Next Article in Journal
Cancer Patient-Derived Cell-Based Models: Applications and Challenges in Functional Precision Medicine
Previous Article in Journal
The Prevalence of Arrhythmias, Including Premature Supraventricular and Ventricular Beats and Other Electrocardiographic Patterns, in 24-Hour Holter Monitoring in Patients with Overweight and Obesity
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Executive Functions and Cognitive Variables in Experimentally Induced Acute Pain Perception during a Distraction Task: A Study on Asymptomatic Pain-Free Individuals

by
Angela Tejera-Alonso
1,2,
Francisco G. Fernández-Palacios
1,2,*,
Juan C. Pacho-Hernández
1,
Arvin Naeimi
1,3,
Ana I. de-la-Llave-Rincón
4,
Silvia Ambite-Quesada
4,
Ricardo Ortega-Santiago
4,
César Fernández-de-las-Peñas
4 and
Margarita Cigarán-Mendez
1
1
Department of Psychology, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, 28922 Alcorcón, Spain
2
Escuela Internacional de Doctorado, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, 28922 Alcorcón, Spain
3
Student Research Committee, School of Medicine, Guilan University of Medical Sciences, Rasht 41446-66949, Iran
4
Department Physical Therapy, Occupational Therapy, Rehabilitation, and Physical Medicine, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, 28922 Alcorcón, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Life 2024, 14(9), 1141; https://doi.org/10.3390/life14091141
Submission received: 6 August 2024 / Revised: 7 September 2024 / Accepted: 8 September 2024 / Published: 10 September 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Physiology and Pathology)

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to investigate the influence of executive functioning and cognitive performance on individual experimentally induced pain perception during distractor tasks in an asymptomatic pain-free population. A total of 59 healthy pain-free subjects (59.3% women, mean age: 46.5 ± 24.7 years) completed a battery test that assessed execution functions (cognitive flexibility, working memory, mental inhibition), attention level, and psychological aspects (anxiety/depressive levels—HADS, pain catastrophizing—PCS, pain anxiety symptoms—PASS 20, sleep quality—PSQI) before conducting two n-back distraction tasks. Pain was experimentally induced with a thermal stimulus that was able to induce moderate pain (70/100 points) and applied to the non-dominant forearm. The thermal stimulus was applied before and during both (one-back and two-back) distraction tasks. The analyses consisted of separated repeated-measures ANOVA that considered the functioning on each test (cognitive flexibility, working memory, mental inhibition, selective attention) and controlled for sociodemographic and psychological aspects by comparing the pain intensity at the baseline and during the one-back and two-back distractor tasks. All ANOVAs found a significant effect of the distraction task, which indicates that the perceived pain intensity scores were lower during the one-back and two-back tasks (p < 0.001) as compared with the baseline. No interaction effect between the distractor tasks and working memory (p = 0.546), mental inhibition (p = 0.16), cognitive flexibility (p = 0.069), or selective attention (p = 0.105) was identified. The current study found that a distraction task decreased the perceived intensity of experimentally induced pain in asymptomatic pain-free individuals and that this effect was not related to executive function or attention levels.

1. Introduction

Millions of patients annually suffer from pain because of trauma, illness, or surgery. Pain is the most common reason for admission to emergency departments, where it comprises more than 40% of the over 100 million visits in the United States of America each year [1]. Pain is a multidimensional personal experience that is affected by biological, psychological, and social factors [2]. Biopsychosocial pain models suggest that an individual’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors can modulate pain perception [3]. This multidimensional nature accounts for inter-individual variability within the pain experience [2,4]. Experimental evidence strongly supports this multidimensional nature of the pain experience, thus indicating that pain perception involves a widely distributed neural network in pain processing [4,5]. Several cortical regions engaged in pain processing are also implicated in cognitive processes [6]. Therefore, it is not surprising that increasing evidence suggests a potential involvement of cognitive function in pain sensitivity. Besides the well-recognized role of the attentional control function in pain experience [7], studies underscore that other executive functions may also be crucial in this process [8].
Executive functions encompass advanced cognitive processes, such as sustained attention, response inhibition, working memory, and error processing, which enables subjects to direct their behavior toward goals in an adaptive manner [9]. Research indicates that in the realm of executive functioning, enhanced cognitive inhibition is linked to a reduction in sensitivity to pain and an increase in pain tolerance [10,11]. While these findings are promising, it remains unclear whether such associations are limited to cognitive inhibition domains or extend to other non-cognitive executive functions. Oosterman et al. [11] included several executive functions but found no correlation with experimental pain outcomes. However, their evaluation lacked a well-defined theoretical framework of executive functions, like the one proposed by Miyake et al. [9], which hindered the exploration of potential interactions between different executive functions. Miyake et al. [9] employed factor analyses on widely used neuropsychological and executive function tests and identified the following three factors that accounted for a significant portion of the variance: shifting between tasks or mental sets (shifting); updating or monitoring of working memory representations (updating); and suppressing dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses (inhibition). A comprehensive study into these three executive functions concerning experimentally induced pain in healthy populations is yet lacking.
Distraction is a psychological strategy commonly used for pain inhibition in individuals with chronic pain [12]. Based on the notion that focusing on stimuli unrelated to pain reduces the overall pain experience, distraction endeavors to divert attention away from the painful stimulus by engaging in a competing demand [1,13]. However, there is limited evidence regarding the effect of this strategy in the context of pain perception. The magnitude of the analgesic response of a distraction task varies across studies [14]. Although this disparity in findings may be attributed in part to differences in experimental paradigms and populations, evidence suggests that within-subject variation in executive functions, particularly cognitive inhibition abilities, could play a role in shaping pain perception [10,11,15] and may offer protection against pain-induced interference in task performance [14,16]. However, the impact of cognitive inhibition abilities on the efficacy of a distractive task remains unclear [14,16]. Additionally, the presence of mood disorders, such as anxiety or depression [17], as well as poor sleep [18], can also influence pain modulation. Nevertheless, scientific evidence is heterogeneous since the relevance of anxiety and depression is not consistent, and the effect of poor sleep is small but high for pain catastrophizing [17,18].
To the best of the author’s knowledge, there is a lack of scientific literature examining the connection between executive functioning and pain perception in adult populations. Consequently, due to the limited scientific evidence on this topic, this study investigated the potential influence of executive functioning and cognitive performance on experimentally induced pain perception after a distractor task. It was found that higher executive functioning is associated with a higher hypoalgesia of the distraction task [19], and thus, we hypothesized that subjects with higher executive and cognitive functioning would exhibit a higher pain reduction during the distraction tasks than those with lower executive and cognitive functioning. We used asymptomatic pain-free subjects to identify the influence of executive functions and cognitive performance in experimentally induced pain perception in a situation without the interference of chronic pain.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design

This experimental study utilized a 2 × 3 mixed factorial design with two independent variables and two dependent variables. The first independent variable (within-subject factor) was the type of distracting task used (two levels: 1-back task and 2-back task), whereas the second independent variable (between-subject factor) was the level of functioning on each neurocognitive test (three levels: low, medium, high). The dependent variables were the perceived intensity of experimentally induced pain at the baseline and while performing the 1-back and 2-back distractor tasks.

2.2. Participants

Consecutive healthy adults aged between 20 and 65 years old were voluntarily recruited from local announcements on bulletin boards at Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Madrid (Spain), and by social media platforms. A clinical examination was conducted to assess the exclusion criteria: (a) psychosis or significant psychiatric disorder; (b) taking tricyclic antidepressants (50 mg daily of amitriptyline or equivalent) or psychoactive medications (anticonvulsants, muscle relaxants, antipsychotics), except for low doses of benzodiazepines (10 mg daily of diazepam or equivalent); (c) a history of infectious diseases, metabolic diseases, renal diseases, endocrine diseases, neuromuscular diseases, oncological diseases, or chronic pain; (d) a history of surgery within the last 10 years; (e) pregnancy or lactation; (f) mental disability; (g) cognitive and sensory disorders; (h) caffeine intake in the 2 h prior to measurement; (i) intense physical activity on the day of the test; (j) lack of consent to participate in this study; and (k) having a score under 27 in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) screening test [20]. The MMSE is a short screening test used to measure cognitive functioning. It evaluates the areas of orientation, attention and calculation, memory, and language. Scores equal to or above 27/30 points are considered normal.
The study protocol received approval from the Ethics Committee of Universidad Rey Juan Carlos (URJC 1909202332123) in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants signed the written informed consent prior to their inclusion in this study.

2.3. Sample Size Calculation

We calculated the sample size with G*Power software v. 3.1.9.2. (Dusseldorf, Germany) with the following input parameters: α = 0.05, a target power of 0.80, and a medium effect size (f2 = 0.25). An effect size of small-to-moderate magnitude (0.25) was set according to Cohen’s criteria [21] to detect clinically significant differences. Given the cross-sectional nature of this study, no participant dropouts were anticipated. Therefore, a sample size of at least 42 participants was determined.

2.4. Psychological Variables

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) was employed to evaluate anxiety and depressive symptoms [22]. The HADS consists of 14 items with a Likert-type response format of 4 points ranging from 0 to 3 points. The adapted Spanish version by Vallejo et al. [23] shows, like the original version, a 2-factor solution: anxiety symptoms (HADS-A, 7 items, 0–21 points) and depressive symptoms (HADS-D, 7 items, 0–21 points).
The Spanish version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) was used to measure the degree of catastrophic thinking in the presence of anticipated pain. The PCS consists of 13 items with a 5-point Likert response ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (All the time) [24]. The Spanish-validated version presents a three-factor solution, like the original version: magnification (3 items), rumination (4 items), and helplessness (6 items) [25].
The short version of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale-20 (PASS-20) was used to evaluate pain anxiety [26]. The PASS-20 is a 20-item scale that assesses cognitive anxiety (“I worry when I have pain”), avoidance and escape behaviors (“I try to avoid activities that cause pain”), fear of pain (“pain sensations are terrifying”), and physiological anxiety (“pain makes me nauseous”) using a 6-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 5 (always) [26].
The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), which consists of 19 items that provide an overall score (0–21 points), was used to evaluate the quality of sleep [27]. The PSQI has shown adequate reliability, construct validity, and discriminant validity [27].

2.5. Executive Functions

Different authors studied the influence of various cognitive functions on pain perception. Among the executive functions, working memory, mental inhibition, and cognitive flexibility showed the highest relevance for these processes [28,29,30,31].
The “D/R/I Digits” subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale battery (WAIS-IV) was used to evaluate immediate memory and working memory [32]. This subtest uses sequencing skills, planning, alertness, and cognitive flexibility. It is composed of three tasks: Forward Digit Span (repeating a series of digits in the same order as presented orally), Backward Digit Span (repeating a series of digits in reverse order), and Sequencing Digit Span (repeating a sequence of numbers presented by the examiner in order from the smallest to the highest).
The “response inhibition index” of the Five-Digit Test (FDT), which is a STROOP-like task, was used to assess mental inhibition and cognitive flexibility [33]. The FDT comprises four components—reading, counting, choice, and alternation—each with varying levels of difficulty for assessing executive functions and they are applied sequentially. Each component of the test contains 50 items. The reading and counting components assess automatic and simple processes, whereas the alternation and choice components evaluate more complex processes, as they require active mental control and demand voluntary effort, which slows down the response speed. The resulting scores are as follows: Decoding_FDT, duration in seconds needed to read all numeric elements; Retrieving_FDT, duration in seconds needed to read all non-numeric elements (e.g., asterisks); Inhibiting_FDT, duration in seconds to read the group of the same numeric item; and Shifting_FDT, time in seconds to read the group of the same numeric item mixed with numeric items within a box. Finally, two complementary scores can be obtained: one for response inhibition and one for mental flexibility [33].

2.6. Attention Assessment

Attention is also a relevant cognitive process in the pain experience since it diverts the person’s attentional resources toward the stimulus that is causing pain [34,35,36,37]. Accordingly, the Spanish version of the D2 Attention Test, which assesses mental concentration, was employed [38,39]. This time-limited assessment aims to gauge the ability to focus on relevant aspects of a task while ignoring irrelevant ones, alongside the capacity to complete the task swiftly, continuously, and accurately. It comprises 14 lines, each containing 47 characters, totaling 658 elements. The lines feature the letters ‘d’ and ‘p’, sometimes accompanied by one or two small dashes positioned above or below the letters.
Participants must meticulously scan each line from left to right, marking every ‘d’ with two small dashes (above, below, or both). These are considered relevant elements, while other combinations such as ‘p’ with or without dashes and ‘d’ with either one dash or none are deemed irrelevant. Participants have 20 s per line. The scores obtained include TR (total responses), TA (correct responses, which represent correct relevant elements), O (omissions, which are relevant elements that were not marked), C (commissions, which are irrelevant elements that were marked), TOT (total test effectiveness, calculated as TR minus the sum of O and C), CON (concentration index, which is determined as TA minus C), TR+ (the line with the highest number of tested elements), TR− (the line with the lowest number of tested elements), and VAR (variation index, which is calculated as TR+ minus TR−). In the current study, only the main measure (TOT) of the D2 test was used.

2.7. Distraction Task

Distraction tasks are one of the most used strategies to reduce pain perception, which involves diverting attention from the painful stimulus by refocusing attention on another task [30,36,40].
An n-back task was used as a distraction. The n-back tasks consist of a series of letters or numbers that appear in a row. The participant performing the n-back task must determine whether the stimuli showing are the same or not as the one they saw previously, depending on the n-back number (i.e., 1-back, 2-back, 3-back, etc.). Both 1-back and 2-back were programmed using E-prime 3.0 software, which comprised 10 series with 21 letters each. In the 1-back task, participants indicated whether each letter matched the previous one by pressing ‘1’ for ‘yes’ and ‘2’ for ‘no’. In the 2-back task, they determined whether each letter matched the one from two trials ago. The presentation time for each letter was 840 ms, preceded by a 1000 ms fixation cross [30]. Each series contained 30% repeating letters, with no more than three consecutive repetitions. The series duration-matched that of the painful stimulation for synchronized timing.

2.8. Experimental Study Procedure

Subjects that met the inclusion criteria were assessed at the experimental clinical psychology laboratory of Universidad Rey Juan Carlos between October 2023 and March 2024. They fulfilled all the tests about working memory, mental inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and selective attention, which lasted approximately 20 min (Table 1). The study protocol for each individual session lasted 90 min and was conducted by an experienced clinical neuropsychologist.
First, a pain calibration task that aimed to experimentally induce moderate pain in each subject was conducted using a thermal stimulus. Moderate pain was defined as a score of 70 out of 100 on a computerized visual analog pain scale (CoVAS) from 0 to 100 mm [41,42]. The thermal stimulus was applied with a Thermotest System (Somedic AB®, Sweden). A 30 × 30 mm thermode was positioned on the non-dominant forearm, 15 cm below the wrist [38]. Participants received heat thermal stimuli using a “ramp and hold” procedure. Each stimulus began at 32 °C for 3 s; it was increased by 0.7 °C until the temperature-induced moderate pain (70/100 on the CoVAS). They were instructed to press the stop button when that level of pain (70/100 on the CoVAS) was induced. Afterward, the temperature decreased progressively until returning to 32 °C. A 30 s rest period between stimuli was used. The pain calibration task ended when three consecutive pain intensity scores were ≥70/100 mm on the CoVAS. The thermal stimulus was calculated as the arithmetic mean of the three trials. In the current study, the mean temperature of the thermal stimulus needed for inducing moderate pain in the total sample was 46.7 (SD: 1.2) °C.
Second, participants learned to perform distraction tasks, including the 1-back and 2-back tasks, by engaging complex executive functions, like working memory [37]. They completed a brief three-series training, which involved receiving visual feedback and aiming for at least 80% accuracy. Afterward, the participants underwent two sets of ten painful stimuli each, identical to those used in the pain calibration task and lasting approximately 30 s each. While receiving the first set of stimuli, participants completed 10 trials of the 1-back task, and during the second set, they also completed 10 trials of the 2-back task. After each pain stimulus, the participants rated their perceived pain intensity using the CoVAS scale from 0 to 100. The pain intensity during each distraction task was calculated as the mean of the provided intensities during the distraction task. The order of application of the distraction tasks was randomized to avoid sequential or accumulative effects.

2.9. Statistical Analysis

SPSS version 27 Statistical Software was used to conduct the statistical analyses, where the results were considered significant at the level p < 0.05. As in a prior study [43], the presence of outliers was analyzed using boxplots: those values below the first quartile minus 1.5 × the interquartile range and those values above the third quartile plus 1.5 × the interquartile range were considered as potential outliers (no outliers were identified in the current study). The normality of the data was tested using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (only data from the dependent variables and D2_TOT, response inhibition, mental flexibility, and working memory had a normal distribution).
Subsequently, descriptive and frequency analyses were undertaken for the sociodemographic, psychological, and cognitive variables. First, Spearman’s correlation analyses were conducted for the sociodemographic, psychological, neurocognitive, and dependent variables with the aim of determining potential covariates to include in the subsequent analyses. Significant correlations between the dependent variables and age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety were found. Therefore, these variables were incorporated as covariates in the subsequent analyses.
Second, to analyze the main hypothesis of this study, four repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) were performed to assess the effect of functioning on each neurocognitive test (low, medium, and high levels) on the perceived pain intensity at the baseline and during the 1-back and 2-back distractor tasks. Hence, the perceived pain intensity (baseline, 1-back, and 2-back) was the within-subject factor, and the level of functioning on each neurocognitive test was the independent variable. Age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety were included as covariates. The D2_TOT (selective attention, D2 test), mental inhibition and mental flexibility (Five Digit Test), and working memory (D/R/I Digits) indices were used in the main analyses. In accordance with the interpretation rules of the test manual for each instrument, the centile score of each of these indices was calculated, and this score was subsequently classified into low, medium, and high levels. Prior to the execution of the test, the assumptions of normality and sphericity were examined and they were met. The effect sizes were determined using the partial eta squared (n2p). Cohen [21] defines a small effect as 0.01, a medium effect as 0.06, and a large effect for values greater than 0.14. To identify specific intergroup differences, Bonferroni post hoc tests were performed.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Data

The study sample was composed of 59 healthy pain-free subjects (59.3% women, mean age: 46.5, SD: 24.7 years). The sociodemographic, psychological, and neurocognitive data of the sample can be observed in Table 2.

3.2. Preliminary Analysis: Spearman’s Correlation Analyses

The Spearman’s correlation analyses revealed that the perceived pain intensity during the one-back task was negatively associated with age (r = −0.263, p = 0.044), marital status (r = −0.308, p = 0.018), and employment status (r = −0.289, p = 0.026), and positively related to pain anxiety (r = 0.335, p = 0.010). Furthermore, the perceived pain intensity during the two-back task was also negatively associated with age (r = −0.281, p = 0.031), marital status (r = −0.388, p = 0.002), and employment status (r = −0.353, p = 0.006), and positively associated with pain anxiety (r = 0.352, p = 0.006).

3.3. Effect of Working Memory on Pain Analgesia

The repeated-measures ANOVA that analyzed the effect of working memory on perceived pain intensity during the one-back and two-back tasks (estimated marginal means and standard deviations are shown in Table 3) found a main effect of the distractor task (Wilk’s λ = 0.432, F [2, 51] = 33.556, p < 0.001, n2p = 0.568, β-1 = 0.999). Post hoc analyses indicated that the perceived pain intensity scores were lower during the one-back task (mean difference: 26.0, SD: 3.8, 95% CI 16.6 to 35.4, p < 0.001) and in the two-back task (mean difference: 29.5, SD: 3.5, 95% CI 20.6 to 38.3, p < 0.001) as compared with the baseline. No significant differences in the perceived pain intensity scores between the one-back and two-back tasks (mean difference: 3.4, SD: 2.0, 95% CI −1.5 to 8.5, p = 0.283) were found. Additionally, no significant interaction effect between the distractor task and the level of functioning in working memory (Wilk’s λ = 0.942, F [4, 102] = 0.772, p = 0.546, n2p = 0.029, β-1 = 0.240) was found after controlling for age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety.

3.4. Effects of Mental Inhibition on Pain Analgesia

The repeated-measures ANOVA that analyzed the effect of mental inhibition on the perceived pain intensity during the one-back and two-back tasks (Table 3) showed a significant main effect of the distractor task (Wilk’s λ = 0.511, F [2, 51] = 24.432, p < 0.001, n2p = 0.489, β-1 = 0.999). Pairwise post hoc analyses indicated that the perceived pain intensity scores were lower in both the one-back (mean difference: 33.5, SD: 5.8, 95% CI 19.06 to 47.8, p < 0.001) and two-back (mean difference: 37.7, SD: 5.3, 95% CI 24.5 to 51.0, p < 0.001) tasks as compared with the baseline. No significant difference in the perceived pain intensity scores between the one-back and two-back tasks (mean difference: 4.2, SD: 3.2, 95% CI ranging from −3.8 to 12.2, and a p = 0.594) was found. Furthermore, no significant interaction effect between the distractor task and the level of functioning in mental inhibition (Wilk’s λ = 0.880, F [4, 102] = 1.683, p = 0.160, n2p = 0.062, β-1 = 0.501) was seen after controlling for age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety.

3.5. Effects of Cognitive Flexibility on Pain Analgesia

The estimated marginal means and standard deviations reflecting the effect of cognitive flexibility on perceived pain intensity during the one-back and two-back tasks are presented in Table 3. The results show a main effect of the distractor task (Wilk’s λ = 0.500, F [2, 51] = 25.459, p = 0.001, n2p = 0.500, β-1 = 0.999). Post hoc analyses indicated that the perceived pain intensity scores were lower in the one-back task (mean difference: 27.2, SD: 5.6, 95% CI 13.2 to 41.2, p < 0.001) and in the two-back task (mean difference: 34.4, SD: 4.9, 95% CI 22.1 to 46.7, p < 0.001) as compared with the baseline. No significant difference in the perceived pain intensity scores was found (mean difference: 7.2, SD: 2.9, 95% CI ranging from −0.1 to 14.5, and a p = 0.058) between the one-back and two-back tasks. The data did not reveal a significant interaction effect between the distractor task and the level of functioning in cognitive flexibility (Wilk’s λ = 0.809, F [4, 102] = 2.845, p = 0.069, n2p = 0.074, β-1 = 0.756) after controlling for age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety.

3.6. Effects of Attention Levels on Pain Analgesia

Estimated marginal means and standard deviations reflecting the effect of selective attention on perceived pain intensity during the one-back and two-back tasks are shown in Table 3. The ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the distractor task (Wilk’s λ = 0.422, F [2, 51] = 34.875, p < 0.001, n2p = 0.578, β-1 = 0.999). The pairwise post hoc analyses indicated that the perceived pain intensity scores were lower both in the one-back task (mean difference: 28.6, SD: 4.3, 95% CI 17.9 to 39.3, p < 0.001) and in the two-back task (mean difference: 33.1, SD: 3.9, 95% CI 23.3 to 42.8, p < 0.001) as compared with the baseline. No significant difference in the perceived pain intensity scores was found between the one-back and two-back tasks (mean difference: 4.4, SD: 2.3, 95% CI −1.2 to 10.2, p = 0.177). No significant interaction effect between the distractor task and the level of functioning in attention level (Wilk’s λ = 0.862, F [4, 102] = 1.965, p = 0.105, n2p = 0.072, β-1 = 0.573) after controlling for age, marital status, employment status, and pain anxiety was observed.

4. Discussion

This study found that a distraction task decreased the perceived intensity of experimentally induced pain and that this effect was not related to executive functioning, attention levels, and psychological variables in a sample of asymptomatic pain-free individuals. These results did not support the initial hypothesis of this study since a higher level of executive functioning was not associated with a more analgesic effect of a cognitive distraction task.

4.1. Induced Pain Analgesia and Distraction Task

Distraction tasks are cognitive strategies used to reduce pain based on the premise that diverting attention from the pain source can reduce the pain experience; however, their effect seems to depend on the modality (e.g., visual, tactile, auditive) of the distraction [36]. In the current study, we investigated the effect of visual distraction tasks (n-back) on the experimentally induced acute pain. So far, some studies investigated the impact of various visual distractor tests on acute [35,44,45,46,47] and chronic [48] pain. Consistent with our findings, numerous authors exhibited that visual distraction is related to a lower level of reported acute pain [35,44,45,46]. For instance, Bantick et al. [35] examined the effects of cognitive demand on pain perception using a modified Stroop task, which is known as the Stroop counting task, with two levels of difficulty (high and low; counterbalanced). In this study, eight pain-free healthy adults performed this task while experiencing experimental acute pain induced by a heat pulse stimulus and showed that the participants reported a lower pain intensity when engaged in the higher cognitive demand version of the task [35]. However, Stancak et al. found different results when comparing two counterbalanced distraction conditions [47]. In their study, 24 pain-free healthy subjects were instructed to count the figures in the Rubin vase optical illusion (distraction task) while experiencing experimentally induced acute pain from laser pulses on one hand; while in the focusing condition, they were asked to concentrate on the painful sensation. The authors found that the intensity of the reported pain did not differ between both conditions, which contrasted with current the findings [47].
Overall, distraction mechanisms for acute pain can be understood through cognitive, learning, and neurobiological perspectives [49]. Cognitive theories, such as the limited attentional capacity and multiple resource theories, suggest that distraction tasks reduce the attention to pain by utilizing attentional resources, with more effective distractions competing for the same resources used to process pain [50,51]. In this regard, the neurocognitive model indicates that distraction tasks work by modulating involuntary pain attention through a voluntary focus on specific stimuli, though it is less effective when the pain is highly salient, unless goal-directed motivation is present [49,52]. Moreover, learning processes based on behavioral theory propose that distraction prevents the formation of a conditioned fear response to pain by diverting the focus away from painful stimuli and fostering relaxation [50]. On the other hand, neurobiological evidence from neuroimaging shows that distraction alters brain activation by decreasing activity in pain-processing areas, like the thalamus and somatosensory cortices, and by increasing it in areas associated with pain modulation, such as the periaqueductal gray and cingula and frontal cortices [49,53,54]. A more recent study identified that brain responses during pain analgesia, i.e., enhanced activity at the level of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the posterior parietal cortex, is the same independent of the nature of the distraction task [55].
It is important to note that the magnitude of the analgesic response of distraction tasks also varies across published studies [14], where some authors have reported significant effects [37,40], others have reported small effects [56], and others have reported no effect at all [57]. It seems that the effectiveness of different types of distractions (e.g., cognitive, visual, auditory); individual differences in cognitive abilities; the type, intensity, and duration of pain; the study environment; and smaller sample sizes could all potentially explain these discrepancies.
In fact, the magnitude of the effect of the distraction task on pain analgesia can be influenced by several factors. Probably one of the most important factors is the population investigated. Thus, most published studies included clinical populations with acute or chronic pain conditions [36]. Evidence supports the presence of sensitization mechanisms, particularly in chronic pain conditions [58]. Accordingly, it would be expected that the analgesic response of a distraction task would be smaller in individuals with central sensitization. Similarly, albeit to a lesser extent than in chronic pain, acute pain also features signs of sensitization. Hence, the use of acute pain patients may also lead to a smaller effect on pain modulation with a distraction task. In the current study, we used symptomatic pain-free individuals to avoid the presence of signs of sensitization to the analgesic effect of a distraction task on experimentally induced pain.
The analgesic effect of distraction tasks has mainly been attributed to central mechanisms related to a modulation of the activity of sensory and affective brain areas, e.g., by decreasing the activity of the thalamus, insula, primary and secondary somatosensory cortexes, or anterior cingulate cortex, and by increasing the activity of other areas, including the periaqueductal grey substance [54,59]. In such a scenario, according to the neurocognitive model of attention [52], the distraction task would be considered a top-down strategy that is able to decrease the attention on the painful stimuli (bottom-up input). Thus, all top-down processes can be affected, but the extent of attentional effort needed and what is accordingly regarded as goal-relevant information could be affected by other motivational factors, e.g., executive functions or attention levels (see next heading of this discussion).
The role of cognitive function in the analgesic response of a distraction task is supported by the fact that distraction tasks that need higher cognitive demand induce greater pain analgesia than distraction tasks that need less cognitive demand [31]. However, it is important to consider that cognitive fatigue has been recently shown to be able to impair performance on subsequent distraction tasks, which could decrease the individual’s ability to perform the task and to reduce their pain perception [60]. We did not find differences in the analgesia induced by both distraction tasks used in the current study, albeit the two-back task used has a higher cognitive demand than the one-back task. Two potential explanations could explain these results: (1) the execution of both distraction tasks was random between participants, which may have decreased the cognitive fatigue of the subjects, or (2) the difference in cognitive demand between both distractions tasks was not enough to stimulate pain analgesia to a greater extent.

4.2. Distraction Task, Pain Analgesia, and Cognitive Behaviors

It was hypothesized that the effect on pain modulation of distraction tasks could be lower if the individual exhibits high levels of pain catastrophizing or pain-related threat [53,61]. Our results did not observe an effect of either cognitive behaviors (pain catastrophizing, sleep quality) or psychological/emotional (anxiety, depression) aspects in the analgesic effect induced by the distraction tasks used in the current study. The use of a pain-free population, which, as expected, showed low anxiety, depression, and pain-catastrophizing levels, as well as normal sleep quality, could explain the current results. A recent study has observed that the role of pain catastrophizing on pain analgesia depends on the type of distraction task. In this study, pain catastrophizing had a greater role in pain analgesia with a social rather than with a sensory distraction [62]. Nevertheless, this study did not control for other psychological variables, such as anxiety or depressive levels, which could also interact with pain catastrophizing.
To date, conflicting data exist regarding the perception of experimentally induced pain in depressed patients. Some studies indicate that depressed patients exhibit lower pain thresholds [63,64,65], while others suggest higher pain thresholds [66,67,68,69]. In this regard, a meta-analysis of 32 studies (n = 1317) found no significant differences in pain intensity ratings, pain affect, and pain tolerance between individuals with and without depression [70]. Thus, conflicting results also exist regarding anxiety. While some studies have reported that anxiety predicts pain intensity [71,72], others found that anxiety is not a significant predictor [14]. Furthermore, in line with our findings, some studies indicated no correlation between pain catastrophizing and executive function performance [73]. It is possible that maladaptive coping strategies are more related to pain beliefs than to experimentally induced pain perception [73,74]. Nevertheless, it is possible that this lack of effect could be identified with other psychological variables not measured in this study.

4.3. Distraction Task, Pain Analgesia, and Executive Functions

Given that executive function and cognitive abilities, particularly those related to inhibition, can play a role in the pain experience [10,11,15], we aimed to identify the effects of executive functions that were shown to have the highest relevance for a pain experience, such as working memory, mental inhibition, and cognitive flexibility [28,29,30,31], on the analgesic effect of a distraction task. We did not find such an association in a sample of asymptomatic pain-free subjects. In other words, our findings did not observe any significant correlation between various aspects of executive function (cognitive flexibility, mental inhibition, working memory, and selective attention) and experimentally induced pain. Consistent with our results, Verhoeven et al. found that individuals with better executive functioning abilities (inhibition, task switching, and working memory) did not experience greater pain reduction during a distraction task than those with lesser abilities [14]. However, limited studies that investigated the correlation between executive functions and perception in acute pain settings were undertaken. Furthermore, although several studies focused on clinical pain experience and cognition in chronic pain patients, the results were inconsistent, with negative [75,76] and positive [77,78] associations observed between the pain experience and executive functions. In this regard, some studies revealed inconsistent findings between different aspects of executive function and perceived pain. Elkana et al. demonstrated that perceived pain is associated with two aspects of executive functions, such as inhibition and set shifting, but not with updating [73]. In contrast, others found that lower performance on a working memory task is associated with greater intense pain perception [79]. A systematic review indicated that while executive functioning may negatively correlate with responsiveness to experimental pain, this correlation is weak (very small effect sizes) and only 20% of the studies found a significant correlation [80]. Overall, the discrepancies may have been due to various factors, including the tests selected for evaluating various aspects of executive functions, the variety of acute/chronic pain conditions, different methods for inducing pain (such as heat or cold stimulus), and the diverse populations included. Further research is needed to evaluate the role of executive function on pain perception in different pain settings to confirm or refute our findings.

4.4. Limitations of This Study

The results of the current study should be considered within the study design and its potential limitations. First, we included pain-free asymptomatic subjects; accordingly, the current data cannot be extrapolated to clinical populations with pain, where the interaction of pain with the variables assessed in our study is more complex. Second, the study design was based on an experimentally induced acute pain model, and thus, the analgesic effect induced by the distraction tasks should not be applied to a chronic pain setting. Third, we only used a visual distraction task, and thus, we do not currently know the effect of neurocognitive variables and executive functions in pain analgesia induced by other types of tasks. Fourth, although the total sample was calculated for detecting moderate effects, the sample size was small for the comparison of some subgroups; hence, statistical analyses could have included type II errors. Future studies using the same study design and including clinical populations with different pain conditions are needed to confirm or refute the direction of the current findings.

5. Conclusions

The current study revealed that a distraction task (one-back and two-back) reduced the perceived intensity of experimentally induced pain in asymptomatic, pain-free individuals. This effect was neither associated with various aspects of executive function, such as working memory, mental inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and selective attention, nor with attention levels. Future studies in clinical pain populations with chronic pain conditions should be conducted to determine the analgesic response with the distraction tasks used in the current study in a clinical setting.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; methodology, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; validation, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; formal analysis, C.F.-d.-l.-P., F.G.F.-P. and J.C.P.-H.; investigation, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; resources, C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; data curation, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; writing—review and editing, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; visualization, A.T.-A., F.G.F.-P., J.C.P.-H., A.N., A.I.d.-l.-L.-R., S.A.-Q., R.O.-S., C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; supervision, C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M.; project administration, C.F.-d.-l.-P. and M.C.-M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Decla-ration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Universidad Rey Juan Carlos (internal record ID: URJC 1909202332123).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all the participants.

Data Availability Statement

Materials and analysis code for this study are not available in any repository; however, we will make our data accessible upon request to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Kohl, A.; Rief, W.; Glombiewski, J.A. Acceptance, Cognitive Restructuring, and Distraction as Coping Strategies for Acute Pain. J. Pain 2013, 14, 305–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Raja, S.N.; Carr, D.B.; Cohen, M.; Finnerup, N.B.; Flor, H.; Gibson, S.; Keefe, F.J.; Mogil, J.S.; Ringkamp, M.; Sluka, K.A.; et al. The Revised International Association for the Study of Pain Definition of Pain: Concepts, Challenges, and Compromises. Pain 2020, 161, 1976–1982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Turk, D.C.; Rudy, T.E. Cognitive Factors and Persistent Pain: A Glimpse into Pandora’s Box. Cogn. Ther. Res. 1992, 16, 99–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. López-López, A.; Gutierrez, J.L.G.; Hernández, J.C.P.; Matías-Pompa, B.; Peña, I.J.M. Effectiveness of Spontaneous Pain Coping Strategies for Acute Pain Management: A Laboratory Study. Scand. J. Psychol. 2023, 64, 294–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Melzack, R. Evolution of the Neuromatrix Theory of Pain. The Prithvi Raj Lecture: Presented at the Third World Congress of World Institute of Pain, Barcelona 2004. Pain Pract. 2005, 5, 85–94. [Google Scholar]
  6. Apkarian, A.V.; Bushnell, M.C.; Treede, R.; Zubieta, J. Human Brain Mechanisms of Pain Perception and Regulation in Health and Disease. Eur. J. Pain 2005, 9, 463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Eccleston, C. The Attentional Control of Pain: Methodological and Theoretical Concerns. Pain 1995, 63, 3–10. [Google Scholar]
  8. Abeare, C.A.; Cohen, J.L.; Axelrod, B.N.; Leisen, J.C.C.; Mosley-Williams, A.; Lumley, M.A. Pain, Executive Functioning, and Affect in Patients with Rheumatoid Arthritis. Clin. J. Pain 2010, 26, 683–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Karsdorp, P.A.; Geenen, R.; Vlaeyen, J.W.S. Response Inhibition Predicts Painful Task Duration and Performance in Healthy Individuals Performing a Cold Pressor Task in a Motivational Context. Eur. J. Pain 2014, 18, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Oosterman, J.M.; Dijkerman, H.C.; Kessels, R.P.C.; Scherder, E.J.A. A Unique Association between Cognitive Inhibition and Pain Sensitivity in Healthy Participants. Eur. J. Pain 2010, 14, 1046–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Schreiber, K.L.; Campbell, C.; Martel, M.O.; Greenbaum, S.; Wasan, A.D.; Borsook, D.; Jamison, R.N.; Edwards, R.R. Distraction Analgesia in Chronic Pain Patients. Anesthesiology 2014, 121, 1292–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Elomaa, M.M.; de Williams, A.C.; Kalso, E.A. Attention Management as a Treatment for Chronic Pain. Eur. J. Pain 2009, 13, 1062–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Verhoeven, K.; Van Damme, S.; Eccleston, C.; Van Ryckeghem, D.M.L.; Legrain, V.; Crombez, G. Distraction from Pain and Executive Functioning: An Experimental Investigation of the Role of Inhibition, Task Switching and Working Memory. Eur. J. Pain 2011, 15, 866–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Oosterman, J.M.; Traxler, J.; Kunz, M. The Influence of Executive Functioning on Facial and Subjective Pain Responses in Older Adults. Behav. Neurol. 2016, 2016, 1984827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Verhoeven, K.; Dick, B.; Eccleston, C.; Goubert, L.; Crombez, G. The Role of Executive Functioning in Children’s Attentional Pain Control: An Experimental Analysis. Pain 2014, 155, 413–421. [Google Scholar]
  17. Mansfield, M.; Roviello, G.; Thacker, M.; Willett, M.; Bannister, K.; Smith, T. The Association between Conditioned Pain Modulation and Psychological Factors in People with Chronic Spinal Pain: A Systematic Review. Br. J. Pain 2024, 18, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Herrero Babiloni, A.; Brazeau, D.; Jodoin, M.; Theis-Mahon, N.; Martel, M.O.; Lavigne, G.J.; Moana-Filho, E.J. The Impact of Sleep Disturbances on Endogenous Pain Modulation: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Pain 2024, 25, 875–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Rischer, K.M.; Anton, F.; González-Roldán, A.M.; Montoya, P.; van der Meulen, M. Better Executive Functions Are Associated With More Efficient Cognitive Pain Modulation in Older Adults: An FMRI Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2022, 14, 828742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lobo, A.; Ezquerra, J.; Bugarda, F.G.; Sala, J.M.; Seva, A. El Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo. Un Test Sencillo y Practico Para Detectar Alteraciones Intelectuales en Pacientes Medicos. Actas Luso-Esp. Neurol. Psiquiatr. 1979, 7, 189–202. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge: London, UK, 2013; ISBN 9781134742707. [Google Scholar]
  22. Zigmond, A.S.; Snaith, R.P. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983, 67, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Vallejo, M.A.; Rivera, J.; Esteve-Vives, J.; Rodríguez-Muñoz, M.F. Uso Del Cuestionario Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) Para Evaluar La Ansiedad y La Depresión En Pacientes Con Fibromialgia. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 2012, 5, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sullivan, M.J.L.; Bishop, S.R.; Pivik, J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and Validation. Psychol. Assess. 1995, 7, 524–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. García-Campayo, J.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; Cabellero, L.; Romera, I.; Aragonés, E.; Rodríguez-Artalejo, F.; Gilaberte, I.; Quail, D. Relationship of Somatic Symptoms with Depression Severity, Quality of Life, and Health Resources Utilization in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder Seeking Primary Health Care in Spain. Prim. Care Companion J. Clin. Psychiatry 2008, 10, 355–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. McCracken, L.M.; Dhingra, L. A Short Version of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20): Preliminary Development and Validity. Pain Res. Manag. 2002, 7, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F.; Monk, T.H.; Berman, S.R.; Kupfer, D.J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A New Instrument for Psychiatric Practice and Research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28, 193–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bjekić, J.; Živanović, M.; Purić, D.; Oosterman, J.M.; Filipović, S.R. Pain and Executive Functions: A Unique Relationship between Stroop Task and Experimentally Induced Pain. Psychol. Res. 2018, 82, 580–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dahlquist, L.M.; Gaultney, W.M.; Bento, S.P.; Steiner, E.M.; Zeroth, J.A.; Parr, N.J.; Quiton, R.L. Working Memory and Visual Discrimination Distraction Tasks Improve Cold Pressor Pain Tolerance in Children. Health Psychol. 2020, 39, 10–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gaultney, W.M.; Dahlquist, L.M.; Quiton, R.L. Cognitive Load and the Effectiveness of Distraction for Acute Pain in Children. Eur. J. Pain 2021, 25, 1568–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rischer, K.M.; González-Roldán, A.M.; Montoya, P.; Gigl, S.; Anton, F.; van der Meulen, M. Distraction from Pain: The Role of Selective Attention and Pain Catastrophizing. Eur. J. Pain 2020, 24, 1880–1891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Juan Antonio Amador. Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler Para Adultos-IV (WAIS-IV); Universidad de Barcelona: Barcelona, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  33. Sedó, M.A. Test de Los Cinco Dígitos (FDT); TEA: Madrid, Spain, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  34. Moriarty, O.; McGuire, B.E.; Finn, D.P. The Effect of Pain on Cognitive Function: A Review of Clinical and Preclinical Research. Prog. Neurobiol. 2011, 93, 385–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Bantick, S.J.; Wise, R.G.; Ploghaus, A.; Clare, S.; Smith, S.M.; Tracey, I. Imaging How Attention Modulates Pain in Humans Using Functional MRI. Brain 2002, 125, 310–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Bascour-Sandoval, C.; Salgado-Salgado, S.; Gómez-Milán, E.; Fernández-Gómez, J.; Michael, G.A.; Gálvez-García, G. Pain and Distraction According to Sensory Modalities: Current Findings and Future Directions. Pain Pract. 2019, 19, 686–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Buhle, J.; Wager, T.D. Performance-Dependent Inhibition of Pain by an Executive Working Memory Task. Pain 2010, 149, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Brickenkamp, R.C.N. Test de Atención D2; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  39. Seisdedos, N. Test de Atención D2; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  40. Buhle, J.T.; Stevens, B.L.; Friedman, J.J.; Wager, T.D. Distraction and Placebo. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 246–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Jensen, M.; Chen, C.; Brugger, A.M. Interpretation of Visual Analog Scale Ratings and Change Scores: A Reanalysis of Two Clinical Trials of Postoperative Pain. J. Pain 2003, 4, 407–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Boonstra, A.M.; Schiphorst Preuper, H.R.; Balk, G.A.; Stewart, R.E. Cut-off Points for Mild, Moderate, and Severe Pain on the Visual Analogue Scale for Pain in Patients with Chronic Musculoskeletal Pain. Pain 2014, 155, 2545–2550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Pacho-Hernández, J.C.; González-Gutiérrez, J.L.; Yunta-Rua, L.; Pocinho, R.; López-López, A. Effectiveness of Cognitive Reappraisal and Distraction for Induced Acute Pain: A Laboratory Study. Health Psychol. 2024, 43, 500–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Chayadi, E.; McConnell, B.L. Gaining Insights on the Influence of Attention, Anxiety, and Anticipation on Pain Perception. J. Pain Res. 2019, 12, 851–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hylands-White, N.; Derbyshire, S.W. Modifying Pain Perception: Is It Better to Be Hypnotizable or Feel That You Are Hypnotized? Contemp. Hypn. 2007, 24, 143–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Liu, C.-C.; Ohara, S.; Franaszczuk, P.J.; Crone, N.E.; Lenz, F.A. Attention to Painful Cutaneous Laser Stimuli Evokes Directed Functional Interactions between Human Sensory and Modulatory Pain-Related Cortical Areas. Pain 2011, 152, 2781–2791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Stancak, A.; Fallon, N.; Fenu, A.; Kokmotou, K.; Soto, V.; Cook, S. Neural Mechanisms of Attentional Switching Between Pain and a Visual Illusion Task: A Laser Evoked Potential Study. Brain Topogr. 2018, 31, 430–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Stankewitz, A.; Sorg, C.; von Kalckreuth, A.; Schulz, E.; Valet, M.; Neufang, S.; Zimmer, C.; Henningsen, P.; Gündel, H.; Wohlschläger, A.M.; et al. Fronto-Insular Connectivity during Pain Distraction Is Impaired in Patients with Somatoform Pain. J. Neuroimaging 2018, 28, 621–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Birnie, K.A.; Chambers, C.T.; Spellman, C.M. Mechanisms of Distraction in Acute Pain Perception and Modulation. Pain 2017, 158, 1012–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cohen, L.L.; Cousins, L.A.; Martin, S.R. Procedural Pain Distraction. In Oxford Textbook of Paediatric Pain; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 553–559. [Google Scholar]
  51. Johnson, M.H. How Does Distraction Work in the Management of Pain? Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 2005, 9, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Legrain, V.; Van Damme, S.; Eccleston, C.; Davis, K.D.; Seminowicz, D.A.; Crombez, G. A Neurocognitive Model of Attention to Pain: Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence. Pain 2009, 144, 230–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jackson, T.; Yang, Z.; Li, X.; Chen, H.; Huang, X.; Meng, J. Coping When Pain Is a Potential Threat: The Efficacy of Acceptance versus Cognitive Distraction. Eur. J. Pain 2012, 16, 390–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Valet, M.; Sprenger, T.; Boecker, H.; Willoch, F.; Rummeny, E.; Conrad, B.; Erhard, P.; Tolle, T.R. Distraction Modulates Connectivity of the Cingulo-Frontal Cortex and the Midbrain during Pain—An FMRI Analysis. Pain 2004, 109, 399–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Silvestrini, N.; Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C. Distraction and Cognitive Control Independently Impact Parietal and Prefrontal Response to Pain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2023, 18, nsad018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. McCaul, K.D.; Monson, N.; Maki, R.H. Does Distraction Reduce Pain-Produced Distress among College Students? Health Psychol. 1992, 11, 210–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Goubert, L.; Crombez, G.; Eccleston, C.; Devulder, J. Distraction from Chronic Pain during a Pain-Inducing Activity Is Associated with Greater Post-Activity Pain. Pain 2004, 110, 220–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Nijs, J.; George, S.Z.; Clauw, D.J.; Fernández-de-las-Peñas, C.; Kosek, E.; Ickmans, K.; Fernández-Carnero, J.; Polli, A.; Kapreli, E.; Huysmans, E.; et al. Central Sensitisation in Chronic Pain Conditions: Latest Discoveries and Their Potential for Precision Medicine. Lancet Rheumatol. 2021, 3, e383–e392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Frankenstein, U.N.; Richter, W.; McIntyre, M.C.; Rémy, F. Distraction Modulates Anterior Cingulate Gyrus Activations during the Cold Pressor Test. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 827–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Vogel, T.A.; Desjardins, S.; Chabot, B.; Otto, A.R.; Falk, C.F.; Roy, M. Cognitive Fatigue Weakens the Effects of Distraction on Pain. Pain 2023, 164, 2845–2851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Verhoeven, K.; Goubert, L.; Jaaniste, T.; Van Ryckeghem, D.M.L.; Crombez, G. Pain Catastrophizing Influences the Use and the Effectiveness of Distraction in Schoolchildren. Eur. J. Pain 2012, 16, 256–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Asefi Rad, A.; Wippert, P.-M. Insights into Pain Distraction and the Impact of Pain Catastrophizing on Pain Perception during Different Types of Distraction Tasks. Front. Pain Res. 2024, 5, 1266974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hermesdorf, M.; Berger, K.; Baune, B.T.; Wellmann, J.; Ruscheweyh, R.; Wersching, H. Pain Sensitivity in Patients with Major Depression: Differential Effect of Pain Sensitivity Measures, Somatic Cofactors, and Disease Characteristics. J. Pain 2016, 17, 606–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Zambito Marsala, S.; Pistacchi, M.; Tocco, P.; Gioulis, M.; Fabris, F.; Brigo, F.; Tinazzi, M. Pain Perception in Major Depressive Disorder: A Neurophysiological Case–Control Study. J. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 357, 19–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Nitzan, U.; Hecht, M.; Braw, Y.; Maoz, H.; Levkovitz, Y.; Yarnitsky, D.; Granovsky, Y.; Bloch, Y. Initial Evaluation of Pain Intensity Among Depressed Patients as a Possible Mediator Between Depression and Pain Complaints. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Bär, K.-J.; Brehm, S.; Boettger, M.K.; Boettger, S.; Wagner, G.; Sauer, H. Pain Perception in Major Depression Depends on Pain Modality. Pain 2005, 117, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Dickens, C.; McGowan, L.; Dale, S. Impact of Depression on Experimental Pain Perception: A Systematic Review of the Literature with Meta-Analysis. Psychosom. Med. 2003, 65, 369–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Rodriguez-Raecke, R.; Ihle, K.; Ritter, C.; Muhtz, C.; Otte, C.; May, A. Neuronal Differences between Chronic Low Back Pain and Depression Regarding Long-term Habituation to Pain. Eur. J. Pain 2014, 18, 701–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Schwier, C.; Kliem, A.; Boettger, M.K.; Bär, K.-J. Increased Cold-Pain Thresholds in Major Depression. J. Pain 2010, 11, 287–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Thompson, T.; Correll, C.U.; Gallop, K.; Vancampfort, D.; Stubbs, B. Is Pain Perception Altered in People with Depression? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Experimental Pain Research. J. Pain 2016, 17, 1257–1272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Granot, M.; Ferber, S.G. The Roles of Pain Catastrophizing and Anxiety in the Prediction of Postoperative Pain Intensity. Clin. J. Pain 2005, 21, 439–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Meredith, P.; Strong, J.; Feeney, J.A. Adult Attachment, Anxiety, and Pain Self-Efficacy as Predictors of Pain Intensity and Disability. Pain 2006, 123, 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Elkana, O.; Conti, Y.; Heyman, O.; Hamdan, S.; Franko, M.; Vatine, J.-J. The Associations between Executive Functions and Different Aspects of Perceived Pain, beyond the Influence of Depression, in Rehabilitation Setting. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2020, 30, 1303–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Monticone, M.; Ferrante, S.; Ferrari, S.; Foti, C.; Mugnai, R.; Pillastrini, P.; Rocca, B.; Vanti, C. The Italian Version of the Pain Beliefs and Perceptions Inventory: Cross-Cultural Adaptation, Factor Analysis, Reliability and Validity. Qual. Life Res. 2014, 23, 1789–1795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Park, R.; Wallace, M.S.; Schulteis, G. Relative Sensitivity to Alfentanil and Reliability of Current Perception Threshold vs. von Frey Tactile Stimulation and Thermal Sensory Testing. J. Peripher. Nerv. Syst. 2001, 6, 232–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Weiner, D.K.; Rudy, T.E.; Morrow, L.; Slaboda, J.; Lieber, S. The Relationship between Pain, Neuropsychological Performance, and Physical Function in Community-Dwelling Older Adults with Chronic Low Back Pain. Pain Med. 2006, 7, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Oosterman, J.M.; de Vries, K.; Dijkerman, H.C.; de Haan, E.H.F.; Scherder, E.J.A. Exploring the Relationship between Cognition and Self-Reported Pain in Residents of Homes for the Elderly. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2009, 21, 157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Scherder, E.J.A.; Eggermont, L.; Plooij, B.; Oudshoorn, J.; Vuijk, P.J.; Pickering, G.; Lautenbacher, S.; Achterberg, W.; Oosterman, J. Relationship between Chronic Pain and Cognition in Cognitively Intact Older Persons and in Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease. Gerontology 2008, 54, 50–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Garcia, S.; Foster, E.; Johnson, P.J.; Thomas, B.; Askew, R.L. Executive Function as a Predictor of Pain Perception in Healthy Young Adults. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2024, 39, 766–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Bunk, S.; Preis, L.; Zuidema, S.; Lautenbacher, S.; Kunz, M. Executive Functions and Pain. Z. Für Neuropsychol. 2019, 30, 169–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Cognitive domains and neuropsychological tasks.
Table 1. Cognitive domains and neuropsychological tasks.
Cognitive DomainsNeuropsychological TestsOutcomesMethod of Administration
Cognitive screeningMMSECognitive statusAuditory/visual/manual (paper)
Working memoryDigit Span Forward (WAIS-IV)Span of digitsAuditory/oral
Digit Span Backward (WAIS-IV)Auditory working memory
Digit Span Sequencing (WAIS-IV)Auditory working memory
Selective attentionD2 Test of AttentionD2_TRVisual/manual (paper)
D2_TA
D2_TOT
D2_CON
D2_VAR
D2_O
D2_C
Mental inhibition and cognitive flexibilityFive Digits Test FDTInhibiting_FDTVisual/oral
Shifting_FDT
Decoding_FDT
Retrieving_FDT
MMSE: Mini-Mental Scale Examination; WAIS-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults-IV; D2_TR: total number of items answered; D2_TA: number of items answered correctly; D2_O: errors of omission committed; D2_C: commission errors made; D2_TOT: number of elements processed minus the total number of errors committed; D2_CON: number of relevant elements marked minus the number of commissions; D2_VAR: variation index; DSF: Digit Span Forward; DSB: Digit Span Backward; DSS: Digit Span Sequencing; Decoding_FDT: time in seconds to read all numeric items; Retrieving_FDT: time in seconds to read all non-numeric items; Inhibiting_FDT: time in seconds to read numeric items; Shifting_FDT: time in seconds to read non-numeric items.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the sociodemographic, psychological, and neurocognitive data.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the sociodemographic, psychological, and neurocognitive data.
DataMean (Standard Deviation)
Age (mean, SD)46.4 (24.7)
Gender (n, %)
Male24 (40.7%)
Female35 (59.3%)
Marital status (n, %)
Single26 (44.1%)
Married25 (42.4%)
Divorced4 (6.8%)
Widowed4 (6.8%)
Educational level (n, %)
Primary school1 (1.7%)
Middle school44 (74.6%)
High school14 (23.7%)
Employment status (n, %)
Student25 (42.4%)
Working5 (8.5%)
Unemployed1 (1.7%)
Retired28 (47.5%)
Psychological data
Anxiety (HADS-A, 0–21; mean, SD)6.5 (3.2)
Depression (HADS-D, 0–21; mean, SD)3.1 (2.4)
Pain catastrophizing (PCS, 0–52; mean, SD)15.8 (9.0)
Pain anxiety (PASS-20, 0–100; mean, SD)27. 2 (12.0)
Sleep quality (PSQI, 0–21; mean, SD)7.4 (3.4)
Neurocognitive data
Mental inhibition (Inhibiting_FDT; mean, SD)17.1 (7.1)
Cognitive flexibility (Shifting_FDT; mean, SD)28.2 (10.9)
Selective attention (D2_TOT; mean, SD)392.3 (91.4)
Working memory (D/R/I Digits; mean, SD)23.9 (5.5)
n: the number of subjects; SD: standard deviation; HADS-A: anxiety dimension of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HADS-D: depression dimension of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; PCS: Pain Catastrophizing Scale; PASS-20: Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale-20; PSQI: The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; FDT: Five Digits Test; D2: D2 Test of Attention; D/R/I Digits: Working memory subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale battery.
Table 3. Estimated marginal means and standard deviations of the repeated-measures ANOVA for each neurocognitive domain.
Table 3. Estimated marginal means and standard deviations of the repeated-measures ANOVA for each neurocognitive domain.
Working Memory
Low (n = 14)Medium (n = 32)High (n = 13)Univariate tests
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Fpn2pβ-1
Baseline pain intensity 70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0) 1.0
Pain intensity 1-back task40.6 (7.5)50.7 (4.7)40.5 (7.9)0.9890.3790.0370.213
Pain intensity 2-back task36.1 (7.0)44.2 (4.5)41.0 (7.4)0.4760.6240.0180.124
Attention
Low (n = 9)Medium (n = 40)High (n = 10)Univariate tests
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Fpn2pβ-1
Baseline pain intensity70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0) 1.0
Pain intensity 1-back task34.5 (8.9)50.4 (4.2)39.1 (8.5)1.6330.2050.0590.330
Pain intensity 2-back task24.3 (8.1)45.8 (3.8)40.4 (7.7)2.7720.0720.0960.523
Mental Inhibition
Low (n = 12)Medium (n = 44)High (n = 3)Univariate tests
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Fpn2pβ-1
Baseline pain intensity70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0) 1.0
Pain intensity 1-back task35.9 (8.6)50. 4 (4.0)23.0 (15.2)2.3340.1070.0820.452
Pain intensity 2-back task28.0 (7.8)46.6 (3.7)21.9 (13.9)3.2260.0480.1100.591
Cognitive Flexibility
Low (n = 8)Medium (n = 47)High (n = 4)Univariate tests
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Fpn2pβ-1
Baseline pain intensity70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0)70.0 (0.0) 1.0
Pain intensity 1-back task31.2 (10.4)48.3 (3.9)48.7 (14.2)1.1580.3220.0430.243
Pain intensity 2-back task16.4 (9.1)45.6 (3.4)44.5 (12.5)4.3520.0180.1430.730
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tejera-Alonso, A.; Fernández-Palacios, F.G.; Pacho-Hernández, J.C.; Naeimi, A.; de-la-Llave-Rincón, A.I.; Ambite-Quesada, S.; Ortega-Santiago, R.; Fernández-de-las-Peñas, C.; Cigarán-Mendez, M. Effects of Executive Functions and Cognitive Variables in Experimentally Induced Acute Pain Perception during a Distraction Task: A Study on Asymptomatic Pain-Free Individuals. Life 2024, 14, 1141. https://doi.org/10.3390/life14091141

AMA Style

Tejera-Alonso A, Fernández-Palacios FG, Pacho-Hernández JC, Naeimi A, de-la-Llave-Rincón AI, Ambite-Quesada S, Ortega-Santiago R, Fernández-de-las-Peñas C, Cigarán-Mendez M. Effects of Executive Functions and Cognitive Variables in Experimentally Induced Acute Pain Perception during a Distraction Task: A Study on Asymptomatic Pain-Free Individuals. Life. 2024; 14(9):1141. https://doi.org/10.3390/life14091141

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tejera-Alonso, Angela, Francisco G. Fernández-Palacios, Juan C. Pacho-Hernández, Arvin Naeimi, Ana I. de-la-Llave-Rincón, Silvia Ambite-Quesada, Ricardo Ortega-Santiago, César Fernández-de-las-Peñas, and Margarita Cigarán-Mendez. 2024. "Effects of Executive Functions and Cognitive Variables in Experimentally Induced Acute Pain Perception during a Distraction Task: A Study on Asymptomatic Pain-Free Individuals" Life 14, no. 9: 1141. https://doi.org/10.3390/life14091141

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop