Next Article in Journal
Morphogeometric Evaluation of the Left Ventricle and Left Atrioventricular Ring in Dogs: A Computerized Anatomical Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Variations in the Reproductive Strategies of Different Charadrius alexandrinus Populations in Xinjiang, China
Previous Article in Journal
The Impacts of Supplemental Protein during Development on Amino Acid Concentrations in the Uterus and Pregnancy Outcomes of Angus Heifers
Previous Article in Special Issue
Illegal and Exploitative Sand-Digging Activities Could Be Managed to Create Suitable Nesting Habitats for Blue-Tailed Bee-Eaters (Merops philippinus)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Foraging, Fear and Behavioral Variation in a Traplining Hummingbird

by
Katarzyna Wojczulanis-Jakubas
1,* and
Marcelo Araya-Salas
2
1
Department of Vertebrate Ecology and Zoology, University of Gdansk, 80-308 Gdansk, Poland
2
Centro de Investigación en Neurociencias, University of Costa Rica, San José 11501-2060, Costa Rica
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(12), 1997; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13121997
Submission received: 20 May 2023 / Revised: 11 June 2023 / Accepted: 13 June 2023 / Published: 15 June 2023

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Animals differ in their foraging efficiency and it is not clear what drives this variation. We examined how the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermit hummingbirds changes with regard to three behavioral traits: (a) exploration, (b) risk avoidance and (c) arousal in conditions at two different levels of perceived risk (low and high). We found that foraging efficiency was lower in high-risk conditions, but behavioral traits explained the additional variation in foraging efficiency in a condition-dependent manner. More explorative individuals had higher foraging efficiency in low-risk conditions, but the opposite was the case in high-risk conditions. Regardless of the conditions, foraging efficiency increased with bird arousal and decreased if they were more cautious (higher risk avoidance). Our findings highlight the importance of taking into account additional behavioral dimensions to better understand the foraging strategies of individuals.

Abstract

Traditionally, foraging behavior has been explained as the response to a trade-off between energetic gain from feeding resources and potential costs from concomitant risks. However, an increasing number of studies has shown that this view fails to explain an important fraction of the variation in foraging across a variety of taxa. One potential mechanism that may account for this variation is that various behavioral traits associated with foraging may have different fitness consequences, which may depend on the environmental context. Here, we explored this mechanism by evaluating the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermit hummingbirds (Phaethornis longirostris) with regard to three behavioral traits: (a) exploration (number of feeders used during the foraging visit), (b) risk avoidance (latency to start feeding) and (c) arousal (amount of movements during the foraging visit) in conditions at two different levels of perceived risk (low—control and high—experimental, with a threatening bullet ant model). Foraging efficiency decreased in response to threatening conditions. However, behavioral traits explained additional variation in foraging efficiency in a condition-dependent manner. More exploration was associated with a higher foraging efficiency under control conditions, but this was reversed when exposed to a threat. Regardless of the conditions, arousal was positively associated with foraging efficiency, while risk avoidance was negatively related. Importantly, exploratory behavior and risk avoidance were quite repeatable behaviors, suggesting that they may be related to the intrinsic traits of individuals. Our findings highlight the importance of taking into account additional behavioral dimensions to better understand the foraging strategies of individuals.

1. Introduction

A variety of ecological factors have been identified as major determinants in shaping the foraging strategies of animals (i.e., resource exploitation). Of those, the most commonly evoked are the amount and distribution of available food resources [1,2] and animal motivation (both in the sense of the marginal value theorem [3] and/or body condition [4]), but predation pressure is also frequently considered [5,6]. The intensity of prey vigilance increases with the level of risk predation (probability of predator presence), affecting its foraging efficiency. As imposed according to the risk allocation hypothesis, the prey allocates time for foraging inversely proportional to the predation pressure [5,6,7]. Thus, with given food resources or a predation landscape, a fixed foraging strategy is expected to evolve [8]. However, an increasing number of studies demonstrates a high variation in foraging strategies [9,10,11], which is hard to explain using only food- and predation-based arguments [4].
Variation in the foraging strategies of individuals remains an intriguing topic [1,12,13,14,15,16]. Existing evidence demonstrates not only the variation per se, but also the consistent inter-individual differences in the average level of a behavior displayed across a range of contexts [17], and in response to environmental variation [12,16], a species/population and individuals usually represent a full continuum of a given behavioral display, with two polar-opposite phenotypes (e.g., high and low exploratory behavior) and various intermediate forms in between. Importantly, the fitness advantages of contrasting behaviors may differ within various contexts, sometimes dramatically [18]. For example, highly exploratory behavior can be advantageous in conditions of resource competition, but disadvantageous in a higher-predation-pressure environment [18,19,20,21]. Despite the growing number of studies showing high behavioral variability in foraging performance in animals, what could be the fitness consequences of this variation is still not entirely recognized.
The fitness payoff of a behavior under different scenarios is likely to be the main source maintaining the observed variation in foraging strategy [11,18,22]. Under variable conditions, the diversity in behavioral strategies can be maintained if a different performance results in different cost and benefits within different contexts. If so, performance may vary as a function of interactions between social and/or ecological selective forces, which can help reveal the complex interplay of intrinsic and extrinsic factors shaping behavioral variability [23,24,25,26].
Here, we examined the fitness consequences of various behavioral strategies during foraging in wild-ranging hummingbirds. This avian group is known for their extreme metabolism, with a high need for energy intake that makes them constantly motivated to forage [27]. As such, they are expected to be under strong selection for maximizing foraging efficiency, which can have a considerable effect on fitness. This is particularly significant for traplining foraging species, which move around in a route-like fashion across the habitat to visit dispersed flower patches [28,29]. Such a free-ranging strategy entails a period of high vulnerability for foraging individuals, exploited by a wide range of predators [30,31,32,33,34,35], which in turn results in a trade-off between efficient foraging and risk avoidance. The differential payoffs in the selective landscape given by these two factors are expected to shape the foraging strategies of hummingbirds.
In this study, we observed the foraging behavior of the long-billed hermit hummingbird (LBH, Phaethornis longirostris). The species is a relatively large-sized hummingbird of humid neo-tropical lowlands. It exhibits a lekking mating system, in which males sing and display from traditional areas inside the forest for mate attraction during an eight-month breeding season [28]. Unlike many hummingbirds, hermits do not defend a patch of flowers, but visit widely scattered flowers in a foraging route (i.e., traplining). Aggressive interactions related to disputes over lek territories are common [29]. Floaters can be found singing from perches of territorial males, while the latter are absent-foraging. Territorial ownership is typically regained after an aggressive interaction with intruders, suggesting a selective pressure to reduce the foraging time and increase territory attendance. Indeed, more efficient foraging males are more likely to own a lek territory [29].
To examine the payoffs of different behavioral strategies under a trade-off between food resource exploitation and risk avoidance, we considered the issue in the context of a low and high level of perceived threat. We also analyzed the issue in regard to three behavioral axes, commonly linked to the exploitation of food resources: (a) exploration (number of foraging spots (i.e., feeders) used during the foraging visit), (b) risk avoidance (latency to approach the foraging spot to forage) and (c) arousal (amount of movements during the foraging event) [1,18,19,36,37]. We hypothesized that foraging efficiency (expressed as the amount of time spent on feeding in respect to the total time of the visit at the feeder) may be lower under threatening conditions. We also hypothesized that foraging efficiency is further modulated by the three behavioral traits (exploration, risk avoidance and arousal), and that high values of these traits can be expected, which will negatively affect foraging efficiency.

2. Methods

2.1. Fieldwork

We carried out the study at La Selva Biological Station, Costa Rica (10°23′ N, 84°10′ W) between May and June 2015. We captured and marked 21 individuals with foam tags (total weight of 0.02 g, which is ~0.3% of LBH body mass, 6 g) with unique color combinations, attached to the birds’ back and chest with nontoxic eyelash glue [29]. To evaluate the foraging efficiency of marked individuals and quantify it within low- and high-risk context, we conducted a field experiment using hummingbird feeders. Commercial feeders (Perky model Pet #209B, 900 mL) were modified to have a single opening for accessing “nectar”. Three feeders were arranged in a line (separated by ca. 10 cm distances from each other, Figure 1) and filled with fresh nectar (~30% sugar–water). The set-up was located at a distance of ca. 100 m from the lek border. The feeders (filled with nectar/changed daily) were exposed in the field for two weeks before the onset of experiments, to habituate the birds to feeders. All marked birds were observed on the lek after being caught, either defending territory or as floaters (i.e., all of them survived the capturing and marking procedure, and behaved normally at the lek area). The location of the feeders did not overlap with the foraging area of all the captured individuals; however, for the total, we had 12 visitors at the feeder area.
The experiment session consisted of two consecutive phases performed on the same day, within low- (first phase, control) and high-risk contexts (second phase, experimental). The birds were allowed to forage on the nectar spontaneously, and their behavior was recorded with a commercial camera (continuous recording mode; model: Fujifilm HS30); the camera was set up on a tripod at a distance of ca. 10 m from the feeders (zoomed on the feeder area). During the second phase, we glued a dead bullet ant (Paraponera clavata; found dead in the forest) to each of the three feeders (2 cm from the flower hole) to simulate a threat. Although the ant is not a predator of hummingbirds, the presence of large insects on flowers often scares various hummingbird species [37], including long-billed hermits (MAS., pers. obs., Supplementary Online Materials: Videos S1 and S2). Given the existence of behavioral syndrome based on fear generalization, wherein fear of one danger is correlated to the fear of another [38], we assumed that despite not being a danger on its own, through its connotations with truly dangerous insects and its overall novelty, the ant would be perceived by the birds as a threat [37]. Importantly, the attached bullet ants did not completely scare the birds off, allowing for the quantification of their foraging behavior in these circumstances. Hence, attaching the bullet ant to the feeders was expected to resemble situations of increased risk of being injured, while still exploiting the feeding resource.
We performed three complete experiment sessions (with two phases) and two incomplete sessions (with the control phase only due to the weather conditions) all within two weeks (with 2–3 days between the sessions). The two complete (two phases) and two incomplete sessions (control phase only) were conducted in the mornings, when the foraging activity peak occurs [28], and only a single complete experiment session was performed in the afternoon, when the activity is lower. Since the time of the day which we considered in the present study did not affect the foraging efficiency considerably (generalized mixed effect model: foraging efficiency ~ hour (fixed effect; numeric) + birdID (random effect); estimate: −0.009 ± SE 0.005, t = −1.74, df = 137.97, p = 0.08), we did not consider it in further analyses. Furthermore, since the entire experiment session was completed within a relatively short window of time (up to three hours), and both phases were completed during the same time window, we do not expect time to bias the comparisons of the control and experimental phases.
The duration of the control phases, including those from incomplete sessions, varied from 0.5 to 3 h. Owing to the regular visits of the birds under these control conditions (ranging from 1 to 12 per hour per individual), we could record multiple visits of focal individuals, with an average of 6 visits per individual (range 2–18). For the experimental phases, we kept recording until all visitors observed during the preceding control phase returned to the feeders, resulting in an average of 3 visits per individual during this phase (range: 1–7). The control phase was always performed before the experimental one, as doing so, we could ensure the recruitment of individuals at both phases. The presence of the bullet ant on their very first encounter with the feeders in a given day might have precluded the birds to explore the resource. To mitigate the potentially negative effect of bullet ant exposure on the frequency of visits, we performed the sessions with a 2–3 day gap in between. For all these practical reasons, we could not randomize the treatment and control phases. A potentially confounding effect that might bias our results due to the lack of randomization is habituation. Under this scenario, familiarity with the experimental set up results in a foraging efficiency trend over time, regardless of the treatment. To address this concern, we analyzed individual foraging efficiency over consecutive visits using data from control phases (see Supplementary Online Materials S2). We found that foraging efficiency changes over time only for one individual, and that happened after a considerable number of visits. Furthermore, in this case, foraging efficiency improved over time. If this pattern had affected the observed difference in efficiency between the treatments, we could expect higher efficiency in the high-risk phase, which was always conducted after the control phase. However, this was not the case (Supplementary Online Materials S2). Consequently, we treated all visits of particular individuals as independent data points, but obviously controlling for their identity in further analyses.

2.2. Videos Analysis

We screened the video recordings using VLC software (https://www.videolan.org/vlc/download-windows.html, accessed on 6 June 2015) to locate and cut out video fragments with the foraging visits of focal birds. The events of two or more individuals (long-billed hermits or a different species) visiting the feeders at the same time were uncommon and were excluded from analysis, as interactions disrupted regular foraging behavior. A foraging visit was considered whenever a bird inserted the bill into a feeder at least once. For each bird’s visit, we established key time-points (black circles in Figure 1) with 0.1 s precision, using Cowlog software 2 [39]. Based on these time-points, we calculated the duration of latency to forage, defined as the interval between the appearance in the feeder area (when hovering in front of the feeder at a distance of around 0.5 m was initiated) and the onset of foraging (time from a to b in Figure 1); the duration of the feeding interval/s (time from b to c in Figure 1; on average 4.9 events, with a range of 1–26); duration of the feeding break/s (time from c to d in Figure 1); the duration of the total foraging (time from b to g in Figure 1); and the duration of the total foraging visit (time from a to g in Figure 1). For each visit, we also noted which and how many times each of the three feeders were used by the focal bird.
To quantify bird movements around the feeders, we took advantage of the line arrangement of the feeders that greatly restricted bird activity into two axes. This simplified the subsequent analyses, as operating in a two-dimensional space, we could establish Cartesian coordinates of a bird’s position for each video frame, using the software Tracker version 5.1.5 (https://www.physlets.org/tracker, accessed on 6 June 2015). Based on those coordinates, we calculated the distances between two birds’ positions using the Pythagorean theorem.

2.3. Parameters

We calculated foraging efficiency as the ratio of the total duration of foraging (sum of the duration of all feeding intervals) to the total duration of the foraging visit. To characterize the foraging strategies, we measured three behavioral features that have been linked to intrinsic individual characteristics in other species and have been shown to affect foraging: exploration, risk avoidance and arousal [1,18,19,20,36]. As a proxy for exploration, we utilized the rate of the visited feeder: the number of feeders divided by the total duration of the visit, as the absolute number of feeder changes is likely to be a function of the time spent at the feeders. As a proxy for risk avoidance, we used latency to start foraging (the very first use of the feeder during the visit), as defined above (time from a to b in Figure 1). For both parameters, the higher the value, the stronger the exhibited behavior. As arousal, we considered the coefficient of variance in the spatial distances covered by an individual during the whole foraging visit, divided by the number of visited feeders. Birds changing the position frequently (high value of the coefficient) were assumed to exhibit higher arousal.

2.4. Data Analysis

All analyses were performed in R [40]. Since all parameters had a skewed distribution, we log-transformed them prior to the analyses (Supplementary Online Materials S1). To examine how consistent the birds were in their behavior during the foraging, we estimated the repeatability of all the examined parameters (foraging efficiency, exploration, risk avoidance and arousal) using the rptR package [41]. To do this, we used behavioral features measured during the low-risk (control) treatment only, which represented undisturbed conditions, and for which we recorded multiple visits per individual. When estimating repeatability, we considered behavioral traits as single-response variables and bird identity as a varying intercept factor (i.e., random effect) [41].
To assess variation in foraging efficiency (response variable) as a function of the risk level and intrinsic behavioral features (exploration, latency and arousal as predictors), we applied a Bayesian generalized linear mixed model with individuals as a random effect. We fit three models representing alternative hypotheses explaining the variation in foraging efficiency. The first model represents a more traditional view of foraging behavior, in which efficiency is only affected by the level of risk. The second model included an interaction among behavioral traits and the risk level, which represents a more nuanced scenario in which the interplay between the risk level and intrinsic behavioral differences determines foraging efficiency. There was only little collinearity between the predictors (VIF for each of the parameters in the model < 1.5, and the correlation coefficient was also low and ranged from −0.21 to −0.10; Supplementary Online Materials S1). We ran this model with a single predictor for intrinsic behavioral traits (separately considering arousal, exploration and risk avoidance), as well as in the form of a global model, with all behavioral predictors included. The two approaches yielded qualitatively similar results; therefore, we presented here only the outcome of the global model, while the outcome of the single-behavior predictor models are presented in Supplementary Online Materials S1. Finally, we fit an intercept-only model, representing the scenario in which the proposed predictors do not affect efficiency. The three alternative models were compared using model selection based on the deviance information criteria (DIC) [42]. The models were fit using the R package MCMCglmm [43].

3. Results

Repeatability was moderate, but significant for all behavioral traits, except arousal (Figure 2). When predicting foraging efficiency, the model, including all intrinsic behavioral traits and their interaction within the risk predation context, performed significantly better than a simpler model including only the risk context (Table 1). All parameters and their interactions were significant in this model, except for risk avoidance (Table 2). Overall, foraging efficiency was lowered within a higher-risk context (Figure 3) and the effect of intrinsic behavioral features on the foraging efficiency was context-dependent (Table 2). The most dramatic effect was found in respect to exploratory behavior, which was positively related to foraging efficiency within a low-risk context, but the opposite pattern was observed within a higher-risk context (Figure 4). Arousal was positively related to foraging efficiency, and this was particularly pronounced when the birds faced a higher risk (Figure 4). Risk avoidance, overall, tended to decrease foraging efficiency, but that was not significant and did not differ between the risk levels (Figure 4).

4. Discussion

As expected according to the risk allocation hypothesis [5,6], the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits decreased in response to threatening conditions. However, behavioral performance relating exploration, risk avoidance and arousal additionally affected the foraging efficiency, and interestingly, it was affected in a condition-dependent manner. These results suggest that a range of strategies, instead of a single fixed strategy, should be considered when modeling foraging behavior within different contexts. The results also highlight the importance of behavioral variability in shaping the evolution of foraging strategy.
Despite not being a specific prey target, hummingbirds may be opportunistically hunted by a wide range of predators, including insects [31,32,33,34,35]. This imposes a considerable predation risk and favors the evolution of vigilant behavior. Indeed, we found that experimental exposure to an ant, potentially representing just the risk of being bitten (but not eaten), was enough for LBHs to exhibit some behavioral changes. Importantly, despite this risk, the birds did forage, although with a lower efficiency. This places LBHs in a group of species representing the so-called paradox of the risk allocation hypothesis: under conditions of frequent predator presence, prey might need to forage actively, even though a threat (either potential or real predator) is present [5,6,45]. In that context, it is worth to evoke one of the assumptions of the risk allocation hypothesis, which is “living on the edge” in terms of meeting energy demands [5,7]. This assumption seems to be rarely met in most animal species used to test the hypothesis so far [45]. Hummingbirds, given their extreme metabolism rate, could be a rare example when the assumption is actually true.
Under control conditions, individuals exhibiting more exploratory behavior also had an overall higher efficiency during the foraging visit. The opposite pattern was observed for the experimental conditions. A simple reason for this shift could be that the time spent switching feeders was longer under risk conditions, which increased the duration of the visit, probably associated with the need to analyze “de novo” the risk situation. Importantly, individuals were consistent during the time of their exploration, which could indicate that this behavior is related to personality [19,22,46,47]. If the exploratory behavior was indeed a personality trait, and that trait had different fitness consequences in regard to predation, the predation pressure is likely to shape the distribution of exploration phenotypes in the population. Unfortunately, examining bird behavior under limited time and contextual space, we were not able to test this prediction. Nevertheless, to encourage future studies, we highlight the potential role of threats in the environment in the evolution of personality [22,48,49].
There was a clear tendency for risk avoidance behavior to negatively affect the foraging efficiency. In the global model, that we presented in the main text, it was not significant but the effect was apparent in a single-trait model (Supplementary Online Materials S1). This indicates that in the most extreme scenario, individuals exhibiting high-risk aversion may tend to jeopardize their survival in terms of energy intake, while individuals with low-risk aversion, although benefiting from high foraging efficiency, would be more likely to be predated. If the risk avoidance was related to the bird personality, the relationship between that and foraging efficiency would contribute to the selection of given behavioral phenotype at a given predation risk level. Consistently, frequent changes and/or an unpredictable level of risk predation in the environment would maintain variability in this behavioral phenotype [18]. Again, we cannot boldly make conclusions about bird personality here, but the results are intriguing and encourage exploring this research avenue in the future.
To maximize fitness, hummingbirds should adaptively allocate both the exploratory and risk avoidance behaviors. Here, we considered the issue in respect to a given species, but interpreting our study in a broader context, we could speculate that exploratory and risk avoidance should be differently allocated in hummingbirds that differ in foraging strategy, such as trapliners and territorials. The two groups are likely to experience different risk levels of predation, and so behaviors such as exploration and risk avoidance could also differ. Weighting the relative effect of these intrinsic behavioral features can allow us to understand the evolutionary factors shaping foraging performance more accurately [50,51,52].
An increasing foraging efficiency with an increase in arousal may be counterintuitive at first glance, as the time allocated to movements potentially limits the time for foraging. However, arousal was not a repeatable trait; thus, birds’ arousal may simply reflect their nutritional state, and may vary considerably between different days and even visits. In such case, more active individuals could be more effective during the foraging, owing to their good body condition or high motivation to forage.
Both the exploratory and risk avoidance behaviors were quite repeatable for individuals. Although more studies are needed to properly examine how stable this repeatability is within different contexts and over a longer period, our results suggest that these two behaviors could be related to bird personality [22,53,54]. In a constantly changing environment, the varying fitness consequences of a given behavioral phenotype would maintain the variation in animal personality [18]. If indeed the exploratory and risk avoidance behavior are at least partially heritable traits, this can be expected to play an important role in the evolution of behavioral phenotypes under diverse conditions of predation pressure. Hummingbirds stand as a useful model for the study of animal behavioral syndromes and their interaction with critical natural history features in the wild.

5. Conclusions

Although with a lower efficiency, long-billed hermits foraged despite an elevated level of risk in the environment. This makes them a good model species for studies on the risk allocation hypothesis, where the propensity to forage is measured in the context of predator presence. Our results show not only that the foraging efficiency of an individual is affected by the presence of a threat, but also that the efficiency depends on the behavioral performance of the individual. More exploration was associated with a higher foraging efficiency under no-risk conditions, but it was lower when the birds were exposed to a risk. Regardless of the conditions, arousal was positively associated with foraging efficiency, while risk avoidance was related negatively. Importantly, exploratory behavior and risk avoidance were quite repeatable behaviors, suggesting that they may be related to the intrinsic traits of individuals. Our findings highlight the importance of taking into account additional behavioral dimensions to better understand the foraging strategies of individuals.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13121997/s1, Supplementary Online Materials S1. All analyses performed for the purpose of the present study. Source code of the statistical analyses can be also found at https://github.com/maRce10/foraging_efficiency_in_long_billed_hermit_hummingbirds (accessed on 4 June 2023). Supplementary Online Materials S2. Analysis of the time effect. Data. lbh_dt.xlsx. Videos S1 and S2. Long-billed hermit visits at the feeder area under control and experimental conditions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; methodology, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; formal analysis, M.A.-S.; investigation, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; resources, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; data curation, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; writing—original draft preparation, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; writing—review and editing, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S.; visualization, M.A.-S.; project administration, M.A.-S.; funding acquisition, K.W.-J. and M.A.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Geographic Society, grant number 9169-12 (to MAS), the British Ornithological Union (awarded in 2015 to KWJ), a research initiation grant from New Mexico State University (to MAS), the College of Arts and Sciences and the Biology Department at New Mexico State University (to MAS) and the Organization for Tropical Studies (to MAS).

Institutional Review Board Statement

All activities (birds marking, feeding and video recording/observations) were performed with the greatest care. The capturing and marking procedure did not have an apparent effect on the birds’ survival, as all the individuals were observed in the lek and/or at the feeders area after the capturing. The foam used to mark the birds was of negligible weight (0.02 g, which constitutes ~0.3% of the average body mass (6 g) of LBHs). The study was reviewed and authorized by the Costa Rican Ministerio del Ambiente y Energia (063-2011-SINAC), and performed in accordance with their guidelines and regulations.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data and data analysis scripts associated with the manuscript are included as supplementary materials.

Acknowledgments

We thank Elizabeth Rogers, Judith Smith and Virgilio Lopez III for assistance in field work, and Yuki Brooknevskaya for overall encouragement during the working on the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Herborn, K.A.; Heidinger, B.J.; Alexander, L.; Arnold, K.E. Personality Predicts Behavioral Flexibility in a Fluctuating, Natural Environment. Behav. Ecol. 2014, 25, 1374–1379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Morrison, M.M.; Raplph, C.J.; Verner, J.; Jehl, J.R.J. Avian Foraging: Theory, Methodology and Applications. Stud. Avian Biol. 1990, 13, 1–515. [Google Scholar]
  3. Charnov, E.L. Optimal Foraging, the Marginal Value Theorem. Theor. Popul. Biol. 1976, 9, 739–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Bautista, L.M.; Tinbergen, J.; Kacelnik, A. To Walk or to Fly? How Birds Choose among Foraging Modes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 1089–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Lima, S.L.; Bednekoff, P.A. Temporal Variation in Danger Drives Antipredator Behavior: The Predation Risk Allocation Hypothesis. Am. Nat. 1999, 153, 649–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Houston, A.I.; Mcnamara, J.M.; Hutchinson, J.M.C.; Trans, P.; Lond, R.S. General Results Concerning the Trade-off between Gaining Energy and Avoiding Predation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B Biol. Sci. 1993, 341, 375–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Verdolin, J.L. Meta-Analysis of Foraging and Predation Risk Trade-Offs in Terrestrial Systems. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2006, 60, 457–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Krebs, J.R. Optimal Foraging, Predation Risk and Territory Defence. Ardea 1980, 68, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Patrick, S.C.; Bearhop, S.; Grémillet, D.; Lescroël, A.; Grecian, W.J.; Bodey, T.W.; Hamer, K.C.; Wakefield, E.; Le Nuz, M.; Votier, S.C. Individual Differences in Searching Behaviour and Spatial Foraging Consistency in a Central Place Marine Predator. Oikos 2014, 123, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Camprasse, E.C.M.; Cherel, Y.; Bustamante, P.; Arnould, J.P.Y.; Bost, C.A. Intra-and Inter-Individual Variation in the Foraging Ecology of a Generalist Subantarctic Seabird, the Gentoo Penguin. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2017, 578, 227–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Moldoff, D.E.; Westneat, D.F. Foraging Sparrows Exhibit Individual Differences but Not a Syndrome When Responding to Multiple Kinds of Novelty. Behav. Ecol. 2017, 28, 732–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dingemanse, N.J.; Kazem, A.J.N.; Reale, D.; Wright, J. Behavioural Reaction Norms: Animal Personality Meets Individual Plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2009, 25, 81–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Alonzo, S.H. Integrating the How and Why of within-Individual and among-Individual Variation and Plasticity in Behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 6, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Toscano, B.J.; Gownaris, N.J.; Heerhartz, S.M.; Monaco, C.J. Personality, Foraging Behavior and Specialization: Integrating Behavioral and Food Web Ecology at the Individual Level. Oecologia 2016, 182, 55–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bell, A.M.; Hankison, S.J.; Laskowski, K.L. The Repeatability of Behaviour: A Meta-Analysis. Anim. Behav. 2009, 77, 771–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Nussey, D.H.; Wilson, A.J.; Brommer, J.E. The Evolutionary Ecology of Individual Phenotypic Plasticity in Wild Populations. J. Evol. Biol. 2007, 20, 831–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Carere, C.; Maestripieri, D. Animal Personalities; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  18. Smith, B.R.; Blumstein, D.T. Fitness Consequences of Personality: A Meta-Analysis. Behav. Ecol. 2008, 19, 448–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Réale, D.; Reader, S.M.; Sol, D.; McDougall, P.T.; Dingemanse, N.J. Integrating Animal Temperament within Ecology and Evolution. Biol. Rev. 2007, 82, 291–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Quinn, J.L.; Cole, E.F.; Bates, J.; Payne, R.W.; Cresswell, W. Personality Predicts Individual Responsiveness to the Risks of Starvation and Predation. Proc. R. Soc. B 2012, 279, 1919–1926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Dall, S.R.X.; Houston, A.I.; McNamara, J.M. The Behavioural Ecology of Personality: Consistent Individual Differences from an Adaptive Perspective. Ecol. Lett. 2004, 7, 734–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Dingemanse, N.J.; Réale, D. Natural Selection and Animal Personality. Behaviour 2005, 142, 1165–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bergeron, P.; Montiglio, P.O.; Réale, D.; Humphries, M.M.; Gimenez, O.; Garant, D. Disruptive Viability Selection on Adult Exploratory Behaviour in Eastern Chipmunks. J. Evol. Biol. 2013, 26, 766–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Roth, A.M.; Dingemanse, N.J.; Nakagawa, S.; McDonald, G.C.; Løvlie, H.; Robledo-Ruiz, D.A.; Pizzari, T. Sexual Selection and Personality: Individual and Group-Level Effects on Mating Behaviour in Red Junglefowl. J. Anim. Ecol. 2021, 90, 1288–1306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Le Cœur, C.; Thibault, M.; Pisanu, B.; Thibault, S.; Chapuis, J.L.; Baudry, E. Temporally Fluctuating Selection on a Personality Trait in a Wild Rodent Population. Behav. Ecol. 2015, 26, 1285–1291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Mouchet, A.; Cole, E.F.; Matthysen, E.; Nicolaus, M.; Quinn, J.L.; Roth, A.M.; Tinbergen, J.M.; van Oers, K.; van Overveld, T.; Dingemanse, N.J. Heterogeneous Selection on Exploration Behavior within and among West European Populations of a Passerine Bird. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2024994118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Suarez, R.K. Hummingbird Flight: Sustaining the Highest Mass-Specific Metabolic Rates among Vertebrates. Experientia 1992, 48, 565–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stiles, F.G.; Wolf, L.L. Ecology and Evolution of Lek Mating Behavior in the Long-Tailed Hermit Hummingbird. Ornithol. Monogr. 1979, 27, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Araya-Salas, M.; Gonzalez-Gomez, P.; Wojczulanis-Jakubas, K.; López, V.; Wright, T.F. Spatial Memory Is as Important as Weapon and Body Size for Territorial Ownership in a Lekking Hummingbird. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Stiles, F.G. Possible Specialization for Hummingbird-Hunting in the Tiny Hawk. Auk 1978, 95, 550–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nyffeler, M.; Maxwell, M.R.; Remsen, J.V. Bird Predation by Praying Mantises: A Global Perspective. Wilson J. Ornithol. 2017, 129, 331–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Owen, J.L.; Cokendolpher, J.C. Tailless Whipscorpion (Phrynus longipes) Feeds on Antillean Crested Hummingbird (Orthorhyncus cristatus). Wilson J. Ornithol. 2006, 118, 422–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sazima, I. Lightning Predator: The Ferruginous Pygmy Owl Snatches Flower-Visiting Hummingbirds in Southwestern Brazil. Rev. Bras. Ornitol. 2015, 23, 12–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Zenzal, T.J.; Fish, A.C.; Jones, T.M.; Ospina, E.A.; Moore, F.R. Observations of Predation and Anti-Predator Behavior of Rubythroated Hummingbirds During Migratory Stopover. Southeast. Nat. 2013, 12, N21–N25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lorenz, S. Carolina Mantind (Stagmomantis carolica) Captures and Feeds on a Broad-Tailed Hummingbird (Selasphorus platycercus). Bull. Texas Ornithol. Soc. 2007, 40, 1–40. [Google Scholar]
  36. Couchoux, C.; Cresswell, W. Personality Constraints versus Flexible Antipredation Behaviors: How Important Is Boldness in Risk Management of Redshanks (Tringa totanus) Foraging in a Natural System? Behav. Ecol. 2012, 23, 290–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Carr, J.M.; Golinski, J.E. Vigilance Behaviors of Ruby-Throated Hummingbirds (Archilochus colubris) Reflect Elevated Risk of Competitive Interactions with Vespine Wasps. Wilson J. Ornithol. 2020, 132, 295–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sih, A.; Chyng, H.J.; Neylan Isabelle Ortiz-Jimenez, C.; Sakai, O.; Szeligowski, R. Fear Generalization and Behavioral Responses to Multiple Dangers. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2023, 38, 369–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Pastell, M. CowLog—Cross-Platform Application for Coding Behaviours from Video. J. Open Res. Softw. 2016, 4, e15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Core Team: Vienna, Austria, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  41. Stoffel, M.A.; Nakagawa, S.; Schielzeth, H. RptR: Repeatability Estimation and Variance Decomposition by Generalized Linear Mixed-Effects Models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2017, 8, 1639–1644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Meyer, R. Deviance Information Criterion (DIC). Wiley StatsRef: Statistics Reference Online; Balakrishnan, N., Colton, T., Everitt, B., Piegorsch, W., Ruggeri, F., Teugels, J.L., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Hoboken, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hadfield, J.D. MCMCglmm: MCMC Methods for Multi-Response GLMMs in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2010, 33, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Nakagawa, S.; Schielzeth, H. Repeatability for Gaussian and Non-Gaussian Data: A Practical Guide for Biologists. Biol. Rev. 2010, 85, 935–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Ferrari, M.C.O.; Sih, A.; Chivers, D.P. The Paradox of Risk Allocation: A Review and Prospectus. Anim. Behav. 2009, 78, 579–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Montiglio, P.; Sih, A.; Mathot, K.J.; Wolf, M.; Dingemanse, N.J. Animal Personality and State—Behaviour Feedbacks: A Review and Guide for Empiricists. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2015, 30, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Dingemanse, N.J.; Kazem, A.J.N.; Wright, J.; Biologiques, S. Evolutionary and Ecological Approaches to the Study of Personality. Philosoph. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2010, 365, 3937–3946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Nácarová, J.; Veselý, P.; Fuchs, R. Effect of the Exploratory Behaviour on a Bird’s Ability to Categorize a Predator. Behav. Processes 2018, 151, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Exnerová, A.; Svádová, K.H.; Fučíková, E.; Drent, P.; Štys, P. Personality Matters: Individual Variation in Reactions of Naive Bird Predators to Aposematic Prey. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2010, 277, 723–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Tello-Ramos, M.C.; Hurly, T.A.; Healy, S.D. Traplining in Hummingbirds: Flying Shortdistance Sequences among Several Locations. Behav. Ecol. 2015, 26, 812–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Ohashi, K.; Thomson, J.D. Efficient Harvesting of Renewing Resources. Behav. Ecol. 2005, 16, 592–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Gill, F.B. Trapline Foraging by Hermit Hummingbirds: Competition for an Undefended, Renewable Resource. Ecology 1988, 69, 1933–1942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Sih, A.; Bell, A.; Johnson, J.C. Behavioral Syndromes: An Ecological and Evolutionary Overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2004, 19, 372–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Cleasby, I.R.; Nakagawa, S.; Schielzeth, H. Quantifying the Predictability of Behaviour: Statistical Approaches for the Study of between-Individual Variation in the within-Individual Variance. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2015, 6, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Scheme of the foraging visit—the total time spent by focal birds at the feeder area, with at least one feeding event. Time-points crucial for data analysis are denoted with black circles and labelled with letters to denote particular events relating the components of the foraging visit: (a) onset of the foraging visit (appearance in the feeder area, usually hovering in front of the feeder); (b, d, f) onsets of consecutive feeding events (i.e., inserting the bill into the flower hole of the feeder); (c, e) end of the respective feeding events (i.e., removal of the bill from the flower hole of the feeder); (g) end of the foraging visit (i.e., the end of the very last feeding event during the foraging visit). Multiple feeding intervals were possible (1–26, mean: 4.9).
Figure 1. Scheme of the foraging visit—the total time spent by focal birds at the feeder area, with at least one feeding event. Time-points crucial for data analysis are denoted with black circles and labelled with letters to denote particular events relating the components of the foraging visit: (a) onset of the foraging visit (appearance in the feeder area, usually hovering in front of the feeder); (b, d, f) onsets of consecutive feeding events (i.e., inserting the bill into the flower hole of the feeder); (c, e) end of the respective feeding events (i.e., removal of the bill from the flower hole of the feeder); (g) end of the foraging visit (i.e., the end of the very last feeding event during the foraging visit). Multiple feeding intervals were possible (1–26, mean: 4.9).
Animals 13 01997 g001
Figure 2. Repeatability estimates of foraging efficiency and behavioral parameters in the long-billed hermit measured under control conditions, analyzed using a linear mixed-effects models [44]. Dots represent the value of the repeatability coefficient, and the bars 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 2. Repeatability estimates of foraging efficiency and behavioral parameters in the long-billed hermit measured under control conditions, analyzed using a linear mixed-effects models [44]. Dots represent the value of the repeatability coefficient, and the bars 95% confidence intervals.
Animals 13 01997 g002
Figure 3. Foraging efficiency in the context of low and high levels of perceived risk. Data points, boxplot (with the median and inter-quartile range) and data distribution are presented.
Figure 3. Foraging efficiency in the context of low and high levels of perceived risk. Data points, boxplot (with the median and inter-quartile range) and data distribution are presented.
Animals 13 01997 g003
Figure 4. Foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits relating to their behavioral performance (exploration, risk avoidance and arousal) in the context of low and high levels of perceived risk of predation. Regression lines presented with 95% confidence intervals (shadow area).
Figure 4. Foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits relating to their behavioral performance (exploration, risk avoidance and arousal) in the context of low and high levels of perceived risk of predation. Regression lines presented with 95% confidence intervals (shadow area).
Animals 13 01997 g004
Table 1. Ranking of the models explaining the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits, ordered by delta Deviance Information Criterion (DIC; Akaike’s Information Criterion AIC yields the same conclusions).
Table 1. Ranking of the models explaining the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits, ordered by delta Deviance Information Criterion (DIC; Akaike’s Information Criterion AIC yields the same conclusions).
PredictorsdfDICΔDICWeight DICAICΔAICWeight AIC
md_all_interactions 10−400.09090.001−396.30730.000.99
md_arousal_exploration 8−388.238511.850−386.283110.020.01
md_arousal_risk_avoidance 8−378.980721.110−376.818419.490.00
md_arousal 6−363.341036.750−363.250933.060.00
md_risk_avoidance_exploration 8−350.156849.930−348.814047.490.00
md_exploration 6−345.771654.320−346.406549.900.00
md_risk_avoidance 6−315.225884.870−315.092981.210.00
md_context 4−308.603691.490−310.799585.510.00
md_null 3−296.3098103.780−299.834796.470.00
Table 2. Effects of behavioral variables and experimental context (low/high risk) on the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits (N individuals = 12, N data points = 192). Common intercept = 0.4548, effects are the slope estimates derived from the first top Bayesian MCMC generalized linear model (Table 1). Significant effects are denoted in bold.
Table 2. Effects of behavioral variables and experimental context (low/high risk) on the foraging efficiency of long-billed hermits (N individuals = 12, N data points = 192). Common intercept = 0.4548, effects are the slope estimates derived from the first top Bayesian MCMC generalized linear model (Table 1). Significant effects are denoted in bold.
PredictorEffect SizeCI 2.5%CI 97.5%pMCMC
contextHigh risk−0.1409−0.2732−0.01320.0322
Arousal0.06840.02750.10830.0006
Exploration0.36860.12440.61670.0023
risk_avoidance−0.0327−0.06630.00230.0641
contextHigh risk:arousal0.24450.15410.34360.0001
contextHigh risk:exploration−0.8355−1.1641−0.49250.0001
contextHigh risk:risk_avoidance−0.0270−0.07930.0210.2918
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wojczulanis-Jakubas, K.; Araya-Salas, M. Foraging, Fear and Behavioral Variation in a Traplining Hummingbird. Animals 2023, 13, 1997. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13121997

AMA Style

Wojczulanis-Jakubas K, Araya-Salas M. Foraging, Fear and Behavioral Variation in a Traplining Hummingbird. Animals. 2023; 13(12):1997. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13121997

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wojczulanis-Jakubas, Katarzyna, and Marcelo Araya-Salas. 2023. "Foraging, Fear and Behavioral Variation in a Traplining Hummingbird" Animals 13, no. 12: 1997. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13121997

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop