Next Article in Journal
Insights on the Evolutionary History and Genetic Patterns of Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797 in the Northeastern Atlantic Using Mitochondrial DNA
Next Article in Special Issue
Manatees in Zoological Parks throughout the World: History, State, and Welfare
Previous Article in Journal
Population Status and Vulnerability of Mantidactylus pauliani from Ankaratra Protected Area, Madagascar
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fish Welfare in Public Aquariums and Zoological Collections
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Are Dolphins Kept in Impoverished Environments?

Dolphin Research Center, Grassy Key, FL 33050, USA
Animals 2023, 13(17), 2707; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172707
Submission received: 15 April 2023 / Revised: 29 May 2023 / Accepted: 23 August 2023 / Published: 25 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Zoo and Aquarium Welfare, Ethics, Behavior)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Dolphins are among the most popular animals in zoos and aquariums around the world. Recently, however, a paper suggested that environments for dolphins in zoos and aquariums might be “impoverished”, leading to possible problems for their brain and cognitive functioning. This review directly examines that hypothesis in light of the existing scientific literature relevant to dolphin welfare in zoos and aquariums. The results of this analysis—based on the documented standards of modern professional zoological organizations, the results of a multi-national study on dolphin welfare, and the behavior of dolphins in modern zoos and aquariums—show that this claim is clearly false. However, as “not impoverished” would be a ridiculously low bar to set as an animal welfare standard, additional strategies are suggested for providing cognitive challenges in zoos and aquariums to optimize dolphins’ cognitive well-being.

Abstract

Numerous studies have demonstrated the negative effects of impoverished environments versus the positive effects of enriched environments on animals’ cognitive and neural functioning. Recently, a hypothesis was raised suggesting that conditions for dolphins in zoological facilities may be inherently impoverished, and thus lead to neural and cognitive deficits. This review directly examines that hypothesis in light of the existing scientific literature relevant to dolphin welfare in zoological facilities. Specifically, it examines how dolphins are housed in modern zoological facilities, where the characteristics of such housing fall on the continuum of impoverished-to-enriched environments, and the extent to which dolphins show behavioral evidence characteristic of living in impoverished environments. The results of this analysis show that contrary to the original hypothesis, modern zoological facilities do not inherently, or even typically, house dolphins in impoverished conditions. However, it also notes that there is variation in animal welfare across different zoological facilities, and that “not impoverished” would be a particularly low bar to set as an animal welfare standard. To optimize cognitive well-being, strategies for providing additional cognitive challenges for dolphins in zoological facilities are suggested.

1. Introduction

Around the world, bottlenose dolphins are among the most popular species, belonging to the group of animals known as “charismatic megafauna”, to which people feel particularly strong emotional connections [1,2,3,4,5]. At least part of this popularity is likely due to the public’s experience with this species in zoological facilities (i.e., zoos, aquaria, and marine mammal facilities), where they are the most common species of cetacean displayed [6,7], and where their high rates of activity and interaction with humans make them particularly appealing [8,9,10,11].
Research over the past couple of decades suggests that dolphins typically fare well in modern zoological facilities, with life spans as long as or longer than in the wild [12], stress levels as reflected in baseline cortisol no higher than in the wild [13,14], high reproductive rates [15,16], high adaptability [7], and generally good welfare [17]. Recently, however, a paper by Jacobs et al. [18] challenged this view with a controversial hypothesis that living in modern zoos and aquaria might cause neural deficits in dolphins (as well as in other cetaceans and elephants). Specifically, they argue that because it has been shown that housing animals in impoverished environments results in a number of negative neural consequences, housing dolphins in zoological facilities should result in these same negative neural consequences.
The detrimental effects of impoverished environments on neural and cognitive functioning are noncontroversial, as numerous studies have demonstrated the cognitive and neural effects of housing animals in enriched (positive effects) versus impoverished (negative effects) environments (see reviews in [19,20,21,22,23]). It is puzzling, however, why Jacobs et al. [18] would choose to recast this issue as reflecting an inherent difference between animals in the wild versus in zoological facilities, when that entire field of scientific literature examines the effects of different levels of enrichment within zoological facilities and other human-controlled environments (e.g., homes, laboratories) (see references in Jacobs et al. [18]). In reframing this issue to consistently conflate “captive” with “impoverished” environments, Jacobs et al. ignore the last 45 years of scientific literature on animal welfare and the effects of various enrichment practices in zoological settings [24,25,26,27,28]. Be that as it may, the serious allegation they raise calls for serious discussion. The validity of their hypothesis (i.e., that impoverished environments cause neural deficits in other animals, therefore zoos cause neural deficits in dolphins) directly depends on the extent to which the zoological facilities where dolphins are housed embody impoverished environments.
In the current paper, we directly examine this proposed equivalence in light of the existing scientific literature relevant to dolphin welfare in zoological facilities. Specifically, we review the following: (a) how dolphins are housed in modern zoological facilities, (b) where the characteristics of such housing fall on the continuum of impoverished-to-enriched environments described by Jacobs et al. [18], and (c) the extent to which dolphins show behavioral evidence characteristic of living in impoverished environments.

2. What Do We Mean by “Impoverished”?

In the standard experimental framework used in hundreds of experiments to explore the neural and cognitive effects of different environments, animals are housed in one of three conditions: impoverished, standard/control, or enriched/complex. Then, after a period of time, the animals from two or more of these contrasting conditions are compared on a cognitive task, or their brains are compared on a neural characteristic. As summarized by Jacobs et al. [18], these three types of conditions can typically be characterized as follows:
  • Impoverished = animals are alone in smaller cages with no opportunity for social or object interaction;
  • Standard/control = several animals are housed together without opportunities to interact with stimulatory objects;
  • Enriched/complex = animals are housed in a relatively large enclosure together with several conspecifics and multiple objects/toys, which are frequently changed to provide novelty.
As described above, the types of environments in these studies vary along three factors: (a) social situation (whether the animal is solitary or with others); (b) size of enclosure; and (c) complexity and variability of the environment. The levels of these factors are not independently varied, but rather each varies along a similar but not identical continuum across the environments, as shown in Table 1.
With this framework in mind, we are now in a position to examine the conditions in which dolphins are housed in modern zoological facilities, with particular attention to these three factors.

3. Dolphin Environments in Modern Zoological Facilities

Jacobs et al. [18] argue that zoological facilities are inherently impoverished environments for dolphins, framing their argument as the effects of “captivity” as a whole, and repeatedly using the conflated term “captive/impoverished” (or “impoverished/captive”). Notably, their claim is not simply that some dolphins are housed in impoverished conditions, but rather that the conditions in zoological facilities are inherently impoverished. This is an important distinction. If poor conditions at some zoological facilities are negatively impacting animal welfare, then the solution to poor welfare is to improve the conditions in those facilities. However, if variation between zoological facilities is irrelevant because conditions at even the best facilities are inherently impoverished, then the only possible solution to poor welfare is to not have dolphins in zoological facilities at all.
Professional zoological organizations—including the Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums (AMMPA), the Association of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA), the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA), and the European Association for Aquatic Mammals (EAAM)—provide standards and guidelines that govern the housing conditions and management practices in every zoological facility accredited by that organization [29,30,31,32]. In the United States and Europe, approximately 75% of facilities housing dolphins belong to at least one of these organizations [33,34,35,36,37]. Thus, we can use the standards and guidelines of these organizations to examine the relevant practices typical of facilities in these jurisdictions.

3.1. Social Situation

According to Jacobs et al.’s [18] framework, in an impoverished environment, the animals are housed alone. In contrast, the standards of professional zoological organizations stipulate that dolphins must be housed in appropriate social groupings, only isolated when strictly necessary for the animal’s health or well-being [30,31,32].

3.2. Enclosure Size

According to Jacobs et al.’s [18] framework, in an impoverished environment, the animals are housed in small enclosures. There is no agreed-upon objective measurement defining what qualifies as “small”, however, other than being smaller than the other enclosures to which it is being compared. Similarly, the standards of professional zoological organizations require that dolphin enclosures provide sufficient space to meet the animals’ physical and social needs [29,30,31,32]; however, there is no universally agreed-upon standard as to how large that space should be. Deciding on this issue scientifically can be difficult, as numerous animal welfare experts have noted that it is the quality, rather than quantity, of the space that may be most important for animal well-being [38,39,40]. Ideally, then, enclosure size requirements for any particular species should be informed by studies of how the size, quality, and management of the space impact welfare outcomes for that species. Recently, such a series of studies was conducted as part of a large, multi-institutional collaborative research effort (called the Cetacean Welfare Study) undertaken among zoological facilities accredited by AMMPA and AZA (see [41] and associated papers).
Two of the biggest concerns with small enclosures are that they may: (a) restrict the animals’ ability to perform normal behaviors and achieve typical and healthy levels of activity and exercise, and (b) increase aggression due to forced proximity while simultaneously restricting animals’ ability to retreat from aggressive situations [18,40,42,43].

3.2.1. Activity Levels

The only study to date that has directly compared activity levels of the same dolphins in different-sized pools seems to give credence to this concern. Bassos and Wells [44] reported that two dolphins swam significantly more when housed in a larger pool (a 54 × 40 ft oval), and were inactive significantly more when housed in a smaller pool (a 30 ft diameter circle). It is worth noting, however, that both pools in that study were significantly smaller (i.e., the longest horizontal distance for the larger pool was 2.5 times smaller) than the average size of enclosures in the Cetacean Welfare Study [45]. Thus, while there is evidence that particularly small enclosures can negatively impact activity levels, this enclosure size is not representative of dolphin enclosures in modern zoological facilities.
In addition, several studies have reported that dolphins kept in larger sea pen enclosures swim more than dolphins that are kept in smaller pool enclosures [45,46,47,48]. However, since these enclosures differ in both size and complexity/variability, it is unclear which variable is responsible for this difference in activity (see Section 3.3 on environmental complexity and variability). Representative data on activity levels in modern zoological facilities can be seen in the Cetacean Welfare Study, which found that dolphins swam an average of 2.32 km each hour outside of training sessions [49]. For comparison, studies of wild dolphins have reported travel distances of up to 30 to 50.2 km in a day [50,51,52], which translates to an average of 1.25 to 2.09 km each hour. These numbers may not be directly analogous, given differences in sampling times and methodologies between the two types of environments; however, the data do not show any evidence of a negative impact on activity levels for dolphins in modern zoological facilities.

3.2.2. Aggression

The only study to date that has directly examined the aggressive behavior of dolphins in different-sized pools also gives credence to the concern about particularly small enclosures. Specifically, Myers and Overstrom [53] reported that dolphins displayed a large number of sexual/aggressive responses when they were kept in a small holding pool between shows. It is worth noting, however, that the 7.3 m diameter holding pool was actually divided by nets into four quadrants in which each of the four dolphins was kept individually [53,54]. In this arrangement, the longest horizontal distance for each quadrant was approximately eight times smaller than the average for the enclosures in the Cetacean Welfare Study [45]. Therefore, while there is evidence that particularly small enclosures with physically isolated animals can increase aggression, this enclosure size and social situation are not at all representative of dolphin enclosures in modern zoological facilities. In fact, once the net barriers were removed and the animals were housed together in the holding pool, the sexual/aggressive response ceased occurring [53]. A more representative picture of aggression in modern facilities can be seen in the Cetacean Welfare Study, which found levels of overt aggression among bottlenose dolphins to be quite low [55].

3.3. Complexity and Variability of the Environment

According to Jacobs et al.’s [18] framework, in an impoverished environment the animals are housed in barren enclosures with no objects to interact with and no variability. In contrast, the standards of professional zoological organizations stipulate that dolphins’ housing must provide environmental complexity and enrichment [29,30,31,32]. With respect to the current discussion, it is worth noting that any environmental complexity or enrichment at all would be enough to elevate the level of this factor above “impoverished”. However, with respect to the broader issues regarding animal welfare, it is worth pointing out that the practice of environmental enrichment is much more elaborate than this criterion suggests [24,27,28].
In practice, zoological facilities differ with respect to the type(s) of enrichment provided to dolphins, how much variability there is in the enrichment schedule, and how frequently new types of enrichment are introduced [45]. These differences are then reflected in multiple indicators of animal welfare, including habitat use, activity levels, and positive social interactions [41,49,55,56]. In fact, consistent with previous research on terrestrial animals [57,58,59,60], the Cetacean Welfare Study found that enrichment programs were more important to dolphin welfare than was the size of their enclosure [41].
In addition to environmental features and objects, the positive reinforcement training sessions that are standard for dolphins in zoological facilities likely provide additional enrichment [43,61,62,63,64,65,66]. Different from older traditional training techniques that may rely on coercion and punishment, positive reinforcement training focuses on the timing of providing various reinforcers (e.g., food, praise, rubs) dependent on the animal’s behavior in order to encourage specific behaviors over others [67,68]. By gradually changing the criteria for which behaviors get reinforced, the trainer can lead the animal through a series of successive approximations to a target behavior. To be clear, this technique does not require withholding food [63,69]. Rather, the trainer makes precise moment-to-moment decisions regarding exactly whennot whetherto provide reinforcers. Performed correctly, this kind of training provides animals with mental stimulation [43,63,67], increases their physical activity and behavioral variability [62,63,67,70], and increases opportunities for choice and control over aspects of their environment [43,63,64,66].

3.4. Impoverished, Standard, or Enriched?

The results of the above analysis suggest that, contrary to Jacobs et al.’s [18] hypothesis, modern zoological facilities do not inherently, or even typically, house dolphins in impoverished conditions. If they did, then according to Jacobs et al.’s definitions, the dolphins would be housed alone, in small enclosures, with no objects or other enriching experiences (see Table 1). Instead, both the accreditation standards of professional zoological organizations and the results of a large multi-national study on cetacean welfare showed that dolphins in zoological facilities today are normally housed in social groups, with a variety of enriching objects and experiences, in enclosures much larger than the abnormally small pools referenced as problematic in earlier studies. Rather than impoverished, a more accurate description of these environments would thus place them at the level of standard/control or enriched/complex (Table 1).

4. Do Dolphins Show Any Behavioral Evidence of Living in Impoverished Environments?

In spite of the preceding analysis, one might theoretically argue that even if the conditions of their housing in modern zoological facilities do not fit the classic definition of impoverished environments, it is possible that dolphins could have particularly demanding cognitive and social needs, and that therefore what counts as “impoverished” (i.e., leading to detrimental effects) might fall at a different point on the continuum. To explore this possibility, we next examine whether dolphins in modern zoological facilities routinely exhibit behavioral characteristics suggesting the detrimental effects of living in impoverished environments. Specifically, Jacobs et al. [18] propose that cetaceans in facilities exhibit three general behavioral/psychological characteristics—stereotypies, hyperaggression, and symptoms characteristic of depression—that appear to be “manifestations of the same kinds of neurobiological deficits demonstrated in other mammals in impoverished environments” (p. 443). As before, the relevant question is not whether any dolphin in any facility has ever exhibited these characteristics (which would be an individual welfare problem that needs to be addressed), but rather whether this is the normal state for dolphins in modern facilities, as would be expected if zoological facilities are inherently an impoverished situation for dolphins.

4.1. Stereotypies

Stereotypies are generally defined as invariant, repetitive behavior patterns that have no obvious goal or function [71,72]. These behaviors can take many forms, such as rocking or weaving [73,74], pacing [75,76], or repetitive jumping [77,78]. Note that it is typically not the specific behavioral unit that is considered problematic—nobody would be the least concerned about an animal that shifted their weight, walked across the front of their enclosure, or jumped. Rather, it is the largely invariant and perseverative repetition that marks the behavior as aberrant.
The existence of stereotypical behavior has long been thought to indicate that an animal’s environment is deficient or inadequate in some way (e.g., [71,79]), as suggested by a multitude of studies showing that housing animals in impoverished environments is associated with the development of stereotypies (reviewed in, e.g., [71,80]), while enriching an animal’s environment often reduces stereotypies (reviewed in, e.g., [81,82]). One complicating factor with this interpretation is that stereotypies can persist even after the animal is moved from an impoverished to an enriched environment, not unlike a psychological scar indicating the effects of a past situation [80,83]. For current purposes, then, it seems fair to conclude that if stereotypical behaviors are prevalent, this would likely indicate some deficiency in the animals’ current or past environment.
To be sure, stereotypical behavior has been—and continues to be—an ongoing issue with many animals in various types of human-controlled environments, occurring, for example, in an estimated 18% of horses housed in stables, 82% of wild carnivores in zoos, and 91% of pigs (sows) kept in gestation crates [80]. This suggests that even with the considerable advancements in animal welfare science and practice over the past several decades in zoos, farms, and laboratories [84,85,86,87,88], there is still a need for even more widespread attention to environmental complexity and enrichment for animal species under all types of human care (see Section 5 on optimizing cognitive well-being below).
With respect to dolphins, several decades ago, Greenwood [89] reported a clear case of stereotyped “head-pressing” behavior in three individual bottlenose dolphins, in which they would repeatedly press their foreheads against the wall of their pools. The enclosures in which the dolphins were kept during this time were extremely small and barren. Specifically, the largest was 60,000 L, which is smaller than the minimum size required by professional zoological standards [29,30,31], and would be illegal in many countries today [90]. When the dolphins were moved to larger pools, this stereotypy immediately disappeared [89]. This tells us that dolphins, like many other animals, exhibit stereotypies in problematic environments. So are stereotypies a prevalent issue with dolphins in modern zoological facilities?
For bottlenose dolphins, the behavior most often suggested as a potential stereotypy is repetitively swimming in a fixed pattern around their pool [42,47,91,92,93]. To that end, studies have found that dolphins in facilities do spend a considerable amount of time swimming around the perimeter of their enclosures [47,92,94], perhaps especially when the enclosures are smaller [47,92]. However, a number of researchers have questioned whether this is actually a stereotypy (e.g., [42,91,95]). Importantly, a hallmark of stereotypic route tracing (such as stereotypic pacing or swimming) is a dramatic invariance in the animal’s motor pattern, such as turning at the same location, placing their feet at the same spot, and/or taking the same number of steps in their route [76,96,97]. When viewed through this lens, the one study that coded aspects of the dolphins’ behavior in addition to their route [92] found that although the dolphins swam in clockwise circles around their pool, the “routes of the swimming patterns were not followed very strictly” (p. 342), and that “turning, breathing and leaping were not shown consistently at any particular locations” (p. 340). Similarly, more recent studies that incorporated this notion of a relatively invariant path found that stereotypic route tracing happened extremely infrequently, occurring in less than 1% of scans in a study of 18 dolphins in 6 facilities [98], and an average of 0.00 times per minute in a multi-national study of 47 dolphins at 25 facilities [99].
However, if perimeter swimming is not a stereotypical behavior, then why do dolphins do it? Several researchers have noted that it may actually serve a patrolling function [91,100]. However, this explanation overlooks the finding that perimeter swimming may be more common in smaller enclosures than in large ones [47,92]. A simpler explanation may stem from the fact that dolphins typically spend a large proportion of their time traveling [101,102,103]. In an enclosure, circling the perimeter is the closest approximation to sustained traveling without abrupt changes of direction for no reason. From this explanation, one would expect that dolphins who have the most linear space would swim linearly more, while dolphins with less linear space would circle the perimeter more (cf. [47]).

4.2. Hyperaggression

Despite Jacobs et al.’s [18] claim that “hyperaggression is more common in captive cetaceans than in their free counterparts” (pp. 442–443), there is in fact no scientific evidence to suggest that this is true. On the contrary, multiple studies have documented wild dolphins displaying aggressive and even lethal behavior towards adults and infants of their own and other cetacean species (see, e.g., [104,105,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113]). This is not an aberration, but rather typical dominance, territorial, and reproduction-related behavior common in dolphins and other animal species [112,114,115,116]. It is precisely because social conflicts are both natural and expected that the standards of professional zoological organizations all require that facilities have strategies to monitor and mitigate potential conflict and incompatibility between animals, including things such as behavioral training, barriers, and possible separation when conflicts arise [29,30,31,32]. While there have been no studies as yet directly comparing aggression rates or severity of dolphins in the wild versus in zoological facilities, as noted earlier, a large, multi-institutional collaborative research study found that aggression among bottlenose dolphins in modern facilities is in fact quite low [55].

4.3. Symptoms Characteristic of Depression

In humans, depression is a complex, variable disorder characterized by feelings of sadness, a loss of interest or pleasure, changes in weight or appetite, feeling fatigued, a decreased ability to concentrate, sleep problems, psychomotor agitation or retardation, low self-confidence, feelings of worthlessness and guilt, and recurrent thoughts of death or suicide [117,118]. While depressed individuals may not show evidence of all of these characteristics, a diagnosis of depression requires the presence of at least five of these characteristics co-occurring for an extended period of time (i.e., at least two weeks), and excluding any characteristics that are attributable to other physical or medical causes [117,118].
Although some of these diagnostic criteria can only be assessed via verbal self-report (e.g., low self-confidence, feelings of worthlessness) and are thus only knowable for humans, other criteria can be, and have been, objectively assessed in other species, leading to well-established indicators of possible depression in non-human animals [117]. Within this context, Jacobs et al.’s [18] claim that some cetaceans “exhibit symptoms characteristic of depression (e.g., logging on the surface, lying motionless on the bottom of the tank, and loss of appetite)” (p. 442) is problematic on several levels. First, to use any characteristic (e.g., inactivity, loss of appetite) as an indicator of possible depression in an individual, one would need prevalence rates of that characteristic for that individual. How frequently the behavior occurs is the relevant concern. Second, one would need to be reasonably certain that the characteristic in question is not attributable to physical causes such as illness or old age. This is particularly important given that these particular characteristics (i.e., decrease in activity, loss of appetite) are specifically known to develop with illness and/or age in dolphins and other species [119,120,121,122]. Third, there would need to be a co-occurrence of several diagnostic criteria in the same individual over the same time period. The practice of relying on single behaviors is a known problem in animal research on depression-like states, which risks mislabeling animals as depressed when they are not [117]. Finally, to credibly argue that this is a population-level issue for animals in modern zoological facilities, one would need to show that this was prevalent in most, or at least many, of the animals in that environment. Jacobs et al. [18] present no data that address any of these criteria.
However, even if appetite loss and inactivity (including both floating/logging on the surface and resting on the bottom) do not necessarily indicate depression, it is worth noting that both of these measures have been used to assess individual health and welfare in a variety of animals (e.g., [123,124,125]). With respect to dolphins, for example, welfare studies have shown that how often dolphins receive new enrichment is associated with their activity rates (including how often they interact with other dolphins, how much they swim, and how much energy they expend) [49,55]; that interacting with a caregiver can lead to increases in activity (including playing with toys more and floating in place less) [126]; and that decreased appetite may be associated with stressful situations, disease, or other medical conditions (e.g., [119,120,121,127]). Such measures can thus be extremely useful for assessing the welfare of individual dolphins. However, there are no data suggesting that these are population-wide problems in modern zoological facilities as Jacobs et al. [18] suggest.

5. Optimizing Cognitive Well-Being

Thus far, we have been examining the existing evidence relevant to assessing Jacob et al.’s [18] claim that conditions for dolphins in zoological facilities are inherently impoverished (and thus lead to neural deficits). The results of this analysis show that this claim is clearly false. The accreditation standards of modern professional zoological organizations, the results of a large multi-national study on cetacean welfare, and the behavior of dolphins in modern zoological facilities all point to the same conclusion that modern zoological facilities do not inherently, or even typically, house dolphins in impoverished conditions.
That said, however, “not impoverished” would be a particularly low bar to set as an animal welfare standard. As noted in the introduction, numerous studies have documented not only the negative effects of impoverished environments, but also the positive effects of enriched environments on the neural and cognitive functioning of a variety of animals (see reviews in, e.g., [19,20,21,22,23]). Further, we have seen that even among accredited zoological facilities, there can be variation in welfare outcomes (e.g., [41,49,55,99]). To optimize the welfare of animals in their care, then, zoological facilities would be well-served to pay specific attention to environmental promoters of animals’ cognitive well-being [91,128,129]. To that end, beyond the three foundational factors already discussed (i.e., social situation, enclosure size, and environmental complexity/variability; see Table 1), we would like to briefly review additional practices that have been suggested to positively impact cognitive welfare.

Beyond Simple Enrichment

Environmental enrichment can come in a variety of forms and complexity levels. For both aquatic and terrestrial animals, however, the most common type tends to be “toys” or other manipulable objects [42,45,91,130]. To be sure, many studies have shown positive benefits of this type of enrichment [81,82,131]. However, others have shown that such simple object enrichment may not be sufficient to produce long-lasting benefits due to animals’ lack of interest or habituation [132,133,134,135].
Beyond providing animals with objects, studies have shown that giving animals choices and increased control in their environment—such as the ability to choose whether to be inside or outside, or to control a light or a shower—can further improve their welfare [136,137,138,139]. With dolphins, this could be implemented by practices such as providing them with methods to explicitly choose between different training activities, enrichment items, types of reinforcers (e.g., food, rubs, toys), or to explicitly opt out of a particular activity [140,141,142].
Beyond choice and control, it has also been suggested that providing animals with situations and devices that challenge them cognitively could further improve their welfare [128,129,143,144]. Multiple studies have demonstrated that introducing cognitive challenges can lead to indications of improved welfare such as increases in movement, habitat utilization, and signs of positive excitement, along with decreases in stereotypies and other stress behaviors [145,146,147,148,149,150]. With dolphins, such cognitive challenges might come in a variety of forms, including cognitive research, puzzle devices, computer tasks, or “thinking games” (i.e., training of conceptual rules such as repeat, imitate, and innovate) [91,129,150,151,152,153,154].

6. Conclusions

In conclusion, the effects of the quality of the environment on animals’ cognitive and neural functioning are well-established. Numerous studies with numerous species have demonstrated detrimental effects when animals are housed in impoverished conditions [20,21,22]. The relevance of this concern to dolphins is supported by studies that have shown significant inactivity, aggression, and a head-pressing stereotypy when dolphins were kept in abnormally small, barren enclosures [44,53,89]. Thus, we agree with Jacobs et al. [18] on the importance of this concern. However, it is puzzling why, instead of pointing to these cases as individual welfare problems that clearly needed to be addressed, Jacobs et al. simply stipulate that zoos are inherently impoverished for dolphins, rather than thoughtfully engaging in the scientific question of whether this is true.
Indeed, when one does examine the data, one finds that the typical environment of dolphins in modern zoological facilities is in fact more accurately described as belonging to the standard-to-enriched side of the continuum, and that there is no evidence of wide-spread (or even common) behavioral characteristics that would suggest the effects of an inherently impoverished environment.
In addition to avoiding impoverished environments, multiple studies have documented the effects of enriching animals’ environments in various ways [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28]. The most recent addition to this literature is the suggestion that beyond complexity, environmental elements that provide cognitive challenges can further enhance animal welfare [128,129,143]. While there have been a few studies so far exploring the welfare effects of cognitive challenge with dolphins [153,155,156], further research is warranted to determine the characteristics that make effective cognitive challenges, and to explore positive welfare benefits that might encourage more widespread adoption of such experiences into standard zoological practice.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Emily Guarino and Sara Chi for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper. I thank Dolphin Quest, Sea World, Loro Parque, and American Humane for their sponsorship of this special edition.

Conflicts of Interest

The author is employed by the Dolphin Research Center.

References

  1. Albert, C.; Luque, G.M.; Courchamp, F. The Twenty Most Charismatic Species. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0199149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Driscoll, J.W. Attitudes toward Animals: Species Ratings. Soc. Anim. 1995, 3, 139–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ducarme, F.; Luque, G.M.; Courchamp, F. What Are “Charismatic Species” for Conservation Biologists? Biosci. Master Rev. 2013, 10, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  4. Leader-Williams, N.; Dublin, H.T. Charismatic Megafauna as “Flagship Species”. In Priorities for the Conservation of Mammalian Biodiversity: Has the Panda Had Its Day? Entwistle, A., Dunstone, N., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 53–81. [Google Scholar]
  5. Powell, D.M.; Bullock, E.V.W. Evaluation of Factors Affecting Emotional Responses in Zoo Visitors and the Impact of Emotion on Conservation Mindedness. Anthrozoös 2014, 27, 389–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Asper, E.D.; Duffield, D.A.; Dimeo-Ediger, N.; Shell, D. Marine Mammals in Zoos, Aquaria and Marine Zoological Parks in North America: 1990 Census Report. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2007, 29, 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Couquiaud, L. Whales, Dolphins, and Porpoises: Presentation of the Cetaceans. Aquat. Mamm. 2005, 31, 288–310. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bitgood, S.; Patterson, D.; Benefield, A. Exhibit Design and Visitor Behavior: Empirical Relationships. Environ. Behav. 1988, 20, 474–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Luebke, J.F.; Watters, J.V.; Packer, J.; Miller, L.J.; Powell, D.M. Zoo Visitors’ Affective Responses to Observing Animal Behaviors. Visit. Stud. 2016, 19, 60–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Moss, A.; Esson, M. Visitor Interest in Zoo Animals and the Implications for Collection Planning and Zoo Education Programmes. Zoo Biol. 2010, 29, 715–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Myers, O.E.; Saunders, C.D.; Birjulin, A.A. Emotional Dimensions of Watching Zoo Animals: An Experience Sampling Study Building on Insights from Psychology. Curator Mus. J. 2004, 47, 299–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jaakkola, K.; Willis, K. How Long Do Dolphins Live? Survival Rates and Life Expectancies for Bottlenose Dolphins in Zoological Facilities vs. Wild Populations. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2019, 35, 1418–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Proie, S. A Systematic Review of Cortisol Levels in Wild and Captive Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus), Killer Whale, (Orcinus orca), and Beluga Whale (Delphinapterus leucas). Master’s Thesis, Evergreen State College, Olympia, WA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  14. Aubin, D.J.S.; Ridgway, S.H.; Wells, R.S.; Rhinehart, H. Dolphin Thyroid and Adrenal Hormones: Circulating Levels in Wild and Semidomesticated Tursiops truncatus, and Influence of Sex, Age, and Season. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1996, 12, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Ridgway, S.H.; Benirschke, K. Breeding Dolphins: Present Status, Suggestions for the Future; National Technical Information Service: Springfield, VA, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  16. Urian, K.W.; Duffield, D.A.; Read, A.J.; Wells, R.S.; Shell, E.D. Seasonality of Reproduction in Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. J. Mammal. 1996, 77, 394–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mason, G.J. Species Differences in Responses to Captivity: Stress, Welfare and the Comparative Method. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2010, 25, 713–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Jacobs, B.; Rally, H.; Doyle, C.; O’Brien, L.; Tennison, M.; Marino, L. Putative Neural Consequences of Captivity for Elephants and Cetaceans. Rev. Neurosci. 2022, 33, 439–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Arai, J.A.; Feig, L.A. Long-Lasting and Transgenerational Effects of an Environmental Enrichment on Memory Formation. Brain Res. Bull. 2011, 85, 30–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Diamond, M.C. Enriching Heredity: The Impact of the Environment on the Anatomy of the Brain; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1988; ISBN 978-0-02-907431-2. [Google Scholar]
  21. Mohammed, A.H.; Zhu, S.W.; Darmopil, S.; Hjerling-Leffler, J.; Ernfors, P.; Winblad, B.; Diamond, M.C.; Eriksson, P.S.; Bogdanovic, N. Environmental Enrichment and the Brain. In Progress in Brain Research; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2002; Volume 138, pp. 109–133. ISBN 978-0-444-50981-9. [Google Scholar]
  22. Renner, M.J.; Rosenzweig, M.R. Enriched and Impoverished Environments Effects on Brain and Behavior; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  23. Segovia, G.; Arco, A.d.; Mora, F. Environmental Enrichment, Prefrontal Cortex, Stress, and Aging of the Brain. J. Neural Transm. 2009, 116, 1007–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Mellen, J.; MacPhee, M.S. Philosophy of Environmental Enrichment: Past, Present, and Future. Zoo Biol. 2001, 20, 211–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Markowitz, H. Behavioral Enrichment in the Zoo; Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York, NY, USA, 1982; ISBN 978-0-442-25125-3. [Google Scholar]
  26. Markowitz, H. Engineering Environments for Behavioral Opportunities in the Zoo. Behav. Anal. 1978, 1–2, 34–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shepherdson, D.J.; Mellen, J.D.; Hutchins, M. (Eds.) Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Zoo and Aquarium Biology and Conservation Series; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; ISBN 978-1-56098-397-2. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kemp, C. Enrichment. In Nonhuman Primate Welfare; Robinson, L.M., Weiss, A., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2023; pp. 463–500. ISBN 978-3-030-82707-6. [Google Scholar]
  29. AMMPA. AMMPA Accreditation Standards & Guidelines. Available online: https://www.ammpa.org/membership/standards-guidelines (accessed on 7 February 2022).
  30. AZA. The Accreditation Standards & Related Policies. Available online: https://www.aza.org/assets/2332/aza-accreditation-standards.pdf (accessed on 7 February 2022).
  31. EAAM. Standards and Guidelines for the Management of Aquatic Mammals Under Human Care. Available online: https://eaam.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/06/EAAM-Standards-and-guidelines-2019.pdf (accessed on 7 February 2022).
  32. EAZA. EAZA Standards for the Accommodation and Care of Animals in Zoos and Aquaria. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/assets/Uploads/Standards-and-policies/2020-10-EAZA-Standards-for-Accomodation-and-Care.pdf (accessed on 7 February 2022).
  33. Ceta-Base Bottlenose—Ceta-Base. Available online: https://www.cetabase.org/facility/tursiops/ (accessed on 8 April 2023).
  34. Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums Our Members|AMMPA. Available online: https://www.ammpa.org/about/our-members (accessed on 8 April 2023).
  35. Association of Zoos and Aquariums Institution Status: Association of Zoos & Aquariums. Available online: https://www.aza.org/inst-status?locale=en (accessed on 8 April 2023).
  36. European Association for Aquatic Mammals Accredited Institutional Members—EAAM. Available online: https://eaam.org/category/about-eaam/accredited-institutional-members/ (accessed on 8 April 2023).
  37. European Association of Zoos and Aquaria European Association of Zoos and Aquaria » EAZA. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/#map_home (accessed on 8 April 2023).
  38. Maple, T.L.; Perdue, B.M. Zoo Animal Welfare; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-3-642-35954-5. [Google Scholar]
  39. McPhee, M.E.; Carlstead, K. The Importance of Maintaining Natural Behaviors in Captive Mammals. In Wild Mammals in Captivity: Principles and Techniques for Zoo Management, 2nd ed.; Kleiman, D.G., Thompson, K.V., Baer, C.K., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 303–313. ISBN 978-0-226-44011-8. [Google Scholar]
  40. Morgan, K.N.; Tromborg, C.T. Sources of Stress in Captivity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 262–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lauderdale, L.K.; Mellen, J.D.; Walsh, M.T.; Granger, D.A.; Miller, L.J. Towards Understanding the Welfare of Cetaceans in Accredited Zoos and Aquariums. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0255506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Brando, S.; Broom, D.M.; Acasuso-Rivero, C.; Clark, F. Optimal Marine Mammal Welfare under Human Care: Current Efforts and Future Directions. Behav. Process. 2018, 156, 16–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Laule, G.E. Positive Reinforcement Training and Environmental Enrichment: Enhancing Animal Well-Being. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2003, 223, 969–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Bassos, M.K.; Wells, R.S. Effect of Pool Features on the Behavior of Two Bottlenose Dolphins. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1996, 12, 321–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lauderdale, L.K.; Walsh, M.T.; Mellen, J.D.; Granger, D.A.; Miller, L.J. Environmental Enrichment, Training, and Habitat Characteristics of Common Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and Indo-Pacific Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0253688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Blasio, A.L.; Pérez, R.V.; Pardo, M.R.; Maldonado, F.G. Maintenance Behaviour and Cortisol Levels in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncates) in Closed and Open Facilities. Vet. Mex. 2012, 43, 103–112. [Google Scholar]
  47. Ugaz, C.; Valdez, R.A.; Romano, M.C.; Galindo, F. Behavior and Salivary Cortisol of Captive Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) Kept in Open and Closed Facilities. J. Vet. Behav. 2013, 8, 285–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ugaz Ruiz, C.; Sánchez, A.; Maldonado, F.G. Social and Individual Behavior of a Group of Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Open and Closed Facilities. Vet. Mex. 2009, 40, 381–387. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lauderdale, L.K.; Shorter, K.A.; Zhang, D.; Gabaldon, J.; Mellen, J.D.; Walsh, M.T.; Granger, D.A.; Miller, L.J. Bottlenose Dolphin Habitat and Management Factors Related to Activity and Distance Traveled in Zoos and Aquariums. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0250687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Defran, R.H.; Weller, D.W.; Kelly, D.L.; Espinosa, M.A. Range Characteristics of Pacific Coast Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Southern California Bight. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1999, 15, 381–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Irvine, B.; Scott, M.D.; Wells, R.S.; Kaufmann, J.H. Movements and Activities of the Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin, Tursiops truncatus, near Sarasota, Florida. Fish. Bull. 1981, 79, 671–688. [Google Scholar]
  52. Mate, B.R.; Rossbach, K.A.; Nieukirk, S.L.; Wells, R.S.; Blair Irvine, A.; Scott, M.D.; Read, A.J. Satellite-Monitored Movements and Dive Behavior of a Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) in Tampa Bay, Florida. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1995, 11, 452–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Myers, W.A.; Overstrom, N.A. The Role of Daily Observation in the Husbandry of Captive Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Cetology 1978, 29, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  54. Overstrom, N.A. Association between Burst-Pulse Sounds and Aggressive Behavior in Captive Atlantic Bottlenosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biol. 1983, 2, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Miller, L.J.; Lauderdale, L.K.; Mellen, J.D.; Walsh, M.T.; Granger, D.A. Assessment of Animal Management and Habitat Characteristics Associated with Social Behavior in Bottlenose Dolphins across Zoological Facilities. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0253732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lauderdale, L.K.; Shorter, K.A.; Zhang, D.; Gabaldon, J.; Mellen, J.D.; Walsh, M.T.; Granger, D.A.; Miller, L.J. Habitat Characteristics and Animal Management Factors Associated with Habitat Use by Bottlenose Dolphins in Zoological Environments. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0252010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Nash, L.T.; Chilton, S.-M. Space or Novelty?: Effects of Altered Cage Size OnGalago Behavior. Am. J. Primatol. 1986, 10, 37–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ödberg, F.O. The Influence of Cage Size and Environmental Enrichment on the Development of Stereotypies in Bank Voles (Clethrionomys glareolus). Behav. Process. 1987, 14, 155–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sha, J.C.M.; Ismail, R.; Marlena, D.; Lee, J.L. Environmental Complexity and Feeding Enrichment Can Mitigate Effects of Space Constraints in Captive Callitrichids. Lab. Anim. 2016, 50, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Wilson, S.F. Environmental Influences on the Activity of Captive Apes. Zoo Biol. 1982, 1, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Couquiaud, L. Husbandry. Aquat. Mamm. 2005, 31, 371–381. [Google Scholar]
  62. Fernandez, E. Training as Enrichment: A Critical Review. Anim. Welf. 2022, 31, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Laule, G.; Desmond, T. Positive Reinforcement Training as an Enrichment Strategy. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Sheperdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Books: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; pp. 302–313. [Google Scholar]
  64. Perlman, J.E.; Horner, V.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J. Positive Reinforcement Training, Social Learning, and Chimpanzee Welfare. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives; Lonsdorf, E.V., Ross, S.R., Matsuzawa, T., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 320–332. [Google Scholar]
  65. Shepherdson, D.J. Tracing the Path of Environmental Enrichment in Zoos. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Shepherdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  66. Westlund, K. Training Is Enrichment—And Beyond. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 152, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ramirez, K. Animal Training: Successful Animal Management through Positive Reinforcement; Shedd Aquarium: Chicago, IL, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  68. Turner, T.; Feucht, T.; Turner, T. Training and Behavior Management. In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals; Elsevier/Academic Press: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  69. Kuczaj, S.A., II; Xitco, M.J., Jr. It Takes More than Fish: The Psychology of Marine Mammal Training. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2002, 15, 186–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Grant, R.A.; Warrior, J.R. Clicker Training Increases Exploratory Behaviour and Time Spent at the Front of the Enclosure in Shelter Cats: Implications for Welfare and Adoption Rates. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 211, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Mason, G.J. Stereotypies: A Critical Review. Anim. Behav. 1991, 41, 1015–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Ödberg, F.O. Abnormal Behaviours: Stereotypies. In Proceedings of the First World Congress on Ethology Applied to Zootechnics; Industrias Graficas Espana, Madrid, Spain, 23 October 1978; pp. 475–480. [Google Scholar]
  73. Henderson, V.; Waran, N.K. Reducing Equine Stereotypies Using an Equiball. Anim. Welf. 2001, 10, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lutz, C.; Well, A.; Novak, M. Stereotypic and Self-Injurious Behavior in Rhesus Macaques: A Survey and Retrospective Analysis of Environment and Early Experience. Am. J. Primatol. 2003, 60, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Carlstead, K.; Seidensticker, J. Seasonal Variation in Stereotypic Pacing in an American Black Bear Ursus americanus. Behav. Process. 1991, 25, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wechsler, B. Stereotypies in Polar Bears. Zoo Biol. 1991, 10, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Ödberg, F.O. The Jumping Stereotypy in the Bank Vole (Clethrionomys glareolus). Biol. Behav. 1986, 11, 130–143. [Google Scholar]
  78. Würbel, H.; Stauffacher, M.; Holst, D. Stereotypies in Laboratory Mice—Quantitative and Qualitative Description of the Ontogeny of ‘Wire-Gnawing’ and ‘Jumping’ in Zur:ICR and Zur:ICR Nu. Ethology 1996, 102, 371–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Broom, D.M. Stereotypies as Animal Welfare Indicators. In Indicators Relevant to Farm Animal WElfare; Schmidt, D., Ed.; Martinus Nijhoff: The Hague, The Netherlands, 1983; pp. 81–87. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mason, G.; Latham, N. Can’t Stop, Won’t Stop: Is Stereotypy a Reliable Animal Welfare Indicator? Anim. Welf. 2004, 13, S57–S69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Shyne, A. Meta-Analytic Review of the Effects of Enrichment on Stereotypic Behavior in Zoo Mammals. Zoo Biol. 2006, 25, 317–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Swaisgood, R.; Shepherdson, D. Environmental Enrichment as a Strategy for Mitigating Stereotypies in Zoo Animals: A Literature Review and Meta-Analysis. In Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare; Mason, G., Rushen, J., Eds.; CABI: Wallingford, CT, USA, 2006; pp. 256–285. ISBN 978-0-85199-004-0. [Google Scholar]
  83. Swaisgood, R.R.; Shepherdson, D.J. Scientific Approaches to Enrichment and Stereotypies in Zoo Animals: What’s Been Done and Where Should We Go Next? Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 499–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Broom, D.M. A History of Animal Welfare Science. Acta Biotheor. 2011, 59, 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Medina, L.V. Building a Culture of Animal Welfare: Past, Present and Future. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Sci. 2008, 10, T104–T111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Mellor, D.; Patterson-Kane, E.; Stafford, K.J. The Sciences of Animal Welfare; John Wiley & Sons: Oxford, UK, 2009; ISBN 978-1-4443-0769-6. [Google Scholar]
  87. Mellor, D.J.; Bayvel, A.C.D. The Application of Legislation, Scientific Guidelines and Codified Standards to Advancing Animal Welfare. In Proceedings of the Global Conference on Animal Welfare: An OIE Initiative (World Organisation for Animal Health), Paris, France, 23 February 2004; pp. 237–244. [Google Scholar]
  88. Powell, D.M.; Watters, J.V. The Evolution of the Animal Welfare Movement in U.S. Zoos and Aquariums. Zool. Gart. 2017, 86, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Greenwood, A.G. A Stereotyped Behaviour Pattern in Dolphins. Aquat. Mamm. 1977, 5, 15–17. [Google Scholar]
  90. Williamson, C. Marine Mammals: Guidelines and Criteria Associated with Captivity 2006. Available online: http://www.car-spaw-rac.org/IMG/pdf/OVERVIEW_CAPTIVITY_MARINE_MAMMALS_WCR.pdf (accessed on 12 April 2023).
  91. Clark, F.E. Marine Mammal Cognition and Captive Care: A Proposal for Cognitive Enrichment in Zoos and Aquariums. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2013, 1, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Gygax, L. Spatial Movement Patterns and Behaviour of Two Captive Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Absence of Stereotyped Behaviour or Lack of Definition? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993, 38, 337–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Serres, A.; Hao, Y.; Wang, D. Swimming Features in Captive Odontocetes: Indicative of Animals’ Emotional State? Behav. Process. 2020, 170, 103998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Sobel, N.; Supin, A.Y.; Myslobodsky, M.S. Rotational Swimming Tendencies in the Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behav. Brain Res. 1994, 65, 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Clegg, I.L.K.; Delfour, F. Can We Assess Marine Mammal Welfare in Captivity and in the Wild? Considering the Example of Bottlenose Dolphins. Aquat. Mamm. 2018, 44, 181–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Garner, J.P.; Meehan, C.L.; Mench, J.A. Stereotypies in Caged Parrots, Schizophrenia and Autism: Evidence for a Common Mechanism. Behav. Brain Res. 2003, 145, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Mason, G. Stereotypic Behaviour in Captive Animals: Fundamentals and Implications for Welfare and Beyond. In Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare; Mason, G., Rushen, J., Eds.; CABI Pub: Wallingford, UK; Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-0-85199-004-0. [Google Scholar]
  98. Miller, L.; Mellen, J.; Greer, T.; Kuczaj, S.A.I. The Effects of Education Programmes on Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Behaviour. Anim. Welf. 2011, 20, 159–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Miller, L.J.; Lauderdale, L.K.; Mellen, J.D.; Walsh, M.T.; Granger, D.A. Relationships between Animal Management and Habitat Characteristics with Two Potential Indicators of Welfare for Bottlenose Dolphins under Professional Care. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0252861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Makecha, R.N.; Highfill, L.E. Environmental Enrichment, Marine Mammals, and Animal Welfare: A Brief Review. Aquat. Mamm. 2018, 44, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Durden, W.N.; O’Corry-Crowe, G.; Shippee, S.; Jablonski, T.; Rodgers, S.; Mazzoil, M.; Howells, E.; Hartel, E.; Potgieter, B.; Londono, C.; et al. Small-Scale Movement Patterns, Activity Budgets, and Association Patterns of Radio-Tagged Indian River Lagoon Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Aquat. Mamm. 2019, 45, 66–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Powell, J.R.; Wells, R.S. Recreational Fishing Depredation and Associated Behaviors Involving Common Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Sarasota Bay, Florida. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2011, 27, 111–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Shane, S.H.; Wells, R.S.; Wursig, B. Ecology, behavior and social organization of the bottlenose dolphin: A review. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1986, 2, 34–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Coscarella, M.A.; Crespo, E.A. Feeding Aggregation and Aggressive Interaction between Bottlenose (Tursiops truncatus) and Commerson’s Dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) in Patagonia, Argentina. J. Ethol. 2010, 28, 183–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Dunn, D.G.; Barco, S.G.; Pabst, D.A.; McLellan, W.A. Evidence for Infanticide in Bottlenose Dolphins of the Western North Atlantic. J. Wildl. Dis. 2002, 38, 505–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Kaplan, J.D.; Lentell, B.J.; Lange, W. Possible Evidence for Infanticide among Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off St. Augustine, Florida. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2009, 25, 970–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Marley, S.A.; Cheney, B.; Thompson, P.M. Using Tooth Rakes to Monitor Population and Sex Differences in Aggressive Behaviour in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Aquat. Mamm. 2013, 39, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Parsons, K.M.; Durban, J.W.; Claridge, D.E. Male-Male Aggression Renders Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Unconscious. Aquat. Mamm. 2003, 29, 360–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Patterson, I.A.P.; Reid, R.J.; Wilson, B.; Grellier, K.; Ross, H.M.; Thompson, P.M. Evidence for Infanticide in Bottlenose Dolphins: An Explanation for Violent Interactions with Harbour Porpoises? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1998, 265, 1167–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Robinson, K.P. Agonistic Intraspecific Behavior in Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins: Calf-Directed Aggression and Infanticidal Tendencies by Adult Males. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2014, 30, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Ross, H.M.; Wilson, B. Violent Interactions between Bottlenose Dolphins and Harbour Porpoises. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1996, 263, 283–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Scott, E.M.; Mann, J.; Watson-Capps, J.J.; Sargeant, B.L.; Connor, R.C. Aggression in Bottlenose Dolphins: Evidence for Sexual Coercion, Male-Male Competition, and Female Tolerance through Analysis of Tooth-Rake Marks and Behaviour. Behaviour 2005, 142, 21–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Wedekin, L.L.; Daura-Jorge, F.G.; Simões-Lopes, P.C. An Aggressive Interaction Between Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and Estuarine Dolphins (Sotalia guianensis) in Southern Brazil. Aquat. Mamm. 2004, 30, 391–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Hrdy, S.B. Infanticide among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1979, 1, 13–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Smuts, B.B.; Smuts, R.W. Male Aggression and Sexual Coercion of Females in Nonhuman Primates and Other Mammals: Evidence and Theoretical Implications. In Advances in the Study of Behavior; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993; Volume 22, pp. 1–63. ISBN 978-0-12-004522-8. [Google Scholar]
  116. Fuxjager, M.J.; Zhao, X.; Rieger, N.S.; Marler, C.A. Why Animals Fight: Uncovering the Function and Mechanisms of Territorial Aggression. In APA handbook of Comparative Psychology: Basic Concepts, Methods, Neural Substrate, and Behavior; Call, J., Burghardt, G.M., Pepperberg, I.M., Snowdon, C.T., Zentall, T., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2017; pp. 853–875. ISBN 978-1-4338-2350-3. [Google Scholar]
  117. MacLellan, A.; Fureix, C.; Polanco, A.; Mason, G. Can Animals Develop Depression? An Overview and Assessment of ‘Depression-like’ States. Behaviour 2021, 158, 1303–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  119. Russell, J.P.; Osborn, S.D.; Ivančić, M.; Aristizabal-Henao, J.J.; Bowden, J.A.; Stedman, N.; Stacy, N.I. Chronic Nonchylous Lymphatic Pleural Effusion in a Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2022, 260, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Johnson, S.P.; Venn-Watson, S.K.; Cassle, S.E.; Smith, C.R.; Jensen, E.D.; Ridgway, S.H. Use of Phlebotomy Treatment in Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphins with Iron Overload. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2009, 235, 194–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Schmitt, T.L.; Sur, R.L. Treatment of Ureteral Calculus Obstruction with Laser Lithotripsy in an Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J. Zoo Wildl. Med. 2012, 43, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Ingram, D.K. Age-Related Decline in Physical Activity: Generalization to Nonhumans. Med.Sci. Sports Exerc. 2000, 32, 1623–1629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Fureix, C.; Meagher, R.K. What Can Inactivity (in Its Various Forms) Reveal about Affective States in Non-Human Animals? A Review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 8–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Stephen, J.M.; Ledger, R.A. An Audit of Behavioral Indicators of Poor Welfare in Kenneled Dogs in the United Kingdom. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2005, 8, 79–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Millman, S. Sickness Behaviour and Its Relevance to Animal Welfare Assessment at the Group Level. Anim. Welf. 2007, 16, 123–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Jaakkola, K.; Brignac, S.; Erb, L.; Guarino, E.; Haddock, A.; Rodriguez, A. Trainer Interaction Can Improve Welfare Outcomes of Toy Enrichment for Isolated Animals: A Case Study. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2023, 4, 72–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Waples, K.A.; Gales, N.J. Evaluating and Minimising Social Stress in the Care of Captive Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Zoo Biol. 2002, 21, 5–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Jaakkola, K.; Erb, L.; Ramirez, K. Beyond Physical Welfare: Addressing Marine Mammals’ Cognitive and Social Needs; International Marine Animal Trainers Association: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  129. Clark, F. Cognitive Enrichment and Welfare: Current Approaches and Future Directions. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2017, 4, 52–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. de Azevedo, C.S.; Cipreste, C.F.; Young, R.J. Environmental Enrichment: A GAP Analysis. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 329–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Li, T.-H.; Lim, E.; Chen, I.-C.; Lin, C.-F.; Chen, S.-C.; Tsai, M.-A. Environmental Enrichment and the Behavioral Effects on Captive Beluga Whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Thai J. Vet. Med. 2022, 52, 559–565. [Google Scholar]
  132. Delfour, F.; Beyer, H. Assessing the Effectiveness of Environmental Enrichment in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Effectiveness of Environmental Enrichment for Dolphins. Zoo Biol. 2012, 31, 137–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Line, S.W.; Morgan, K.N.; Markowitz, H. Simple Toys Do Not Alter the Behavior of Aged Rhesus Monkeys. Zoo Biol. 1991, 10, 473–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Crockett, C.M. Psychological Well-Being of Captive Nonhuman Primates. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Sheperdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; pp. 129–152. [Google Scholar]
  135. Kuczaj, S.; Lacinak, T.; Fad, O.; Trone, M. Keeping Environmental Enrichment Enriching. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2002, 15, 127–137. [Google Scholar]
  136. Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Badihi, I. The Psychology of Control: Effects of Control over Supplementary Light on Welfare of Marmosets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 137, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Legrand, A.; Schütz, K.E.; Tucker, C.B. Using Water to Cool Cattle: Behavioral and Physiological Changes Associated with Voluntary Use of Cow Showers. J. Dairy Sci. 2011, 94, 3376–3386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Owen, M.A.; Swaisgood, R.R.; Czekala, N.M.; Lindburg, D.G. Enclosure Choice and Well-Being in Giant Pandas: Is It All about Control? Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 475–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Ross, S.R. Issues of Choice and Control in the Behaviour of a Pair of Captive Polar Bears (Ursus maritimus). Behav. Process. 2006, 73, 117–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Bahe, H.; Broadway, M.; Hoffland, T.; Lyn, H. Choice and Control of Enrichment for a Rescued and Rehabilitated Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops trucatus); International Marine Animal Trainers Association: Orlando, FL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  141. Cruz, A.; Doshner, B.; Phillips, W. Increasing Motivation during Dolphin Interactive Programs by Utilizing, and Mixing, Concepts and Providing Participation Opportunities; International Marine Animal Trainers Association: Orlando, FL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  142. Rocho-Levine, J.R.; West, N.D. Designing Environmental Influence through Alternative Response Training (ART); International Marine Animal Trainers Association: San Diego, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  143. Meehan, C.L.; Mench, J.A. The Challenge of Challenge: Can Problem Solving Opportunities Enhance Animal Welfare? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 246–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Ward, S.J.; Sherwen, S.; Clark, F.E. Advances in Applied Zoo Animal Welfare Science. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2018, 21, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Murphy, D.E. Enrichment and Occupational Devices for Orangutans and Chimpanzees. Int. Zoo News 1976, 23, 24–26. [Google Scholar]
  146. Crockett, C.M.; Bellanca, R.U.; Heffernan, K.S.; Ronan, D.A.; Bonn, W.F. Puzzle Ball Foraging Device for Laboratory Monkeys. Lab. Primate Newsl. 2001, 40, 4–7. [Google Scholar]
  147. Puppe, B.; Ernst, K.; Schön, P.C.; Manteuffel, G. Cognitive Enrichment Affects Behavioural Reactivity in Domestic Pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 105, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. McGowan, R.T.S.; Rehn, T.; Norling, Y.; Keeling, L.J. Positive Affect and Learning: Exploring the “Eureka Effect” in Dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 577–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Yamanashi, Y.; Matsunaga, M.; Shimada, K.; Kado, R.; Tanaka, M. Introducing Tool-Based Feeders to Zoo-Housed Chimpanzees as a Cognitive Challenge: Spontaneous Acquisition of New Types of Tool Use and Effects on Behaviours and Use of Space. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2016, 4, 147–155. [Google Scholar]
  150. Lauderdale, L.; Miller, L. Efficacy of an Interactive Apparatus as Environmental Enrichment for Common Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Anim. Welf. 2020, 29, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Pryor, K.W.; Haag, R.; O’Reilly, J. The Creative Porpoise: Training for Novel Behavior. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1969, 12, 653–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Stevens, J.P.; Brignac, S.E.; Keaton, L. Dolphins Demonstrate the Understanding of an Abstract Idea. Soundings 2013, 38. [Google Scholar]
  153. Clark, F.E.; Davies, S.L.; Madigan, A.W.; Warner, A.J.; Kuczaj, S.A. Cognitive Enrichment for Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evaluation of a Novel Underwater Maze Device. Zoo Biol. 2013, 32, 608–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Amundin, M.; Starkhammar, J.; Evander, M.; Almqvist, M.; Lindström, K.; Persson, H.W. An Echolocation Visualization and Interface System for Dolphin Research. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2008, 123, 1188–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Clegg, I.L.K.; Domingues, M.; Ström, E.; Berggren, L. Cognitive Foraging Enrichment (but Not Non-Cognitive Enrichment) Improved Several Longer-Term Welfare Indicators in Bottlenose Dolphins. Animals 2023, 13, 238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Matrai, E.; Kwok, S.T.; Boos, M.; Pogány, Á. Testing Use of the First Multi-Partner Cognitive Enrichment Devices by a Group of Male Bottlenose Dolphins. Anim. Cogn. 2022, 25, 961–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Factors characterizing different environments on the impoverished/enriched continuum.
Table 1. Factors characterizing different environments on the impoverished/enriched continuum.
EnvironmentFactor
Social SituationEnclosure SizeEnvironmental
Complexity/
Variability
ImpoverishedAloneSmallBarren
Standard/controlWith othersSmall or mediumBarren
Enriched/complexWith othersLargeComplex/variable
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jaakkola, K. Are Dolphins Kept in Impoverished Environments? Animals 2023, 13, 2707. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172707

AMA Style

Jaakkola K. Are Dolphins Kept in Impoverished Environments? Animals. 2023; 13(17):2707. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172707

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jaakkola, Kelly. 2023. "Are Dolphins Kept in Impoverished Environments?" Animals 13, no. 17: 2707. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172707

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop