Next Article in Journal
Heavy Quark Symmetry and Fine Structure of the Spectrum of Hadronic Dark Matter
Previous Article in Journal
Symmetries in Teleportation Assisted by N-Channels under Indefinite Causal Order and Post-Measurement
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Hand Preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana Infants: Is It Influenced by Familial Inheritance?

1
Shaanxi Key Laboratory for Animal Conservation, Shaanxi Institute of Zoology, Xi’an 710032, China
2
Shaanxi Key Laboratory for Animal Conservation, College of Life Sciences, Northwest University, Xi’an 710069, China
3
Center for Excellence in Animal Evolution and Genetics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming 650223, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2020, 12(11), 1905; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12111905
Submission received: 1 November 2020 / Revised: 15 November 2020 / Accepted: 18 November 2020 / Published: 20 November 2020

Abstract

:
The Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) is a typical arboreal group-living Old-World primate and has been studied broadly in hand preference. However, infants have not been tested independently from other immature individuals to date. The purpose of the present study was to investigate hand preference in a spontaneously unimanual feeding task in nine infants at 12 months and the relationship of hand preference with their parents in R. roxellanae. Most infants (89%) showed individual-level hand preference. No correlation was found in the direction of hand preference between infant and its parents, and a significant negative correlation in the strength of hand preference was found between infants and their mothers (r = −0.715, p = 0.03). Moreover, there was no sex difference in the direction and strength of hand preference both in infants and adults (i.e., parents). Meanwhile, the strength of hand preference in adults was stronger than that in infants. This study is a first and preliminary exploration for the expression of hand preference in R. roxellanae infants and whether their hand preference was influenced by familial inheritance.

1. Introduction

Handedness was considered as the most obvious index and an exclusive characteristic in human brain asymmetry and has been greatly studied [1,2]. Almost 90% of humans showed right-hand preference at a population level [3]. However, the evolution and origin of human right-handedness remain unsolved [4,5]. Previous studies showed that hand preference was present at birth and appears gradually during development in humans. For example, no more than 6-month-old infants begin to present strong hand preference, become stronger at 6 months of life and are tending towards stability over these ages [6,7,8,9]. More recently one review summarized that no one study can completely excluded genetic or environmental influence on the formation of hand preference [10]. One previous review, meanwhile, also emphasized the genetic influence (i.e., familial inheritance) on the development of handedness and indicated that the influence was supported by compelling evidence [1]. The handedness of a child showed a positive correlation with that of its biological parents, but not with that of adoptive parents [11,12,13]. In addition, a child was more likely to be right-handed when its parents were right-handed compared with its parents that were left-handed or different direction of handedness [13,14,15]. Although these research studies showed the impact of familial inheritance, at least to some extent, on the development of handedness, the question about the inheritance of handedness is not so straightforward [1,10].
Research on the development of limb preferences had been reported in many other nonhuman mammalian species (e.g., carnivorous [16], marsupials [17] and nonhuman primates [18]). In these species, only nonhuman primates were considered as an excessive model for exploring the origin and evolution of human hemispheric specialization because of the similar brain asymmetry and the relatively close biological systems with humans [19]. However, it is less clear when infant handedness appears. One early study in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) reported that significant hand preference was developed by 5–8 months in an unimaual holding task and was maintained from 10 to 70 months [18]. Although there were some studies related to infants, these studies were not completely removed in infants from other ages [20,21,22], nor neglected a difference on handedness among species [23]. In addition, the influence of familial inheritance on handedness is rarely focused on nonhuman primates, and existing data were contradictory. Some research results showed a significant correlation between the handedness of offspring and that of its parents (mainly of its mother), including New World monkeys (common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus jacchus) [24]; capuchins (Cebus paella) [22]) and Old World monkeys (rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and pit-tailed macaques (M. leonine) [25]). On the contrary, no significant correlation was founded in other studies, including Old World monkeys (bonnett macaques (M. radiate) [26]) and apes (gorillas (Gorilla gorilla berengei) [27]; chimpanzees (Pan troglodyte) [28]). The current research findings are not enough to reveal the complex relationships of familial inheritance and handedness, and more investigation is needed in other species for further understanding the development mechanism of handedness in non-human primates.
The Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) is a typical arboreal group-living Old-World primate and has been studied broadly in hand preference in different behavioral tasks [29,30,31,32,33]. Although some studies focus on adults and immatures [29,30], infants have not been tested independently in hand preference to date. The close relationship with their mothers in their first years makes us recognize the infants from other individuals of the same age. The main purpose of the present study was to investigate hand use of 12-month-old infants during unimanual feeding in the wild. Meanwhile, the relationship with their parents will also be investigated for exploring the development level of handedness in the first year of life and the potential factor influencing the expression of handedness in R. roxellana.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Site and Species

Our study site was in Guanyinshan National Nature Reserve (1150–2574 m above sea level) on the southern slopes of Qinling Mountains in China. For R. roxellanae lived in the Qinling Mountains, deciduous broadleaf forests at 1400–2200 m above sea level are their most favorite, the mixed coniferous broadleaf forests at 2200–2600 m above sea level are the second favorite, and finally the coniferous forests at 2600–2900 m above sea level are the third favorite. Two wild populations were observed at the study site and the focus population was the one that was found in 2008, which consists of multi-one-male units and one-multi-male unit. During the observation period, the field assistants began to search for the focus population at about 5:00 a.m. every day. Once the monkeys in the focus population were found, field assistants attracted them to the provisioning site [34]. Approximately 200 g of corn grain, sliced apple and radish were provided per monkey per day at three-time points (9:00 a.m., 12:00 a.m. and 3: 00 p.m.) [34,35]. All adult individuals were identified mainly via their physical characteristics by a close distance observation (5–50 m). The mother-offspring dyads were determined based on suckling of infants and 9 infants and their parents (9 females and 5 males) were involved in this study. Our study conformed to the national laws and regulations on animal care in China.

2.2. Data Collection

Data were collected between 9:00 a.m. and 7:00 p.m. over 56 days from December 2019 to May 2020. Unimanual feeding was used for the assessment of hand preference in R. roxellanae. Hand use during feeding on the ground was developed in R. roxellanae at the third month of life. We only recorded the behavior when a subject began to feed on the ground and kept a bipedal posture. In this posture, two hind limbs were mainly used to suspending its body weight and one of forelimbs was sometimes used as a supplementary role to maintain stability (Figure 1). The individual, sex, age and the dominant hand were recorded as a data point. The dominant hand was the one that firstly grasped the food on the ground and brought it into its mouth, and then the other hand considered as subordinate tended to be unused or placed on its hind limb. Data of individual dominant hand were recorded as the word “L” if the individual showed left-side dominant and right-side subordinate, or as “R” if the one showed right-side dominant and left-side subordinate in unimanual feeding.
Focus animal sampling and behavioral sampling [36] were used for the data collection of hand preference in R. roxellanae. Behavioral sampling was used at the beginning of the observation, while focus animal sampling was only used to obtain reasonable data points per subject at the final stage of the observation. If the focal subject was continuing feeding under the same place and time, the data were recorded only once. A minimum of 30 data points per subject was collected in our study.

2.3. Data Analyses

There have been three indexes for the analysis of hand preference. The handedness index (HI) and the absolute value of HI (ABS-HI) were used to assess the degree and strength of individual hand preference, respectively. The HI score was obtained from the formula: (R − L)/(R + L), and the scores varied between −1.0 and 1.0, indicating left and right hand preference, respectively. The last index was the binomial z-score, which was used to determine whether the frequency of right- or left-hand use was higher than that was expected by chance (50% right-hand use). The subjects were classified as left-hand preference (z ≤ −1.96), right hand preference (z ≥ 1.96) and ambidextrous (−1.96 < z <1.96) according to the z-scores [28].
To exclude the influence of the number of observations, we used the Spearman correlation test to test the relationship between the number of data points per subject and the HI scores and ABS-HI scores. We performed one-sample tests to evaluate the population-level hand preference based on individual HI scores. Finally, we used the Mann–Whitney U-test to evaluate sex and age influences on hand preference. SPSS 23.0 and a two-tailed test with a level of significance of p ≤ 0.05 were used in all analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Hand Preference in Infants

In total, 329 data points were recorded in 9 R. roxellana infants. The number of observations per individual was 36.56 ± 1.67 (range: 31–47) and was no significant correlation with HI scores (r = −0.49, p = 0.181) and ABS-LI scores (r = −0.029, p = 0.941), respectively, indicating that handedness was not affected by the number of observations. Moreover, the mean HI and ABS-HI scores were −0.21 ± 0.13 (range: −0.56–0.35) and 0.41 ± 0.04 (range: 0.14–0.56), respectively (Table 1). Based on the z-scores, there were six left side (67%), two right bias (22%) and one ambidextrous (11%) hand preferences at an individual level (Figure 2); no significant hand preference was showed at a population level in infant R. roxellana (t8 = −1.59; p = 0.150). Then we evaluated the sex effect on hand preference of infants and showed no significant differences between females and males in both HI scores (Na = 5, Nb = 4; U = 10.00, p = 1.00) and ABS-HI scores (Na = 5, Nb = 4; U = 4.00, p = 0.142) (Figure 3A).

3.2. Hand Preference in Adults

We recorded the parents of R. roxellana infants as for adults and 732 data points were recorded in all 14 adults. The number of observations per individual was 52.29 ± 4.51 (range: 34–88) and was no significant correlation with HI scores (r = 0.261, p = 0.367) and ABS-LI scores (r = −0.197, p = 0.499), respectively, indicating that handedness was not affected by the number of observations. The mean HI and ABS-HI scores were −0.14 ± 0.18 (range: −0.80–0.90) and 0.67 ± 0.04 (range: 0.36–0.90), respectively (Table 2). Based on the z-scores, nine individuals were left-handed (64%) and five right-handed (36%) were found in individual level, and there was no one showed ambidextrous handedness (Figure 1); there was no significant hand preference was showed at a population level in adults (t13 = − 0.780; p = 0.449). As with as the sex effect on handedness in infants, there was also no significant differences between females and males in both HI scores (Na = 9, Nb = 5; U = 21.00, p = 0.841) and ABS-HI scores (Na = 9, Nb = 5; U = 20.00, p = 0.739) in adults (Figure 3B).

3.3. Relationship between Infants and Their Parents

We firstly compared the effect of familial inheritance on handedness in R. roxellana. Based on HI scores, the handedness of infants was no significant correlation with their mothers (r = −0.119, p = 0.761) and their fathers (r = −0.013, p = 0.974). Based on ABS-HI scores, there was also no significant correlation between infants and their fathers (r = −0.420, p = 0.260), but a significant negative correlation was found between infants and their mothers (r = −0.715, p = 0.03). Moreover, we considered the effects of age on hand preference in R. roxellana. No significant difference was found between infants and adults in HI scores (Na = 14, Nb = 9; U = 51.00, p = 0.450), while the hand preference of adults was stronger than that in infants in strength of hand preference (Na = 14, Nb = 9; U = 12.00, p = 0.001) in R. roxellana (Figure 4).

4. Discussion

This is the first study on hand preference in 12-month-old R. roxellana infants in wild settings. Studying handedness in infants has been motivated by accumulated evidence suggesting that hand preference can be identified during early infancy. A majority of infants (89%) show a significant hand preference at the individual level at 12 months. The result reveals obvious brain function asymmetry at 12 months of age in R. roxellana. Previous research showed that when an individual reached a particular age (e.g., proximately 14 months in bonnett macaques [26]; 3-year-old in gorillas [27]), the degree of laterality was no longer increasing with age. Concerning R. roxellana, it is not enough to assess the particular age and the developmental trajectories of infant handedness need to be investigated in the next. Additionally, no sex effect was found in hand preference in infants, revealing brain development between sexes reached the same extent at 12 months. The finding was consistent with that in other nonhuman primates (such as 12-month-old capuchins and bonnett macaques) [26,37].
No significant correlations were obtained in the direction of hand preference between infants and their parents in this study. The result was consistent with that in bonnet macaques [26], rather than rhesus macaques and pig-tailed macaques [25], although they all belong to Old World monkeys. Previous studies also used the unimanual reaching to investigate the hand preference. As with as our finding in R. roxellana, bonnet macaques did not show a significant population-level handedness [26]. On the contrary, rhesus macaques and pit-tailed macaques showed hand preference at a population level because they reached food in a quadrupedal posture [25]. Although there was no evidence that supported that the direction of handedness was influenced by either parent in reaching task in capuchin monkeys, a correction was found in the direction of looking bias between offspring and parents [22]. Based on these findings [22,25,26], we should select the lateralized behavior that can elicit a stronger preference than simple reaching task (such as bimanual coordinated task [29,32]) or other lateralized behaviors (such as turning [22]) to further exclude the influence of manual tasks on handedness.
Surprisingly, the present research showed a negative correlation in the strength of hand preference between the infant and its mother. The finding is inconsistent with results from other nonhuman primates in showing no correlation between parents and offspring in the strength of hand preference (such as capuchins [22] and chimpanzees [38]). Although the strength of lateralization increased as monkeys matured and finally reached the same lateralization degrees as adults, it is unknown how long the negative correlation was maintained before the monkey matured.
No population-level hand preference was found in adult R. roxellana, which is not consistent with one previous study on a unimanual task of the same species [29]. The low sample size in our study may be one of the reasons for the difference found in the same task and species. The difference between the provisioned foods also may lead to the different results. The focal population in the previous study was provisioned by more corn than that provisioned to our focus population. Corn was so small and light that the monkeys required precision grasping and therefore, were more likely to elicit greater hand preference at a population level [29,39]. Besides, our focus population was provisioned for about six years, while the one in the previous study had been provisioned for more than ten years during the collection of data [29,40]. Thus, the focus population in the previous study may have more obvious and steady manual skills in feeding artifact provisioned food than our focus population.
Compared with infants, adults showed stronger handedness in the present study. Hopkins and Bard in their early study mentioned a positive correlation between the strength of hand preference and age [4] and the view was supported by other studies in nonhuman primates. For example, adult capuchins (mean age per subject: 12.1) also expressed a stronger hand preference than did immature individuals (mean age per subject: 1.4) [21]. Likewise, the degree of laterality increased with age in bonnett macaques before 14 months [26]. The result is also quite similar to these obtained in humans [41].
As with infants, adults also showed no sex effect in hand preference. The result agreed with previous studies in the same task and species [29,30,31] as well as other nonhuman primates [42,43]. It is important to note that some studies reported sex difference, especially on the bimanual coordinated task, including apes (e.g., gorillas [43]; chimpanzee [44]; orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus) [45]), Old-World monkeys (e.g., de Brazza’s monkeys (Cercopithecus neglectus) [46]) and New-World monkeys (e.g., capuchins [47]; white-face capuchins (C. capucinus) [39]). However, for the unimanual reaching task, a sex effect on hand preference was reported in very few research studies. For example, Meguerditchian and colleges found that a significant right-handedness was mostly found only in female squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus), while a higher degree of population-level left-handedness was found in males than that in females [20]. About the previous studies on R. roxellana, no sex effect was found either in the unimanual task [29,30,31] or in the bimanual coordinated task [33], which was also supported by the current study.

5. Conclusions

This is the first investigation on the expression of hand preference in 12-month-old R. roxellana infants and the relationship with their parents. Our results showed hand preference at the individual level during unimanual feeding at 12 months of age. The important result of this study was no correlation in the direction of hand preference between infants and their parents, and a negative correlation in the strength of hand preference was only found in the mother–offspring relationship. Moreover, we also found no sex difference in the direction and strength of hand preference in infants and adults (i.e., their parents). Meanwhile, the strength of hand preference in adults was stronger than that in infants in our study. These findings were consistent with previous studies on the same species [29,30,31,32,33] and provided some new evidence for the research on sex and age influences on hand preference in R. roxellana.

Author Contributions

Methodology, W.-W.F.; formal analysis, W.-W.F., and C.-L.W.; investigation, W.-W.F., and Y.R.; writing—original draft preparation: W.-W.F., and X.-W.W.; writing—review and editing, B.-G.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the key project of Nature science Function of China, No. 31730104; Natural Science Foundation of China, No. 31900338, No. 32070457 and No. 32000317; Key Foundation of Shaanxi Province, No. 2018ZDXM-NY-049; Natural Science Foundation of Shaanxi Province, No. 2019JQ-989; “One institute one brand” of Shaanxi Academy of Sciences, No. 2020k-01.

Acknowledgments

We thank to the director and staff of Foping tourism administration for their permission to conduct this research. We also thank to Jian-Qiang Zhao from the Foping Culture and Tourism Administration for providing the picture and Ming-Wen Qiao from the Weinan Normal University for the language revision.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ocklenburg, S.; Beste, C.; Güntürkün, O. Handedness: A neurogenetic shift of perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2788–2793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Perelle, I.B.; Ehrman, L. On the other hand. Behav. Genet. 2005, 35, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Annett, M. Handedness and Brain Asymmetry: The Right Shift Theory; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hopkins, W.D.; Bard, K.A. The ontogeny of lateralized behavior in nonhuman primates with special reference to chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In Primate Laterality: Current Behavioral Evidence of Primate Asymmetries; Ward, J.P., Hopkins, W.D., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1993; pp. 251–265. [Google Scholar]
  5. Olulade, O.A.; Seydell-Greenwald, A.; Chambers, C.E.; Turkeltaub, P.E.; Dromerick, A.W.; Berl, M.M.; Gaillard, W.D.; Newport, E.L. The neural basis of language development: Changes in lateralization over age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 23477–23483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Campbell, J.M.; Marcinowski, E.C.; Babik, I.; Michel, G.F. The influence of a hand preference for acquiring objects on the development of a hand preference for unimanual manipulation from 6 to 14 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015, 39, 107–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Campbell, J.M.; Marcinowski, E.C.; Michel, G.F. The development of neuromotor skills and hand preference during infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 2017, 60, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Ferre, C.L.; Babik, I.; Michel, G.F. Development of infant prehension handedness: A longitudinal analysis during the 6- to 14-month age period. Infant Behav. Dev. 2010, 33, 492–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Michel, G.F.; Babik, I.; Sheu, C.F.; Campbell, J.M. Latent classes in the developmental trajectories of infant handedness. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 50, 349–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Marcori, A.J.; Okazaki, V.H.A. A historical, systematic review of handedness origins. Laterality 2020, 25, 87–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Carter-Saltzman, L. Biological and sociocultural effects on handedness: Comparison between biological and adoptive parents. Science 1980, 209, 1263–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Curt, F.; De Agostini, M.; Maccario, J.; Dellatolas, G. Parental hand preference and manual functional asymmetry in preschool children. Behav. Genet. 1995, 25, 525–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. McManus, I.C.; Bryden, M.P. The genetics of handedness, cerebral dominance, and lateralization. In Handbook of Neuropsychology; Boller, F., Rafman, J., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1992; Volume 6, pp. 115–143. [Google Scholar]
  14. Laland, K.N. Exploring gene–culture interactions: Insights from handedness, sexual selection and niche-construction case studies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. 2008, 363, 3577–3589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Porac, C.; Coren, S. Life-span age trends in the perception of the mueller-lyer: Additional evidence for the existence of two illusions. Can. J. Psychol. 1981, 35, 58–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Wells, D.L.; Millsopp, S. The ontogenesis of lateralized behavior in the domestic cat, Felis silvestris catus. J. Comp. Psychol. 2012, 126, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Giljov, A.; Karenina, K.; Ingram, J.; Malashichev, Y. Early expression of manual lateralization in bipedal marsupials. J. Comp. Psychol. 2017, 131, 225–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Hook, M.A.; Rogers, L.J. Development of hand preferences in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and effects of aging. J. Comp. Psychol. 2000, 114, 263–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Hopkins, W.D. The Evolution of Hemispheric Specialization in Primates; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  20. Meguerditchian, A.; Donnot, J.; Molesti, S.; Francioly, R.; Vauclair, J. Sex difference in squirrel monkeys’ handedness for unimanual and bimanual tasks. Anim. Behav. 2012, 83, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Westergaard, G.C.; Suomi, S.J. Hand preference in capuchin monkeys varies with age. Primates 1993, 34, 295–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Westergaard, G.C.; Suomi, S.J. Lateral bias in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Concordance between parents and offspring. Dev. Psychobiol. 1997, 31, 143–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ward, J.P.; Milliken, G.W.; Dodson, D.L.; Stafford, D.K.; Wallace, M. Handedness as a function of sex and age in a large population of Lemur. J. Comp. Psychol. 1990, 104, 167–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Matoba, M.; Masataka, N.; Tanioka, Y. Cross-generational continuity of hand-use preferences in marmosets. Behaviour 1991, 117, 281–286. [Google Scholar]
  25. Westergaard, G.C.; Lussier, I.D.; Higley, J.D. Between-species variation in the development of hand preference among macaques. Neuropsychologia 2001, 39, 1373–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Brooker, R.J.; Lehman, R.A.W.; Heimbuch, R.C.; Kidd, K.K. Hand usage in a colony of bonnett monkeys, Macaca radiata. Behav. Genet. 1981, 11, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Byrne, R.W.; Byrne, J.M. Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. berengei). Cortex 1991, 27, 521–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hopkins, W.D. Heritability of hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence from a partial interspecies cross-fostering study. J. Comp. Psychol. 1999, 113, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fu, W.W.; Wang, X.W.; Wang, C.L.; Zhao, H.T.; Ren, Y.; Li, B.G. Effects of age, sex and manual task on hand preference in wild Rhinopithecus roxellana. Zool. Res. 2019, 40, 129–138. [Google Scholar]
  30. Liang, B.; Zhang, S.Y. Hand preference in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Acta Theriol. Sin. 1998, 18, 107–111. [Google Scholar]
  31. Zhao, D.P.; Ji, W.H.; Watanabe, K.; Li, B.G. Hand preference during unimanual and bimanual reaching actions in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Am. J. Primatol. 2008, 70, 500–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Zhao, D.P.; Gao, X.; Li, B.G. Hand preference for spontaneously unimanual and bimanual coordinated tasks in wild Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys: Implication for hemispheric specialization. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 208, 85–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Zhao, D.P.; Hopkins, W.D.; Li, B.G. Handedness in nature: First evidence on manual laterality on bimanual coordinated tube task in wild primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2012, 148, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Zhang, P.; Watanabe, K.; Li, B.G.; Tan, C.L. Social organization of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains, Central China. Primates 2006, 47, 374–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Li, B.G.; Zhao, D.P. Copulation behavior within one-male groups of wild Rhinopithecus roxellana in the Qinling Mountains of China. Primates 2007, 48, 190–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Altmann, J. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Westergaard, G.C.; Byrne, G.; Suomi, S.J. Early lateral bias in tufted capuchins (Cebus apella). Dev. Psychobiol. 1998, 32, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hopkins, W.D.; Bales, S.A.; Bennett, A.J. Heritability of hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan). Intern. J. Neuroence. 1994, 74, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Meunier, H.; Vauclair, J. Hand preferences on unimanual and bimanual tasks in white-face capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am. J. Primatol. 2007, 69, 1064–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Li, B.G.; Chen, C.; Ji, W.H.; Ren, B.P. Seasonal home range changes of the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains of China. Folia. Primatol. 2000, 71, 375–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Teixeira, L.A. Categories of manual asymmetry and their variation with advancing age. Cortex 2008, 44, 707–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. McGrew, W.C.; Marchant, L.F. On the other hand: Current issues in and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality of hand function in non-human primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1997, 40, 201–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Meguerditchian, A.; Calcutt, S.E.; Lonsdorf, E.V.; Ross, S.R.; Hopkins, W.D. Brief communication: Captive gorillas are right-handed for bimanual feeding. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2010, 141, 638–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Corp, N.; Byrne, R.W. Sex difference in chimpanzee handedness. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2004, 123, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Rogers, L.J.; Kaplan, G. Hand preferences and other lateral biases in rehabilitated orangutans, Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus. Anim. Behav. 1996, 51, 13–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Schweitzer, C.; Bec, P.; Blois-Heulin, C. Does the complexity of the task influence manual laterality in de Brazza’s monkeys (Cercopithecus neglectus)? Ethology 2007, 113, 983–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Phillips, K.A.; Sherwood, C.C. Cerebral petalias and their relationship to handedness in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 2398–2401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Unimanual feeding behavior in R. roxellanae. One 12-month-old infant (the intermediate individual), its father (the right individual) and its mother (the left individual) were all feeding on the ground under a bipedal posture.
Figure 1. Unimanual feeding behavior in R. roxellanae. One 12-month-old infant (the intermediate individual), its father (the right individual) and its mother (the left individual) were all feeding on the ground under a bipedal posture.
Symmetry 12 01905 g001
Figure 2. The percentage of individuals exhibiting left-side, right-side and ambidextrous hand preference in R. roxellana infants and adults based on the z-scores. There is no one showing ambidextrous in adult individuals.
Figure 2. The percentage of individuals exhibiting left-side, right-side and ambidextrous hand preference in R. roxellana infants and adults based on the z-scores. There is no one showing ambidextrous in adult individuals.
Symmetry 12 01905 g002
Figure 3. Sex differences on the direction and strength of hand preference in R. roxellana based on HI scores and ABS-HI scores, respectively. (A) Sex differences in adult individuals. (B) Sex differences in infant individuals. There were no significant sex differences both in adult and infant R. roxellan. ABS-HI, the absolute value of HI.
Figure 3. Sex differences on the direction and strength of hand preference in R. roxellana based on HI scores and ABS-HI scores, respectively. (A) Sex differences in adult individuals. (B) Sex differences in infant individuals. There were no significant sex differences both in adult and infant R. roxellan. ABS-HI, the absolute value of HI.
Symmetry 12 01905 g003
Figure 4. Comparison between adults and infants in the direction and strength of hand preference in R. roxellana based on HI scores and ABS-HI scores, respectively. * represented a significant difference between adults and infants in the strength of hand preference (p ≤ 0.05).
Figure 4. Comparison between adults and infants in the direction and strength of hand preference in R. roxellana based on HI scores and ABS-HI scores, respectively. * represented a significant difference between adults and infants in the strength of hand preference (p ≤ 0.05).
Symmetry 12 01905 g004
Table 1. Hand preference in 12-month-old R. roxellana infants.
Table 1. Hand preference in 12-month-old R. roxellana infants.
NumberSexL/RHIz-ScoreHandedness
1Female34/13−0.45−3.06Left
2Female24/10−0.41−2.40Left
3Male7/240.553.05Right
4Female24/12−0.33−2.00Left
5Male25/7−0.56−3.18Left
6Female20/15−0.14−0.85Ambidextrous
7Male11/230.352.06Right
8Male28/12−0.40−2.53Left
9Male30/10−0.50−3.16Left
L, left-side dominant/right-side subordinate; R, right-side dominant/left-side subordinate; HI, the handedness index.
Table 2. Hand preference of adult R. roxellana.
Table 2. Hand preference of adult R. roxellana.
NumberMotherFather
NameL/RHIz-ScoreHandednessNameL/RHIz-ScoreHandedness
1QB28/6−0.65−3.77LeftHD46/5−0.80−5.74Left
2MB36/8−0.64−4.22Left
3FB36/17−0.36−2.61Left
4XF32/4−0.78−4.67LeftDX7/810.847.89Right
5PP41/7−0.71−4.91Left
6KD2/380.905.69RightYQ6/220.573.02Right
7CY32/6−0.68−4.22Left
8YK3/420.875.81RightBD44/15−0.49−3.78Left
9LS19/530.474.01RightXL64/17−0.58−5.22Left
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fu, W.-W.; Ren, Y.; Wang, C.-L.; Wang, X.-W.; Li, B.-G. Hand Preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana Infants: Is It Influenced by Familial Inheritance? Symmetry 2020, 12, 1905. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12111905

AMA Style

Fu W-W, Ren Y, Wang C-L, Wang X-W, Li B-G. Hand Preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana Infants: Is It Influenced by Familial Inheritance? Symmetry. 2020; 12(11):1905. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12111905

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fu, Wei-Wei, Yi Ren, Cheng-Liang Wang, Xiao-Wei Wang, and Bao-Guo Li. 2020. "Hand Preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana Infants: Is It Influenced by Familial Inheritance?" Symmetry 12, no. 11: 1905. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12111905

APA Style

Fu, W. -W., Ren, Y., Wang, C. -L., Wang, X. -W., & Li, B. -G. (2020). Hand Preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana Infants: Is It Influenced by Familial Inheritance? Symmetry, 12(11), 1905. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12111905

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop