus. Perspect. 2022, 11, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Tang, C.F.; Tan, B.W.; Ozturk, I. Energy consumption and economic growth in Vietnam. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2016, 54, 1506–1514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Goh, E.; Esfandiar, K.; Jie, F.; Brown, K.; Djajadikerta, H. Please sort out your rubbish! An integrated structural model approach to examine antecedents of residential households’ waste separation behaviour. J. Clean. Prod. 2022, 355, 131789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Si, H.; Shen, L.; Liu, W.; Wu, G. Uncovering people’s mask-saving intentions and behaviors in the post-COVID-19 period: Evidence from China. Sustain. Cities Soc. 2021, 65, 102626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wang, B.; Wang, X.; Guo, D.; Zhang, B.; Wang, Z. Analysis of factors influencing residents’ habitual energy-saving behaviour based on NAM and TPB models: Egoism or altruism? Energy Policy 2018, 116, 68–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hofstede, G. Organizations and Cultures: Software of the Mind; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  10. Schwartz, S.H. Normative Influences on Altruism. Adv. Exp. Soc. Econ. 1977, 10, 221–279. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hong, Y.; Furnell, S. Understanding cybersecurity behavioral habits: Insights from situational support. J. Inf. Secur. Appl. 2021, 57, 102710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gkargkavouzi, A.; Halkos, G.; Matsiori, S. Environmental behavior in a private-sphere context: Integrating theories of planned behavior and value belief norm, self-identity and habit. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2019, 148, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 1991, 50, 197–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hagger, M.S. Habit and physical activity: Theoretical advances, practical implications, and agenda for future research. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019, 42, 118–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Mehrabian, A.; Russell, J. An Approach to Environmental Psychology; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
  16. Skinner, B.F. Operant behavior. Am. Psychol. 1963, 18, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chakraborty, D.; Singu, H.B.; Patre, S. Fitness Apps’s purchase behaviour: Amalgamation of Stimulus-Organism-Behaviour-Consequence framework (S–O–B–C) and the innovation resistance theory (IRT). J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2022, 67, 103033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Halder, P.; Hansen, E.N.; Kangas, J.; Laukkanen, T. How national culture and ethics matter in consumers’ green consumption values. J. Clean. Prod. 2020, 265, 121754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hofstede, G. Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context. Online Read. Psychol. Cult. 2011, 2, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Talwar, S.; Jabeen, F.; Tandon, A.; Sakashita, M.; Dhir, A. What drives willingness to purchase and stated buying behavior toward organic food? A Stimulus–Organism–Behavior–Consequence (SOBC) perspective. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 293, 125882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Chwialkowska, A.; Bhatti, W.A.; Glowik, M. The influence of cultural values on pro-environmental behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 2020, 268, 122305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Asgari Toorzani, A.; Rassafi, A.A. The effect of cultural values on pro-environmental attitude in the context of travel mode choice: A hierarchical approach. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2022, 88, 291–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Saracevic, S.; Schlegelmilch, B.B.; Wu, T. How normative appeals influence pro-environmental behavior: The role of individualism and collectivism. J. Clean. Prod. 2022, 344, 131086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Predicting and Changing Behavior: The Reasoned Action Approach; Taylor and Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  25. Nketiah, E.; Song, H.; Cai, X.; Adjei, M.; Adu-Gyamfi, G.; Obuobi, B. Citizens’ intention to invest in municipal solid waste to energy projects in Ghana: The impact of direct and indirect effects. Energy 2022, 254, 124420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Shankar, A.; Jebarajakirthy, C.; Nayal, P.; Maseeh, H.I.; Kumar, A.; Sivapalan, A. Online food delivery: A systematic synthesis of literature and a framework development. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2022, 104, 103240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. De Groot, J.I.M.; Steg, L. Morality and prosocial behavior: The Role of awareness, responsibility, and norms in the norm activation model. J. Soc. Pyschology 2009, 149, 425–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ru, X.; Wang, S.; Yan, S. Exploring the effects of normative factors and perceived behavioral control on individual’s energy-saving intention: An empirical study in eastern China. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2018, 134, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Rezaei, R.; Safa, L.; Damalas, C.A.; Ganjkhanloo, M.M. Drivers of farmers’ intention to use integrated pest management: Integrating theory of planned behavior and norm activation model. J. Environ. Manag. 2019, 236, 328–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Yu, Z.; Ju, X.; Bai, L.; Gong, S. Consumer’s over-ordering behavior at restaurant: Understanding the important roles of interventions from waiter and ordering habits. Appetite 2021, 160, 105092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Chakraborty, D.; Siddiqui, A.; Siddiqui, M.; Mohmmad, H.; Alatawi, F. Exploring consumer purchase intentions and behavior of buying ayurveda products using SOBC framework. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2022, 65, 102889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior: Frequently asked questions. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2020, 2, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sultan, P.; Wong, H.Y.; Azam, M.S. How perceived communication source and food value stimulate purchase intention of organic food: An examination of the stimulus-organism-response (SOR) model. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 312, 127807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Moisander, J. Motivational complexity of green consumerism. Inter. J. Cons. Stu. 2007, 31, 404–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Leonidou, L.C.; Eteokleous, P.P.; Christofi, A.-M.; Korfiatis, N. Drivers, outcomes, and moderators of consumer intention to buy organic goods: Meta-analysis, implications, and future agenda. J. Bus. Res. 2022, 151, 339–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Liu, P.; Teng, M.; Han, C. How does environmental knowledge translate into pro-environmental behaviors?: The mediating role of environmental attitudes and behavioral intentions. Sci. Total. Environ. 2020, 728, 138126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Liu, X.; Wang, Q.-C.; Jian, I.Y.; Chi, H.-L.; Yang, D.; Chan, E.H.-W. Are you an energy saver at home? The personality insights of household energy conservation behaviors based on theory of planned behavior. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2021, 174, 105823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lee, T.H.; Jan, F.-H.; Huang, G.W. The influence of recreation experiences on environmentally responsible behavior: The case of Liuqiu Island, Taiwan. J. Sustain. Tour. 2015, 23, 947–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mobley, C.; Vagias, W.M.; DeWard, S.L. Exploring Additional Determinants of Environmentally Responsible Behavior: The Influence of Environmental Literature and Environmental Attitudes. Environ. Behav. 2009, 42, 420–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Haith, A.M.; Krakauer, J.W. The multiple effects of practice: Skill, habit and reduced cognitive load. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 20, 196–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Vilčinskas, V. Values, Attitudes and Energy Saving Behaviour in Lithuania. Filosofija. Sociol. 2021, 32, 346–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hassin, R.R.; Bargh, J.A.; Zimerman, S. Automatic and flexible: The case of nonconscious goal pursuit. Soc. Cogn. 2009, 27, 20–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hofstede, G. Culture and Organizations: Software of the Mind: Intercultural Cooperation and Its Importance for Survival, 3rd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  44. Hofstede, G. Culture’s Consequences. Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organizations across Nations, 2nd ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gardner, B. Habit and behavioural complexity: Habitual instigation and execution as predictors of simple and complex behaviours. Curr. Res. Behav. Sci. 2022, 3, 100081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Fu, W.; Zhou, Y.; Li, L.; Yang, R. Understanding household electricity-saving behavior: Exploring the effects of perception and cognition factors. Sustain. Prod. Consum. 2021, 28, 116–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Zhu, J.; Alam, M.M.; Ding, Z.; Ekambaram, P.; Li, J.; Wang, J. The influence of group-level factors on individual energy-saving behaviors in a shared space: The case of shared residences. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 311, 127560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. He, L.; Filimonau, V. The effect of national culture on pro-environmental behavioural intentions of tourists in the UK and China. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 2020, 35, 100716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Irawan; Elia, A.; Benius. Interactive effects of citizen trust and cultural values on pro-environmental behaviors: A time-lag study from Indonesia. Heliyon 2022, 8, e09139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Duong, C.D. Using a unified model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to understand students’ energy-saving behaviors: Moderation role of group-level factors and media publicity. Int. J. Energy Sect. Manag. 2023, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Cong Doanh, D.; Gadomska-Lila, K.; Thi Loan, L. Antecedents of green purchase intention: A cross-cultural empirical evidence from Vietnam and Poland. Oeconomia Copernic. 2021, 12, 935–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Sreen, N.; Purbey, S.; Sadarangani, P. Impact of culture, behavior and gender on green purchase intention. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2018, 41, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ianole-Călin, R.; Francioni, B.; Masili, G.; Druică, E.; Goschin, Z. A cross-cultural analysis of how individualism and collectivism impact collaborative consumption. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2020, 157, 104762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Zhang, Y.; Wang, Z.; Zhou, G. Determinants and implications of employee electricity saving habit: An empirical study in China. Appl. Energy 2013, 112, 1529–1535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Wang, S.; Lin, S.; Li, J. Exploring the effects of non-cognitive and emotional factors on household electricity saving behavior. Energy Policy 2018, 115, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Zhang, C.-Y.; Yu, B.; Wang, J.-W.; Wei, Y.-M. Impact factors of household energy-saving behavior: An empirical study of Shandong Province in China. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 185, 285–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Streiner, D.L.; Norman, G.R.; Cairney, J. Health Measurement Scales: A Practical Guide to Their Development and Use; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hair, J.F.; Howard, M.C.; Nitzl, C. Assessing measurement model quality in PLS-SEM using confirmatory composite analysis. J. Bus. Res. 2020, 109, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.R.; Sarstedt, M.; Danks, N.P.; Ray, S. Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) Using R.; Springer Nature Switzerland AG: Cham, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Nguyen, H.V.; Le, M.T.T.; Do, L.T. Intrinsic motivation for reducing single-use plastics: The compensation effects of basic psychological needs. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2022, 185, 106482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hayes, A.F. An Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 3rd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  62. Cho, Y.-N.; Thyroff, A.; Rapert, M.I.; Park, S.-Y.; Lee, H.J. To be or not to be green: Exploring individualism and collectivism as antecedents of environmental behavior. J. Bus. Res. 2013, 66, 1052–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Thi Tuyet Mai, N. An investigation into the relationship between materialism and green purchase behavior in Vietnam and Taiwan. J. Econ. Dev. 2021, 21, 247–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Nguyen, M.H.; Le, C.V.; Atkinson, S.E. The production ineffeciency of US electricity industry in the face of restructuring and emission reduction. J. Econ. Dev. 2022, 24, 278–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Abaidoo, R.; Agyapong, E.K. Financial development and institutional quality among emerging economies. J. Econ. Dev. 2022, 24, 198–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Duong, C.D.; Wach, K.; Vu, N.X.; Ha, S.T.; Nguyen, B.N. Entrepreneurial education, government policies and programmes, and entrepreneurial behavior: A serial moderated mediation model. Entrep. Bus. Econ. Rev. 2022, 10, 37–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Joo, B.A.; Shawl, S.; Makina, D. The interaction between FDI, host country characteristics and economic growth? A new panel evidence from BRICS. J. Econ. Dev. 2022, 24, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Ssekibaala, D.S.; Ariffin, M.I.; Duasa, J. Economic growth, international trade, and environmental degradation in Sub-Saharan Africa. J. Econ. Dev. 2022, 24, 293–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ghosh, S. COVID-19, clean energy stock market, interest rate, oil prices, volatility index, geopolotical risk nexus: Evidence from quantile regression. J. Econ. Dev. 2022, 24, 329–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Duong, C.D. Cultural values and energy-saving attitude-intention-behavior linkages among urban residents: A serial multiple mediation analysis based on stimulus-organism-response model. Manag. Environ. Qual. 2023, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Hypothesised model.
Figure 1. Hypothesised model.
Sustainability 15 02225 g001
Figure 2. Research process.
Figure 2. Research process.
Sustainability 15 02225 g002
Figure 3. Interaction effect of long-term orientation and energy-saving intention on energy-saving behaviours.
Figure 3. Interaction effect of long-term orientation and energy-saving intention on energy-saving behaviours.
Sustainability 15 02225 g003
Figure 4. Interaction effect of collectivism and energy-saving behaviours on energy-saving habits.
Figure 4. Interaction effect of collectivism and energy-saving behaviours on energy-saving habits.
Sustainability 15 02225 g004
Figure 5. Interaction effect of long-term orientation and energy-saving behaviours on energy-saving habits.
Figure 5. Interaction effect of long-term orientation and energy-saving behaviours on energy-saving habits.
Sustainability 15 02225 g005
Table 1. Demographic profiles of respondents.
Table 1. Demographic profiles of respondents.
MeasureValueFrequencyPercent
GenderMale 72347.8
Female 79152.2
AgeFrom 18 to 19 years old 37424.7
From 20 to 29 years old 52134.4
From 30 to 39 years old37424.7
From 40 to 49 years old15510.2
From 50 to 59 years old644.2
Over 60 years old261.7
Educational levelHigh school and below52634.8
University (Undergraduate)90259.6
University (Postgraduate or above)865.6
Income (in months)Less than VND 15,000,000 64242.4
From VND 16,000,000 to 25,000,000 56737.5
From VND 26,000,000 to 35,000,000 18112.0
From VND 36,000,000 to 45,000,000 936.1
Over VND 45,000,000 312.0
Cities Hanoi52534.7
Hai Phong1479.7
Da Nang875.7
Can Tho1399.2
Ho Chi Minh city 61640.7
Note: N = 1514.
Table 2. Results of Cronbach’s Alpha and confirmatory factor analysis.
Table 2. Results of Cronbach’s Alpha and confirmatory factor analysis.
IndicatorCronbach’s AlphaFactor LoadingsAVECR
Energy-saving habit (ESH)
ESH10.8950.8340.7420.896
ESH20.893
ESH30.856
Energy-saving behaviour (ESB)
ESB10.8350.7810.5600.836
ESB20.767
ESB40.781
ESB50.733
Energy-saving intention (ESI)
ESI10.8290.7580.5080.835
ESI20.778
ESI30.799
ESI50.533
ESI60.661
Attitude towards energy saving (AES)
AES10.8750.8210.6380.876
AES20.790
AES30.787
AES40.797
Subjective injunctive norm (SIN)
SIN10.8450.8040.6460.845
SIN20.812
SIN30.794
Subjective description norm (SDN)
SDN10.8030.7490.5770.804
SDN20.768
SDN30.762
Perceived behavioural control (PBC)
PBC10.8230.7850.5460.827
PBC20.752
PBC30.630
PBC40.777
Personal norms (PN)
PN10.8340.7410.5010.833
PN20.696
PN30.645
PN40.682
PN50.768
Awareness of consequences (AOC)
AOC10.8750.8350.6380.876
AOC20.768
AOC30.782
AOC40.808
Attribution of responsibility (AOR)
AOR10.8740.8140.6340.874
AOR20.792
AOR30.777
AOR40.801
Collectivism (COL)
COL10.8920.8240.6270.893
COL20.726
COL30.843
COL40.834
COL50.714
Long-term orientation (LTO)
LTO10.9060.8080.6160.906
LTO20.763
LTO30.767
LTO40.791
LTO50.803
LTO60.778
Note: N = 1514. CR: Composite reliability. AVE: Average variance extracted.
Table 3. Discriminant validity.
Table 3. Discriminant validity.
ConstructsHeterotrait–Monotrait (HTMT) Analysis
SDNSINESHAOCAORPBCAESESIPNESBCOLLTO
SDN
SIN0.672
HB0.5510.360
AOC0.5330.3310.403
AOR0.5880.3310.4120.787
PBC0.7540.5090.5610.6230.634
AES0.6780.4960.5210.6060.5830.749
ESI0.7600.5610.6620.5820.5970.7380.753
PMN0.7030.4640.5560.6490.6490.7760.6610.708
ESB0.6040.4190.5440.5570.5510.6520.7230.7030.577
COL0.4220.2460.4400.5240.5270.4340.3850.4590.4930.402
LTO0.5890.3280.4390.6740.6950.5830.5500.5900.5980.5590.596
Notes: N = 1514.
Table 4. Regression models.
Table 4. Regression models.
VariablesAttitude towards
Energy-Saving
Personal NormsEnergy-Saving
Intention
Energy-Saving
Behaviour
Energy-Saving Habit
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4Model 5Model 6Model 7Model 8Model 9Model 10
Constant3.967 ***
(0.067)
0.832 ***
(0.101)
3.897 ***
(0.066)
1.333 ***
(0.104)
3.861 ***
(0.064)
0.908 ***
(0.092)
3.984 ***
(0.067)
1.985 ***
(0.109)
3.710 ***
(0.080)
0.193 ***
(0.133)
Standardised coefficientsββββββββββ
Gender−0.005
(0.029)
0.031
(0.021)
−0.051 *
(0.028)
−0.040
(0.023)
−0.031 ***
(0.028)
−0.012
(0.020)
0.001
(0.029)
0.015
(0.023)
−0.019
(0.080)
−0.020
(0.030)
Age0.068 *
(0.014)
−0.008
(0.010)
0.113 ***
(0.013)
0.049 *
(0.011)
0.129 *
(0.013)
0.061 **
(0.010)
0.100 ***
(0.014)
0.012
(0.011)
0.114 ***
(0.016)
0.051 *
(0.914)
Educational level0.105 ***
(0.026)
0.095 ***
(0.019)
0.024
(0.025)
0.004
(0.020)
0.054 ***
(0.025)
−0.002
(0.018)
0.047
(0.026)
0.013
(0.021)
0.101 ***
(0.031)
0.063 **
(0.027)
Monthly income−0.129 ***
(0.017)
−0.078 ***
(0.012)
−0.056
(0.016)
−0.022
(0.013)
−0.095 ***
(0.016)
−0.019
(0.011)
−0.090 **
(0.017)
−0.034
(0.013)
0.089 **
(0.020)
−0.042
(0.017)
Subjective injunctive norm 0.099 ***
(0.021)
Subjective descriptive norm 0.250 ***
(0.026)
Perceived behavioural control 0.435 ***
(0.025)
Ascription of responsibility 0.321 ***
(0.029)
Awareness of consequence 0.324 ***
(0.029)
Attitude towards energy-saving 0.451 ***
(0.021)
Personal norms 0.329 ***
(0.022)
Energy-saving intention 0.308 ***
(0.042)
Energy-saving behaviour 0.153 ***
(0.049)
Collectivism x Energy-saving intention 0.030
(0.005)
Long-term orientation x Energy-saving intention 0.294 ***
(0.007)
Collectivism x Energy-saving behaviour 0.139 **
(0.008)
Long-term orientation x Energy-saving behaviour 0.265 ***
(0.006)
R20.0230.4750.0140.3620.0190.4870.0130.3680.0230.283
Adjusted R20.0200.4730.0110.3590.0170.4850.0100.3650.0210.279
Notes: N = 1514, results are based on trimmed scales. The figures in parentheses are standard errors. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Mediation analyses.
Table 5. Mediation analyses.
Mediation Regression Coefficient PathsEffectsBootSEBootstrap 95% CIs
LLCIULCI
ESIESBESH 0.15140.02660.10120.2047
AESESIESB 0.20180.02330.15580.2475
AESESIESBESH 0.04180.01030.02360.0639
PNESIESB 0.27840.02920.22100.3365
PNESIESBESH 0.05660.01350.03160.0850
SINAESESI 0.19530.02560.14410.2452
SINAESESIESB 0.06270.01070.04310.0849
SINAESESIESBESH0.01290.00350.00690.0206
SDNAESESI 0.22710.02380.18110.2741
SDNAESESIESB 0.06440.01100.04420.0871
SDNAESESIESBESH0.01220.00370.00600.0205
PBCAESESI 0.26030.02460.21130.3079
PBCAESESIESB 0.07240.01120.05140.0953
PBCAESESIESBESH0.01310.00390.00630.0215
AORPNESI 0.24010.02420.19350.2881
AORPNESIESB 0.10320.01490.07570.1335
AORPNESIESBESH0.02100.00570.01100.0333
AOCPNESI 0.24650.02540.19630.2976
AOCPNESIESB 0.10590.01520.07750.1372
AOCPNESIESBESH0.02180.00590.01150.0347
Note: N = 1514, results are based on trimmed scales. LLCI: Lower level of confidence interval. ULCI: Upper level of confidence interval. SE: Standard errors.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Xuan, V.N.; Loan, L.T.; Hoa, N.M.; Dao, N.T. Using a Unified Model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to Investigate the Energy-Saving Behaviour of Urban Residents in Vietnam: Moderation Role of Cultural Values. Sustainability 2023, 15, 2225. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15032225

AMA Style

Xuan VN, Loan LT, Hoa NM, Dao NT. Using a Unified Model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to Investigate the Energy-Saving Behaviour of Urban Residents in Vietnam: Moderation Role of Cultural Values. Sustainability. 2023; 15(3):2225. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15032225

Chicago/Turabian Style

Xuan, Vu Ngoc, Le Thi Loan, Nguyen Minh Hoa, and Nguyen Thi Dao. 2023. "Using a Unified Model of TPB, NAM, and SOBC to Investigate the Energy-Saving Behaviour of Urban Residents in Vietnam: Moderation Role of Cultural Values" Sustainability 15, no. 3: 2225. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15032225

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop