Adult Avoidant Attachment, Attention Bias, and Emotional Regulation Patterns: An Eye-Tracking Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Aims and Hypothesis
- (1)
- We hypothesize that, in general, the last fixation on care and its choice will be the more frequent pattern, and that the exposure to stressful conditions will induce a greater occurrence of this pattern, with respect to neutral conditions;
- (2)
- Regarding the role of avoidance, we hypothesize that there will be no differences in the last fixation on the care pictures between avoidant and low-avoidant individuals. Based on the dual-process model [23], we hypothesize that the attachment deactivation does not happen during the last picture fixation, but rather in the later stages of processing that lead to choice. We expect to find a lower consistency between the last fixation and choice of care, and thus a greater inconsistency in both conditions, i.e., avoidant individuals who last fixate on the care picture will then choose the alternative picture of food more frequently than low-avoidant individuals. Food pictures were selected as the alternative choice because of their enhanced attentional effect [36] and for their strong biological-reward function, irrespective of eating style [37];
- (3)
- With regard to the choice reaction time, we hypothesize that, when the last fixation is a care picture, the choice of care will be faster under threatening conditions. This is because care representation is mostly associated with the proximity-seeking behavior predicted by attachment theory, and thus with fewer elaborate processes;
- (4)
- Finally, we expect thats avoidant individuals will make their care-choice faster under both conditions, given their tendency to divert attention to negative and positive attachment-related information, and overall because of their defenses against the representations of care.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Questionnaires
2.2.2. Visual Stimuli
- (a)
- For emotional stimulation, the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) was used [40]. Forty IAPS pictures were selected for the threatening (e.g., accidents, human attack, dangerous animals), and neutral (e.g., domestic objects) conditions (Appendix A). The contents were associated consistently with higher or lower valence or arousal ratings. Mean ratings of valence and arousal were as per Lang [40] (using a 9-point Likert-like scale: 1 = low; negative). The forty pictures had a mean valence of 2.52 (SD = 0.64) and M = 5.11, (SD = 0.32); and a mean arousal of 6.62 (SD = 0.41) and M = 2.91 (SD = 0.68) for threatening and neutral conditions, respectively. Valence and arousal were significantly different between the two conditions, F(1, 38) = 264 and F(1, 38) = 438, respectively.
- (b)
- Forty pictures from the The Besançon Affective Picture Set-Adult (BAPS-Adult [41]), depicting comfort-related scenarios where care was represented, were used (Appendix A). Two random lists of twenty pictures were created: one for the threatening and one for the neutral condition. The mean ratings of perceived comfort, valence, and arousal were as per Szymanska [41]). The two lists did not significantly differ on any dimension, with all Fs < 1.
- (c)
- Forty pictures were taken from Food-Pics_Extended [42]. The pictures were of typical comfort foods (sweet and salty snacks) and were randomly divided into two lists of twenty items: one for the neutral and one for the threatening condition (Appendix A). Mean ratings of calories, palatability, and cravings were as per Blechert [42]. The two lists did not significantly differ on any dimension, with all Fs < 1.
- (d)
- For the twenty filler trials, sixty neutral pictures were selected from the IAPS and Food-Pics_Extended databases.
2.3. Procedure
“You will see neutral images and other images that will probably make you feel negative emotions, then you will have to choose one of the two images that follow, the one that at that time can help you to overcome the negative emotion of the single image you saw before”.
2.3.1. Eye Tracking
2.3.2. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. Limits and Future Directions
4.2. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Bowlby, J. Attachment. In Attachment and Loss, 2nd ed.; Basic Book: New York, NY, USA, 1982; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Long, M.; Verbeke, W.; Ein-Dor, T.; Vrtička, P. A Functional Neuro-Anatomical Model of Human Attachment (NAMA): Insights from First- and Second-Person Social Neuroscience. Cortex 2020, 126, 281–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J.; Webb, H.J.; Pepping, C.A.; Swan, K.; Merlo, O.; Skinner, E.A.; Avdagic, E.; Dunbar, M. Is parent–child attachment a correlate of children’s emotion regulation and coping? Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 41, 74–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waters, H.S.; Waters, E. The Attachment Working Models Concept: Among Other Things, We Build Script-like Representations of Secure Base Experiences. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2006, 8, 185–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dykas, M.J.; Woodhouse, S.S.; Jones, J.D.; Cassidy, J. Attachment-Related Biases in Adolescents’ Memory. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 2185–2201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ainsworth, M.D.S.; Blehar, M.C.; Waters, E.; Wall, S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Main, M. Cross-Cultural Studies of Attachment Organization: Recent Studies, Changing Methodologies, and the Concept of Conditional Strategies. Hum. Dev. 1990, 33, 48–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marks, M.J.; Vicary, A.M. The Interplay and Effectiveness of Implicit and Explicit Avoidant Defenses. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2016, 33, 619–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowlby, J. Loss, sadness and depression. In Attachment and Loss; Basic Book: New York, NY, USA, 1980; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
- Gillath, O.; Giesbrecht, B.; Shaver, P.R. Attachment, Attention, and Cognitive Control: Attachment Style and Performance on General Attention Tasks. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 647–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vrtička, P.; Vuilleumier, P. Neuroscience of Human Social Interactions and Adult Attachment Style. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vandevivere, E.; Braet, C.; Bosmans, G.; Mueller, S.C.; Raedt, R.D. Attachment and Children’s Biased Attentional Processing: Evidence for the Exclusion of Attachment-Related Information. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e103476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liu, Y.; Ding, Y.; Lu, L.; Chen, X. Attention Bias of Avoidant Individuals to Attachment Emotion Pictures. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 41631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhai, J.; Chen, X.; Ma, J.; Yang, Q.; Liu, Y. The Vigilance-Avoidance Model of Avoidant Recognition: An ERP Study under Threat Priming. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 246, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Szymanska, M.; Monnin, J.; Tio, G.; Vidal, C.; Girard, F.; Galdon, L.; Smith, C.C.; Bifulco, A.; Nezelof, S.; Vulliez-Coady, L. How Do Adolescents Regulate Distress According to Attachment Style? A Combined Eye-Tracking and Neurophysiological Approach. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2019, 89, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zheng, M.; Zhang, Y.; Zheng, Y. The Effects of Attachment Avoidance and the Defensive Regulation of Emotional Faces: Brain Potentials Examining the Role of Preemptive and Postemptive Strategies. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2015, 17, 96–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraley, R.C.; Niedenthal, P.M.; Marks, M.; Brumbaugh, C.; Vicary, A. Adult Attachment and the Perception of Emotional Expressions: Probing the Hyperactivating Strategies Underlying Anxious Attachment. J. Pers. 2006, 74, 1163–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Markus, A.M.; Bernier, A.; Pekrun, R.; Zimmermann, P.; Strasser, K.; Grossmann, K.E. Attachment State of Mind and Perceptual Processing of Emotional Stimuli. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2005, 7, 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niedenthal, P.M.; Brauer, M.; Robin, L.; Innes-Ker, Å.H. Adult Attachment and the Perception of Facial Expression of Emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 82, 419–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dan, O.; Raz, S. Adult Attachment and Emotional Processing Biases: An Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) Study. Biol. Psychol. 2012, 91, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanscartier, S.; Maxwell, J.A.; Lockwood, P. No Effect of Attachment Avoidance on Visual Disengagement from a Romantic Partner’s Face. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2020, 37, 2166–2183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Derakshan, N.; Eysenck, M.W.; Myers, L.B. Emotional Information Processing in Repressors: The Vigilance–Avoidance Theory. Cogn. Emot. 2007, 21, 1585–1614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chun, D.S.; Shaver, P.R.; Gillath, O.; Mathews, A.; Jorgensen, T.D. Testing a Dual-Process Model of Avoidant Defenses. J. Res. Pers. 2015, 55, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donges, U.-S.; Zeitschel, F.; Kersting, A.; Suslow, T. Adult Attachment Orientation and Automatic Processing of Emotional Information on a Semantic Level: A Masked Affective Priming Study. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 229, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oppenheimer, D.M.; Kelso, E. Information Processing as a Paradigm for Decision Making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 277–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ghaffari, M.; Fiedler, S. The Power of Attention: Using Eye Gaze to Predict Other-Regarding and Moral Choices. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 1878–1889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholz, A.; von Helversen, B.; Rieskamp, J. Eye Movements Reveal Memory Processes during Similarity- and Rule-Based Decision Making. Cognition 2015, 136, 228–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Weilbächer, R.A.; Krajbich, I.; Rieskamp, J.; Gluth, S. The Influence of Visual Attention on Memory-Based Preferential Choice. Cognition 2021, 215, 104804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kraemer, P.M.; Weilbacher, R.A.; Mechera-Ostrovsky, T.; Gluth, S. Cognitive and Neural Principles of a Memory Bias on Preferential Choices. Curr. Res. Neurobiol. 2022, 3, 100029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polanía, R.; Woodford, M.; Ruff, C.C. Efficient Coding of Subjective Value. Nat. Neurosci. 2019, 22, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ashby, N.J.S.; Johnson, J.G.; Krajbich, I.; Wedel, M. Applications and Innovations of Eye-movement Research in Judgment and Decision Making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 29, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Orquin, J.L.; Loose, S.M. Attention and Choice: A Review on Eye Movements in Decision Making. Acta Psychol. 2013, 144, 190–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krajbich, I.; Armel, C.; Rangel, A. Visual Fixations and the Computation and Comparison of Value in Simple Choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2010, 13, 1292–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krajbich, I.; Rangel, A. Multialternative Drift-Diffusion Model Predicts the Relationship between Visual Fixations and Choice in Value-Based Decisions. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 13852–13857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Smith, S.M.; Krajbich, I. Attention and Choice Across Domains. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2018, 147, 1810–1826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kumar, S.; Higgs, S.; Rutters, F.; Humphreys, G.W. Biased towards Food: Electrophysiological Evidence for Biased Attention to Food Stimuli. Brain Cogn. 2016, 110, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kirsten, H.; Seib-Pfeifer, L.-E.; Koppehele-Gossel, J.; Gibbons, H. Food Has the Right of Way: Evidence for Prioritised Processing of Visual Food Stimuli Irrespective of Eating Style. Appetite 2019, 142, 104372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraley, R.C.; Waller, N.G.; Brennan, K.A. An Item Response Theory Analysis of Self-Report Measures of Adult Attachment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 350–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Busonera, A.; Martini, P.S.; Zavattini, G.C.; Santona, A. Psychometric Properties of an Italian Version of the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) Scale. Psychol. Rep. 2014, 114, 785–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual; Technical Report A-8; The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Szymanska, M.; Comte, A.; Tio, G.; Vidal, C.; Monnin, J.; Smith, C.C.; Nezelof, S.; Vulliez-Coady, L. The Besançon Affective Picture Set-Adult (BAPS-Adult): Development and Validation. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 271, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blechert, J.; Lender, A.; Polk, S.; Busch, N.A.; Ohla, K. Food-Pics_Extended—An Image Database for Experimental Research on Eating and Appetite: Additional Images, Normative Ratings and an Updated Review. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peirce, J.; Gray, J.R.; Simpson, S.; MacAskill, M.; Höchenberger, R.; Sogo, H.; Kastman, E.; Lindeløv, J.K. PsychoPy2: Experiments in Behavior Made Easy. Behav. Res. Methods 2019, 51, 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Uccula, A.; Enna, M.; Treccani, B. Compatibility between Response Position and Either Object Typical Size or Semantic Category: SNARC- and MARC-like Effects in Primary School Children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2020, 189, 104682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, B.A.; Avivi-Reich, M.; Mozuraitis, M. A Cautionary Note on the Use of the Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) in Classification Designs with and without within-Subject Factors. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wilkinson, L.L.; Rowe, A.C.; Robinson, E.; Hardman, C.A. Explaining the Relationship between Attachment Anxiety, Eating Behaviour and BMI. Appetite 2018, 127, 214–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Evers, C.; Dingemans, A.; Junghans, A.F.; Boevé, A. Feeling Bad Or Feeling Good, Does Emotion Affect Your Consumption of Food? A Meta-Analysis of The Experimental Evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 92, 195–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hardman, C.A.; Jones, A.; Burton, S.; Duckworth, J.J.; McGale, L.S.; Mead, B.R.; Roberts, C.A.; Field, M.; Werthmann, J. Food-Related Attentional Bias and Its Associations with Appetitive Motivation and Body Weight: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Appetite 2020, 157, 104986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faber, A.; Dubé, L.; Knäuper, B. Attachment and Eating: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Relevance of Attachment for Unhealthy and Healthy Eating Behaviors in the General Population. Appetite 2018, 123, 410–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Favieri, F.; Marini, A.; Casagrande, M. Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behaviors in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dewitte, M.; Koster, E.H.W. Attentional Breadth and Proximity Seeking in Romantic Attachment Relationships. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 53, 74–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shaver, M. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change, 2nd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 9781462533817. [Google Scholar]
- Wichmann, T.; Buchheim, A.; Menning, H.; Schenk, I.; George, C.; Pokorny, D. A Reaction Time Experiment on Adult Attachment: The Development of a Measure for Neurophysiological Settings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Andriopoulos, P.; Kafetsios, K. Avoidant Attachment and the Processing of Emotion Information: Selective Attention or Cognitive Avoidance? J. Relatsh. Res. 2015, 6, e6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koehn, A.J.; Kerns, K.A. Parent–Child Attachment: Meta-Analysis of Associations with Parenting Behaviors in Middle Childhood and Adolescence. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2018, 20, 378–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dykas, M.J.; Cassidy, J. Attachment and the Processing of Social Information Across the Life Span: Theory and Evidence. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, 19–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Patterns of Choice | |||
---|---|---|---|
Variables | Care-Inconsistency | Food-Inconsistency | Food-Consistency |
BMI | 0.144 | 0.153 | −0.143 |
Restrained eating | −0.077 | −0.147 | 0.081 |
Hunger | 0.066 | 0.031 | 0.098 |
Neutral | Threatening | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Care | Food | Diff | Care | Food | Diff. | |
Last Fixation | 9.99 (3.24) (49.95%) | 10.01 (3.24) (50.05%) | 0.02 | 12.04 (2.75) (60.2%) | 7.96 (2.75) (39.8%) | 3.08 |
Choice | 8.93 (4.81) (44.65%) | 11.07 (4.81) (55.35%) | 2.14 | 14.54 (4.55) (72.7%) | 5.46 (4.55) (27.3%) | 9.08 |
RT Choice (ms) | 2890.29 (1820.13) | 2488.79 (1432.06) | 401.5 | 2441.09 (1605.88) | 2831.88 (1747.63) | −318.41 |
Total | Neutral | Threatening | ANCOVA | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Patterns | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | F |
Care—consistent choice | 17.02 | 6.88 | 6.82 | 4.15 | 10.19 | 3.93 | 44.990 *** |
Care—inconsistent choice | 5.01 | 4.63 | 3.17 | 2.71 | 1.85 | 2.49 | 22.536 *** |
Food—inconsistent choice | 6.46 | 4.26 | 2.11 | 2.15 | 4.35 | 2.98 | 40.142 *** |
Food—consistent choice | 11.51 | 5.65 | 7.90 | 3.98 | 3.61 | 2.92 | 78.396 *** |
Total | 40.00 | 20.00 | 20.00 |
Neutral | Threatening | ANCOVA | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Patterns | M | SD | M | SD | F |
RT Care—consistent choice | 2787 | 1641 | 2438 | 1552 | 17.769 *** |
RT Care—inconsistent choice | 2401 | 1677 | 2657 | 2309 | <1 |
RT Food—inconsistent choice | 2552 | 2000 | 2233 | 1791 | 6.005 * |
RT Food—consistent choice | 2439 | 1377 | 2830 | 1694 | 6.398 * |
Total | 2602 | 1502 | 2518 | 1595 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Uccula, A.; Mercante, B.; Barone, L.; Enrico, P. Adult Avoidant Attachment, Attention Bias, and Emotional Regulation Patterns: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010011
Uccula A, Mercante B, Barone L, Enrico P. Adult Avoidant Attachment, Attention Bias, and Emotional Regulation Patterns: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(1):11. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010011
Chicago/Turabian StyleUccula, Arcangelo, Beniamina Mercante, Lavinia Barone, and Paolo Enrico. 2023. "Adult Avoidant Attachment, Attention Bias, and Emotional Regulation Patterns: An Eye-Tracking Study" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 1: 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010011
APA StyleUccula, A., Mercante, B., Barone, L., & Enrico, P. (2023). Adult Avoidant Attachment, Attention Bias, and Emotional Regulation Patterns: An Eye-Tracking Study. Behavioral Sciences, 13(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010011