Food Restriction in Anorexia Nervosa in the Light of Modern Learning Theory: A Narrative Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method
3. Basic Concepts of Learning Theory
How Cognitive Determinants Are Treated in Modern Learning Theory
4. Progression of the Learning Models for Anorexia Nervosa
5. Introduction to a New Approach: Associative Analysis of Pathological Food Rejection
5.1. Basic Processes Underlying Food Rejection Motivated by Fear
5.1.1. Pavlovian Fear Reactions in Anorexia Nervosa
5.1.2. Instrumental Goal-Directed Avoidance in Anorexia Nervosa
5.1.3. Habitual Avoidance in Anorexia Nervosa
5.2. Basic Processes Underlying Food Rejection Motivated by Taste Aversion
Disgust and Flavour Aversions in Eating Disorders
5.3. Multiple and Different Associations to Explain Anorexia Nervosa Subtypes
6. Abnormal Associative Processes and Vulnerability in Anorexia Nervosa
7. Future Directions and Clinical Implications
8. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Author | Sample and Disorder Category | Type of Psychopathology Research | Learning Theory Concept | Measure of Food Consumption |
---|---|---|---|---|
Agras, Barlow, Chapin, Abel and Leitenberg [140] | 5 females (10–41 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research; single-subject experiments | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning, informational feedback) | Yes: size of the meal |
Annus, Smith, Fischer, Hendricks and Williams [141] | 66 females (n.i.; undergraduate): 34 healthy individuals and 32 with disordered eating behaviours | Descriptive research; correlational study | Modelling; social reinforcement; classical conditioning | No |
Azerrad and Stafford [142] | 1 female (13 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research; single-subject experiment | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: caloric value of the food eaten |
Belke, Pierce and Duncan [143] | 25 rats (5–12 months old): 10 males and 15 females: activity-based anorexia model | Experimental research; preclinical animal study | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: food intake |
Bhanji and Thompson [144] | 11 females (13–21 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: food intake |
Bianco [145] | 2 females (16 and 22 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Blinder, Freeman and Stunkard [146] | 3 females (15–22 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Blue [147] | 1 female (15 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning; punishment with aversive control) | No |
Boey [148] | 1 female (21 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Bruch [149] | 3 females (17–20 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Cardi., Leppanen, Mataix-Cols, Campbell and Treasure [46] | 18 females (19–60 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Classical conditioning (fear conditioning); extinction (inhibitory learning) | No |
Combs, Smith, Flory, Simmons and Hill [150] | 394 females (average 12.84 yo): healthy participants | Non-patient research; longitudinal study | Social learning; learned vulnerability; instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Coniglio, Becker, Franko, Zayas, Plessow, Eddy and Thomas [85] | 76 females and 2 males (average 25.13 yo): AN and atypical AN | Descriptive research; correlational study | Instrumental conditioning (stimulus-response, habitual learning; goal-directed learning); classical conditioning | No |
de Paz, Vidal and Pellón [151] | 24 male Wistar rats (43 weeks old): activity-based AN model | Experimental research; preclinical animal study | Classical conditioning (taste-conditioned aversion); instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: food intake |
Eckert, Goldberg, Halmi, Casper and Davis [152] | 81 patients (n.i.): AN | Experimental research; randomised controlled clinical trial | Classical conditioning (systematic desensitisation); instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Seidel, Boehm and Smolka [153] | 60 females: 30 recovered AN (15–28 yo) and 30 controls (15–27 yo) | Quasi-experimental research | Instrumental conditioning (instrumental motivation task) | No |
Elkin, Hersen, Eisler and Williams [154] | 1 male (24 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research; single-subject experiments | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning, informational feedback) | Yes: caloric intake |
Erwin [155] | 1 female (37 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcing learning) | No |
Foerde and Steinglass [115] | 62 females (16–45 yo): 36 AN and 26 healthy controls | Quasi-experimental research | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcing learning, feedback information) | No |
Frank, Collier, Shott and O’Reilly [156] | 48 females: 24 AN (average 30.3 yo) and 24 healthy controls (average 27.4 yo) | Quasi-experimental research; cross-sectional study | Classical conditioning (food-cue learning) | No |
Frank, DeGuzman, Shott, Laudenslager, Rossi and Pryor [157] | 108 females: 56 AN (average 16.6 yo) and 52 healthy controls (average16.0 yo) | Quasi-experimental research; cross-sectional study | Classical conditioning (food-cue learning) | No |
Frank, Reynolds, Shott, Jappe, Yang, Tregellas and O’Reilly [158] | 63 females: 21 AN (average 22.5 yo), 19 obese (average 27.1) and 23 healthy controls (average 24.8) | Quasi-experimental research; cross-sectional study | Classical conditioning (food-cue learning) | No |
Garfinkel, Kline and Stancer [159] | 5 females (13–19 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcing learning) | Yes: daily food intake of 3000 cal |
Glashouwer, Neimeijer, de Koning, Vestjens and Martijn [160] | 51 females (average 16.73 yo): 15 AN-R, 5 AN-P, 7 atypical AN, 9 BN and 15 other eating disorder | Experimental research: randomised clinical trial | Classical conditioning (evaluative learning) | No |
Godier, de Wit, Pinto, Steinglass, Greene, Scaife and Park [77] | 85 females: 36 AN (average 28.58 yo), 14 recovered AN (average 27.07 yo) and 35 healthy controls (average 27.92 yo) | Quasi-experimental research | Instrumental conditioning (goal-directed learning, habit learning) | No |
Hallsten [161] | 1 female (12 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Classical conditioning (systematic desensitisation) | No |
Halmi, Powers and Cunningham, [162] | 8 females (14–54 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcing learning) | Yes: total calories offered per day |
Hildebrandt, Grotzinger, Reddan, Greif, Levy, Goodman and Schiller [93] | 29 females (11–22 yo): 14 AN and 15 healthy controls | Quasi-experimental research | Classical conditioning (disgust conditioning) | No |
Hurel, Redon, Scocard, Malezieux, Giovanni and Chaouloff [163] | (n.i) males and females (3 and 8 weeks old): C57BL/6N mice; activity-based AN model | Experimental research; preclinical animal study | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: food intake |
Kerr, Moseman, Avery, Bodurka and Simmons [164] | 44 females: 20 AN (13–24 yo) and 20 controls (13–23 yo) | Quasi-experimental research | Classical conditioning (food aversion learning) | No |
Kissel and Arkins [165] | 1 male (16 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Klein, Schebendach, Gershkovich, Bodell, Foltin and Walsh [166] | 16 females (average 27 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Lascelles, Field and Davey [106] | 141 females (average 24.1 yo): healthy participants | Experimental research: preclinical human studies | Classical conditioning (evaluative learning) | No |
Leitenberg, Agras, and Thomson [167] | 2 females (14 and 17 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: daily food intake of calories |
Monti, McCrady and Barlow [168] | 1 female (28 yo): BN-AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning, informational feedback) | Yes: caloric intake |
Munford, Tarlow and Gerner [169] | 1 female (23 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research; single-subject experiments | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Murphy, Nutzinger, Paul and Leplow [170] | 48 females: 16 AN (average 22.3 yo), 16 BN (average 22.0 yo) and 16 healthy controls (25.3 yo) | Quasi-experimental research | Conditional-associative learning | No |
Olatunji [120] | 37 females: 19 individuals high and 18 low in eating disorder symptoms | Quasi-experimental research; preclinical analogue study | Classical conditioning (food aversion learning); extinction learning | Yes: daily calorie intake |
Olsavsky, Shott, DeGuzman and Frank [171] | 111 females (average 26 yo): 28 AN; 20 recovered AN, 20 BN and 43 healthy controls | Quasi-experimental research | Classical conditioning (food-cue learning) | No |
Perkin and Surtees [172] | 1 female (18 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Poole, Sanson-Fisher and Young [173] | 5 females (15- yo): AN | Descriptive research; case series | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Rowland, Cervantez and Robertson [174] | 12 male and 11 female (28 weeks old) C57BL/6 mice; restricted access to food-based AN model | Experimental research; preclinical animal study | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | Yes: food intake |
Schebendach, Klein, Foltin, Devlin and Walsh [175] | 16 females (18–45 yo): AN | Quasi-experimental research | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning with progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement) | Yes: daily calorie intake |
Steele [176] | 1 female (19 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Stumphauzer [177] | 1 female (23 yo): AN | Descriptive research; case report | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Touyz, Beumont, Glaun, Phillips and Cowie [178] | 63 females and 2 males (13–35 yo): AN | Descriptive research; cohort study | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning) | No |
Vandereycken and Pieters [179] | 32 females (14–42 yo): AN | Experimental research; controlled clinical trials | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning; punishment) | Yes: daily food intake of 2500–3000 cal |
Vogel, Dittrich, Horndasch, Kratz, Moll, Erim and Steins-Loeber [65] | 80 females (14–34 yo): 39 AN and 41 healthy controls | Quasi-experimental research | Classical and instrumental conditioning (Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer) | No |
Wulliemier [180] | 12 females (n.i.): 6 AN and 6 healthy controls | Experimental research: clinical trial | Instrumental conditioning (reinforcement learning, avoidance learning) | No |
References
- DeWitt, J.; Attia, E. Anorexia Nervosa. In Encyclopedia of Feeding and Eating Disorders; Wade, T., Ed.; Springer: Singapore, 2017; pp. 13–16. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Galmiche, M.; Déchelotte, P.; Lambert, G.; Tavolacci, M.P. Prevalence of eating disorders over the 2000–2018 period: A systematic literature review. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 1402–1413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smink, F.R.; van Hoeken, D.; Hoek, H.W. Epidemiology, course, and outcome of eating disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2013, 26, 543–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arcelus, J.; Mitchell, A.J.; Wales, J.; Nielsen, S. Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders: A meta-analysis of 36 studies. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2011, 68, 724–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Keski-Rahkonen, A.; Mustelin, L. Epidemiology of eating disorders in Europe. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2016, 29, 340–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Khalsa, S.S.; Portnoff, L.C.; McCurdy-McKinnon, D.; Feusner, J.D. What happens after treatment? A systematic review of relapse, remission, and recovery in anorexia nervosa. J. Eat. Disord. 2017, 5, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gibson, E.L.; Brunstrom, J.M. Learned influences on appetite, food choice and intake: Evidence in human beings. In Appetite and Body Weight: Integrative Systems and the Development of Anti-Obesity Drugs; Kirkham, T.C., Cooper, S.J., Eds.; Academic Press: Billington, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 271–300. [Google Scholar]
- Treasure, J.; Cardi, V.; Kan, C. Eating in eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2012, 20, e42–e49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davey, G.C.L. Learning theory. In Comprehensive Clinical Psychology: Foundations of Clinical Psychology; Walker, C.E., Ed.; Elsevier Science: Oxford, UK, 1998; pp. 391–421. [Google Scholar]
- Haselgrove, M.; Hogarth, L. Clinical Applications of Learning Theory; Psychology Press: Abingdon, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schachtman, T.R.; Reilly, S.S. Things you always wanted to know about conditioning but were afraid to ask. In Conditioning and Animal Learning: Human and Non-Human Applications; Schachtman, T., Reilly, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet, B.; Raes, F. Criteria of validity in experimental psychopathology: Application to models of anxiety and depression. Psychol. Med. 2012, 43, 2241–2244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nees, F.; Heinrich, A.; Flor, H. A mechanism-oriented approach to psychopathology: The role of Pavlovian conditioning. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2015, 98, 351–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jansen, A. Eating disorders need more experimental psychopathology. Behav. Res. Ther. 2016, 86, 2–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wildes, J.E.; Marcus, M.D. Application of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Framework to Eating Disorders: Emerging Concepts and Research. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015, 17, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waller, G.; Raykos, B. Behavioral Interventions in the Treatment of Eating Disorders. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2019, 42, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Butler, R.M.; Heimberg, R.G. Exposure therapy for eating disorders: A systematic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 78, 101851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koskina, A.; Campbell, I.C.; Schmidt, U. Exposure therapy in eating disorders revisited. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bower, G.H.; Hilgard, E.R. Theories of Learning, 5th ed.; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Mowrer, R.R.; Klein, S.B. Handbook of Contemporary Learning Theories; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner, B.F. The behavior of organisms; Appleton-Century: New York, NY, USA, 1938. [Google Scholar]
- Tolman, E.C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 1948, 55, 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rescorla, R.A. Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. Am. Psychol. 1988, 43, 151–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arntz, A. A plea for more attention to mental representations. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2019, 67, 101510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delamater, A.R.; Oakeshott, S. Learning about Multiple Attributes of Reward in Pavlovian Conditioning. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2007, 1104, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mowrer, R.R.; Klein, S.B. A contrast between traditional and contemporary learning theory. In Contemporary Learning Theories; Klein, S.B., Mowrer, R.R., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1989; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Howard, R.W. Reconceptualizing learning. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1999, 3, 251–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shafran, R.; Teachman, B.A.; Kerry, S.; Rachman, S. A cognitive distortion associated with eating disorders: Thought-shape fusion. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1999, 38, 167–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baumeister, R.F.; von Hippel, W. Meaning and evolution: Why Nature selected human minds to use meaning. Evol. Stud. Imaginative Cult. 2020, 4, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, J.; Vandorpe, S.; Beckers, T. On the role of controlled cognitive processes in human associative learning. In New Directions in Human associative Learning; Wills, A.J., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 41–63. [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Burgos, D. Associative learning and high-level cognitive processes in the control of food-related behaviors. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2022, 47, 101207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wardle, J. Cognitive control of eating. J. Psychosom. Res. 1988, 32, 607–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Polivy, J.; Herman, C.P. Dieting and binging: A causal analysis. Am. Psychol. 1985, 40, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buree, B.U.; Papageorgis, D.; Hare, R.D. Eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: An application of the tripartite model of anxiety. Can. J. Behav. Sci. Rev. Can. Sci. Comport. 1990, 22, 207–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slade, P. Towards a functional analysis of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 21, 167–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crisp, A.H. Anorexia nervosa, ‘feeding disorder’, ‘nervous malnutrition’, or ‘weight phobia’? World Rev. Nutr. Diet. 1970, 12, 452–504. [Google Scholar]
- Habermas, T. In defense of weight phobia as the central organizing motive in anorexia nervosa: Historical and cultural arguments for a culture-sensitive psychological conception. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 1996, 19, 317–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mowrer, O.H.; Lamoreaux, R.R. Fear as an intervening variable in avoidance conditioning. J. Comp. Psychol. 1946, 39, 29–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cain, C.K. Avoidance problems reconsidered. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 26, 9–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillan, C.M.; Urcelay, G.P.; Robbins, T.W. An associative account of avoidance. In The Wiley Handbook on the Cognitive Neuroscience of Learning; Murphy, R.A., Honey, R.C., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell Chichester: West Sussex, UK, 2016; pp. 442–467. [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux, J.E.; Moscarello, J.; Sears, R.; Campese, V. The birth, death and resurrection of avoidance: A reconceptualization of a troubled paradigm. Mol. Psychiatry 2016, 22, 24–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hildebrandt, T.; Bacow, T.; Markella, M.; Loeb, K.L. Anxiety in anorexia nervosa and its management using family-based treatment. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2010, 20, e1–e16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steinglass, J.E.; Sysko, R.; Glasofer, D.; Albano, A.M.; Simpson, H.B.; Walsh, B.T. Rationale for the application of exposure and response prevention to the treatment of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2011, 44, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strober, M. Pathologic fear conditioning and anorexia nervosa: On the search for novel paradigms. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2004, 35, 504–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cardi, V.; Leppanen, J.; Mataix-Cols, D.; Campbell, I.C.; Treasure, J. A case series to investigate food-related fear learning and extinction using in vivo food exposure in anorexia nervosa: A clinical application of the inhibitory learning framework. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2019, 27, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brytek-Matera, A.; Czepczor, K. Models of eating disorders: A theoretical investigation of abnormal eating patterns and body image disturbance. Arch. Psychiatry Psychother. 2017, 19, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holland, P.C.; Wheeler, D.S. Representation-mediated food aversions. In Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes; Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009; pp. 196–225. [Google Scholar]
- Affenito, S.G.; Dohm, F.-A.; Crawford, P.B.; Daniels, S.R.; Striegel-Moore, R.H. Macronutrient intake in anorexia nervosa: The national heart, lung, and blood institute growth and health study. J. Pediatr. 2002, 141, 701–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hadigan, C.M.; Anderson, E.J.; Miller, K.K.; Hubbard, J.L.; Herzog, D.B.; Klibanski, A.; Grinspoon, S.K. Assessment of macronutrient and micronutrient intake in women with anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2000, 28, 284–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nova, E.; Varela, P.; López-Vidriero, I.; Toro, O.; Ceñal, M.; Casas, J.; Marcos, A. A one-year follow-up study in anorexia nervosa. Dietary pattern and anthropometrical evolution. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2001, 55, 547–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mayer, L.E.; Schebendach, J.; Bodell, L.P.; Shingleton, R.M.; Walsh, B.T. Eating behavior in anorexia nervosa: Before and after treatment. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2011, 45, 290–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Walsh, B.T. The importance of eating behavior in eating disorders. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 104, 525–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hall, G. Associative structures in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. In Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology; Gallistel, C.R., Ed.; John Wiley and Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, J.; Tackett, J.; Carter, J. Conditioning is more than association formation: On the different ways in which conditioning research is valuable for clinical psychology. Collabra Psychol. 2020, 6, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parker, L.A. Taste avoidance and taste aversion: Evidence for two different processes. Anim. Learn. Behav. 2003, 31, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Parker, L.A. Aversion learning relationship to nausea and conditioned disgust. In The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning; McSweeney, F.K., Murphy, E.S., Eds.; John Wiley and Sons: Malden, MA, USA, 2014; pp. 97–116. [Google Scholar]
- Reilly, S.; Schachtman, T.R. Conditioned Taste Aversion: Neural and Behavioral Processes; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Schier, L.A.; Hyde, K.M.; Spector, A.C. Conditioned taste aversion versus avoidance: A re-examination of the separate processes hypothesis. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0217458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Krypotos, A.-M.; Effting, M.; Kindt, M.; Beckers, T. Avoidance learning: A review of theoretical models and recent developments. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Guarda, A.S.; Schreyer, C.C.; Boersma, G.J.; Tamashiro, K.L.; Moran, T.H. Anorexia nervosa as a motivated behavior: Relevance of anxiety, stress, fear and learning. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 152, 466–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lloyd, E.; Frampton, I.; Verplanken, B.; Haase, A. How extreme dieting becomes compulsive: A novel hypothesis for the role of anxiety in the development and maintenance of anorexia nervosa. Med. Hypotheses 2017, 108, 144–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bolles, R.C. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol. Rev. 1970, 77, 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Wit, S.; Dickinson, A. Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: A case for animal–human translational models. Psychol. Res. 2009, 73, 463–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vogel, V.; Dittrich, M.; Horndasch, S.; Kratz, O.; Moll, G.H.; Erim, Y.; Paslakis, G.; Rauh, E.; Steins-Loeber, S. Pavlovian-to-instrumental-transfer in Anorexia Nervosa: A pilot-study on conditioned learning and instrumental responding to low-and high-calorie food stimuli. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 51, 1794–1805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Campese, V.D. The Lesser Evil: Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer & Aversive Motivation. Behav. Brain Res. 2021, 412, 113431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinson, C.A.; Rodebaugh, T.L.; Fewell, L.; Kass, A.E.; Riley, E.N.; Stark, L.; McCallum, K.; Lenze, E.J. D-Cycloserine facilitation of exposure therapy improves weight regain in patients with anorexia nervosa: A pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2015, 76, e787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, S.B.; Treanor, M.; Liao, B.; Loeb, K.L.; Griffiths, S.; Le Grange, D. Extinction theory & anorexia nervosa: Deepening therapeutic mechanisms. Behav. Res. Ther. 2016, 87, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, S.B.; Strober, M.; Craske, M.G.; Griffiths, S.; Levinson, C.A.; Strigo, I.A. Fear as a translational mechanism in the psychopathology of anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 95, 383–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reilly, E.E.; Anderson, L.M.; Gorrell, S.; Schaumberg, K.; Anderson, D.A. Expanding exposure-based interventions for eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2017, 50, 1137–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinglass, J.E.; Albano, A.M.; Simpson, H.B.; Wang, Y.; Zou, J.; Attia, E.; Walsh, B.T. Confronting fear using exposure and response prevention for anorexia nervosa: A randomized controlled pilot study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2014, 47, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walsh, B.T. The Enigmatic Persistence of Anorexia Nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 2013, 170, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wyrwicka, W. Anorexia nervosa as a case of complex instrumental conditioning. Exp. Neurol. 1984, 84, 579–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickinson, A.; Pérez, O.D. Actions and habits: Psychological issues in dual-system theory. In Goal-Directed Decision Making: Computations and Neural Circuits; Morris, R., Bornstein, A., Shenhav, A., Eds.; Elsevier Academic Press: London, UK, 2018; pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- Crisp, A.H. The differential diagnosis of anorexia nervosa. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 1977, 70, 686–690. [Google Scholar]
- Poser, M. Anorexia nervosa—My story. J. Fam. Ther. 2005, 27, 142–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Godier, L.R.; de Wit, S.; Pinto, A.; Steinglass, J.E.; Greene, A.L.; Scaife, J.; Gillan, C.M.; Walsh, B.T.; Simpson, H.-B.; Park, R.J. An investigation of habit learning in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 244, 214–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Hara, C.B.; Campbell, I.C.; Schmidt, U. A reward-centred model of anorexia nervosa: A focused narrative review of the neurological and psychophysiological literature. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 52, 131–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brumberg, J.J. Fasting Girls: The History of Anorexia Nervosa; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Ramacciotti, C.E.; Coli, E.; Marcacci, G.; Polese, L.; Gabbriellini, G.; Bianchi, M. Different pathways towards anorexia nervosa. In Trends in Eating Disorders Research; Swain, P.I., Ed.; Nova Biomedical Books: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar]
- Buzzichelli, S.; Marzola, E.; Amianto, F.; Fassino, S.; Abbate-Daga, G. Perfectionism and cognitive rigidity in anorexia nervosa: Is there an association? Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2018, 26, 360–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinglass, J.; Walsh, B.T. Habit learning and anorexia nervosa: A cognitive neuroscience hypothesis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2006, 39, 267–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uniacke, B.; Walsh, B.T.; Foerde, K.; Steinglass, J. The role of habits in anorexia nervosa: Where we are and where to go from here? Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2018, 20, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Godier, L.R.; Park, R. Compulsivity in anorexia nervosa: A transdiagnostic concept. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Coniglio, K.A.; Becker, K.R.; Franko, D.L.; Zayas, L.V.; Plessow, F.; Eddy, K.T.; Thomas, J.J. Won’t stop or can’t stop? Food restriction as a habitual behavior among individuals with anorexia nervosa or atypical anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 2017, 26, 144–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, L.; Walsh, B.T.; Schebendach, J.; Glasofer, D.R.; Steinglass, J.E. Habits are stronger with longer duration of illness and greater severity in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2020, 53, 683–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foerde, K.; E Steinglass, J.; Shohamy, D.; Walsh, B.T. Neural mechanisms supporting maladaptive food choices in anorexia nervosa. Nat. Neurosci. 2015, 18, 1571–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treasure, J.; Stein, D.; Maguire, S. Has the time come for a staging model to map the course of eating disorders from high risk to severe enduring illness? An examination of the evidence. Early Interv. Psychiatry 2015, 9, 173–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kimball, B.A. Learning. In Consini Encyclopedia of Psychology, 4th ed.; Weiner, I.B., Craighead, W.E., Eds.; John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010; pp. 1753–1755. [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji, B.O.; Sawchuk, C.N. Disgust: Characteristic Features, Social Manifestations, and Clinical Implications. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 24, 932–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Glashouwer, K.A.; de Jong, P.J. The revolting body: Self-disgust as a key factor in anorexia nervosa. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 41, 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soussignan, R.; Schaal, B.; Rigaud, D.; Royet, J.-P.; Jiang, T. Hedonic reactivity to visual and olfactory cues: Rapid facial electromyographic reactions are altered in anorexia nervosa. Biol. Psychol. 2011, 86, 265–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hildebrandt, T.; Grotzinger, A.; Reddan, M.; Greif, R.; Levy, I.; Goodman, W.; Schiller, D. Testing the disgust conditioning theory of food-avoidance in adolescents with recent onset anorexia nervosa. Behav. Res. Ther. 2015, 71, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chao, A.M.; Roy, A.; Franks, A.T.; Joseph, P.V. A Systematic Review of Taste Differences Among People With Eating Disorders. Biol. Res. Nurs. 2019, 22, 82–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troop, N.A.; Treasure, J.L.; Serpell, L. A further exploration of disgust in eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2001, 10, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia-Burgos, D.; Wilhelm, P.; Vögele, C.; Munsch, S. Food avoidance versus food aversion in restrictive eating disorders. Z. Psychiatr. Psychol. Psychother. 2019, 67, 30–38. [Google Scholar]
- Lappalainen, R.; Tuomisto, M.T. Functional Analysis of Anorexia Nervosa: Some Applications to Clinical Practice. Scand. J. Behav. Ther. 1999, 28, 167–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zucker, N.L.; Bulik, C.M. On bells, saliva, and abdominal pain or discomfort: Early aversive visceral conditioning and vulnerability for anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2020, 53, 508–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Georg, S. Gut Function in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Scand. J. Gastroenterol. 2003, 38, 573–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salvioli, B.; Pellicciari, A.; Iero, L.; Di Pietro, E.; Moscano, F.; Gualandi, S.; Stanghellini, V.; De Giorgio, R.; Ruggeri, E.; Franzoni, E. Audit of digestive complaints and psychopathological traits in patients with eating disorders: A prospective study. Dig. Liver Dis. 2013, 45, 639–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liang, N.-C.; Bello, N.T.; Moran, T.H. Experience with activity-based anorexia enhances conditioned taste aversion learning in rats. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 102, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Batsell, W.R., Jr.; Brown, A.S. Human flavor-aversion learning: A comparison of traditional aversions and cognitive aversions. Learn. Motiv. 1998, 29, 383–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logue, A.; Logue, K.; Strauss, K.E. The acquisition of taste aversions in humans with eating and drinking disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 1983, 21, 275–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wyssen, A.; Debbeler, L.J.; Meyer, A.H.; Coelho, J.S.; Humbel, N.; Schuck, K.; Lennertz, J.; Messerli-Burgy, N.; Trier, S.N.; Isenschmid, B.; et al. Relevance of the Thought-Shape Fusion Trait Questionnaire for healthy women and women presenting symptoms of eating disorders and mixed mental disorders. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2018, 25, 601–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garcia-Burgos, D.; Maglieri, S.; Vögele, C.; Munsch, S. How Does Food Taste in Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa? A Protocol for a Quasi-Experimental, Cross-Sectional Design to Investigate Taste Aversion or Increased Hedonic Valence of Food in Eating Disorders. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lascelles, K.R.; Field, A.P.; Davey, G.C. Using foods as CSs and body shapes as UCSs: A putative role for associative learning in the development of eating disorders. Behav. Ther. 2003, 34, 213–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fox, J.R.; Grange, N.; Power, M.J. Self-disgust in eating disorders: A review of the literature and clinical implications. In The Revolting Self: Perspectives on the Psychological, Social, and Clinical Implications of Self-Directed Disgust; Powell, P., Overton, P., Simpson, J., Eds.; Karnac Books: London, UK, 2015; pp. 167–186. [Google Scholar]
- Franko, D.L.; Wolfe, B.E.; Jimerson, D.C. Elevated sweet taste pleasantness ratings in bulimia nervosa. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 969–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Umberg, E.N.; Shader, R.I.; Hsu, L.G.; Greenblatt, D.J. From disordered eating to addiction: The “food drug” in bulimia nervosa. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2012, 32, 376–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rodin, J.; Bartoshuk, L.; Peterson, C.; Schank, D. Bulimia and taste: Possible interactions. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1990, 99, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vögele, C.; Lutz, A.P.C.; Gibson, L.E. Mood, emotions and eating disorders. In Oxford Handbook of Eating Disorders. Series: Oxford Library of Psychology, 2nd ed.; Agras, W., Robinson, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 155–186. [Google Scholar]
- Bouton, M. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 80–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macht, M. How emotions affect eating: A five-way model. Appetite 2008, 50, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foerde, K.; Daw, N.D.; Rufin, T.; Walsh, B.T.; Shohamy, D.; Steinglass, J.E. Deficient Goal-Directed Control in a Population Characterized by Extreme Goal Pursuit. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2021, 33, 463–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foerde, K.; Steinglass, J.E. Decreased feedback learning in anorexia nervosa persists after weight restoration. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2017, 50, 415–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nunes, A.; Helson, T.J.; Dixon, L.M.; Trappenberg, T.P.; Keshen, A.R. Characteristic impairments of goal-directed and habitual control in bulimia nervosa. bioRxiv 2018, 348250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Itzhak, Y.; Perez-Lanza, D.; Liddie, S. The strength of aversive and appetitive associations and maladaptive behaviors. IUBMB Life 2014, 66, 559–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Frank, G.K.; Kaye, W.H. Current status of functional imaging in eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2012, 45, 723–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pennesi, J.L.; Wade, T.D. A systematic review of the existing models of disordered eating: Do they inform the development of effective interventions? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 43, 175–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olatunji, B.O. Linking Pavlovian Disgust Conditioning and Eating Disorder Symptoms: An Analogue Study. Behav. Ther. 2020, 51, 178–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lennie, T.A. Anorexia in response to acute illness. Heart Lung 1999, 28, 386–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, S.B.; Loeb, K.L.; Le Grange, D. Dissecting the Core Fear in Anorexia Nervosa: Can We Optimize Treatment Mechanisms? JAMA Psychiatry 2016, 73, 891–892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kamin, L.J. Predictability, Surprise, Attention, and Conditioning. In Punishment Aversive Behavior; Campbell, B., Church, R., Eds.; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1969; pp. 279–296. [Google Scholar]
- Quigley, M.; Haselgrove, M. Interactions between the elements of an outcome in human associative learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 2020, 46, 297–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holland, P.C. Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Learn. Behav. 2008, 36, 227–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rachid, F. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of eating disorders: A review of safety and efficacy. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 269, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muratore, A.F.; Attia, E. Current Therapeutic Approaches to Anorexia Nervosa: State of the Art. Clin. Ther. 2020, 43, 85–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mason, E.C.; Richardson, R. Treating disgust in anxiety disorders. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2012, 19, 180–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smits, J.; Telch, M.; Randall, P. An examination of the decline in fear and disgust during exposure-based treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 2002, 40, 1243–1253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treanor, M.; Staples-Bradley, L.K.; Craske, M.G. Learning theory and cognitive behavioral therapy. In Anxiety Disorders: Translational Perspectives on Diagnosis and Treatment; Ressler, K.J., Pine, D., Rothbaum, B.O., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 355–366. [Google Scholar]
- Steinglass, J.E.; Glasofer, D.R.; Walsh, E.; Guzman, G.; Peterson, C.B.; Walsh, B.T.; Attia, E.; Wonderlich, S.A. Targeting habits in anorexia nervosa: A proof-of-concept randomized trial. Psychol. Med. 2018, 48, 2584–2591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, W.; Neal, D.T. A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 843–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Capaldi, E.D.; Privitera, G.J. Flavor–nutrient learning independent of flavor–taste learning with college students. Appetite 2007, 49, 712–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeomans, M.R.; Leitch, M.; Gould, N.J.; Mobini, S. Differential hedonic, sensory and behavioral changes associated with flavor–nutrient and flavor–flavor learning. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 93, 798–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouton, M.E. Extinction of instrumental (operant) learning: Interference, varieties of context, and mechanisms of contextual control. Psychopharmacology 2019, 236, 7–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craddock, P.; Wasserman, J.S.; Polack, C.W.; Kosinski, T.; Renaux, C.; Miller, R.R. Associative structure of second-order conditioning in humans. Learn. Behav. 2017, 46, 171–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Declercq, M.; De Houwer, J. Evidence for a hierarchical structure underlying avoidance behavior. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2009, 35, 123–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Treanor, M.; Barry, T. Treatment of avoidance behavior as an adjunct to exposure therapy: Insights from modern learning theory. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017, 96, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schmajuk, N.A.; Holland, P.C. Occasion Setting: Associative Learning and Cognition in Animals; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Agras, W.S.; Barlow, D.H.; Chapin, H.N.; Abel, G.G.; Leitenberg, H. Behavior modification of anorexia nervosa. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1974, 30, 279–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Annus, A.M.; Smith, G.T.; Fischer, S.; Hendricks, M.; Williams, S.F. Associations among family-of-origin food-related experiences, expectancies, and disordered eating. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2007, 40, 179–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Azerrad, J.; Stafford, R.L. Restoration of eating behavior in anorexia nervosa through operant conditioning and environmental manipulation. Behav. Res. Ther. 1969, 7, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belke, T.W.; Pierce, W.D.; Duncan, I.D. Reinforcement value and substitutability of sucrose and wheel running: Implications for activity anorexia. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2006, 86, 131–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bhanji, S.; Thompson, J. Operant conditioning in the treatment of anorexia nervosa: A review and retrospective study of 11 cases. Br. J. Psychiatry 1974, 124, 166–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianco, F.J. Rapid treatment of two cases of anorexia nervosa. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 1972, 3, 223–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blinder, B.J.; Freeman, D.M.; Stunkard, A.J. Behavior therapy of anorexia nervosa: Effectiveness of activity as a reinforcer of weight gain. Am. J. Psychiatry 1970, 126, 1093–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blue, R. Use of punishment in treatment of anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Rep. 1979, 44, 743–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boey, K.W. The restoration of body weight in anorexia nervosa through operant conditioning: A case report. Singap. Med. J. 1985, 26, 187–191. [Google Scholar]
- Bruch, H. Perils of behavior modification in treatment of anorexia nervosa. JAMA 1974, 230, 1419–1422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Combs, J.L.; Smith, G.T.; Flory, K.; Simmons, J.R.; Hill, K.K. The acquired preparedness model of risk for bulimic symptom development. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2010, 24, 475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- de Paz, A.; Vidal, P.; Pellón, R. Exercise, diet, and the reinforcing value of food in an animal model of anorexia nervosa. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2019, 72, 1692–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eckert, E.D.; Goldberg, S.C.; Halmi, K.A.; Casper, R.C.; Davis, J.M. Behaviour therapy in anorexia nervosa. Br. J. Psychiatry 1979, 134, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ehrlich, S.; Geisler, D.; Ritschel, F.; King, J.A.; Seidel, M.; Boehm, I.; Kroemer, N.B. Elevated cognitive control over reward processing in recovered female patients with anorexia nervosa. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015, 40, 307–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Elkin, T.E.; Hersen, M.; Eisler, R.M.; Williams, J.G. Modification of caloric intake in anorexia nervosa: An experimental analysis. Psychol. Rep. 1973, 32, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erwin, W.J. A 16-year follow-up of a case of severe anorexia nervosa. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 1977, 8, 157–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frank, G.K.; Collier, S.; Shott, M.E.; O’Reilly, R.C. Prediction error and somatosensory insula activation in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. J. Psychiatry. Neurosci. 2016, 41, 304–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Frank, G.K.; DeGuzman, M.C.; Shott, M.E.; Laudenslager, M.L.; Rossi, B.; Pryor, T. Association of brain reward learning response with harm avoidance, weight gain, and hypothalamic effective connectivity in adolescent anorexia nervosa. JAMA Psychiatry 2018, 75, 1071–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Frank, G.K.; Reynolds, J.R.; Shott, M.E.; Jappe, L.; Yang, T.T.; Tregellas, J.R.; O’reilly, R.C. Anorexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward response. Neuropsychopharmacology 2012, 37, 2031–2046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garfinkel, P.E.; Kline, S.A.; Stancer, H.C. Treatment of anorexia nervosa using operant conditioning techniques. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1973, 157, 428–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glashouwer, K.A.; Neimeijer, R.A.; de Koning, M.L.; Vestjens, M.; Martijn, C. Evaluative conditioning as a body image intervention for adolescents with eating disorders. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 86, 1046–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hallsten, E.A., Jr. Adolescent anorexia nervosa treated by desensitization. Behav. Res. Ther. 1965, 3, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halmi, K.A.; Powers, P.; Cunningham, S. Treatment of anorexia nervosa with behavior modification: Effectiveness of formula feeding and isolation. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1975, 32, 93–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hurel, I.; Redon, B.; Scocard, A.; Malezieux, M.; Marsicano, G.; Chaouloff, F. Beyond the activity-based anorexia model: Reinforcing values of exercise and feeding examined in stressed adolescent male and female mice. Front. Pharmacol. 2019, 10, 587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kerr, K.L.; Moseman, S.E.; Avery, J.A.; Bodurka, J.; Simmons, W.K. Influence of visceral interoceptive experience on the brain’s response to food images in anorexia nervosa. Psychosom. Med. 2017, 79, 777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kissel, S.; Arkins, V. Anorexia nervosa reexamined. Child Psych. Hum. Dev. 1973, 3, 255–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klein, D.A.; Schebendach, J.E.; Gershkovich, M.; Bodell, L.P.; Foltin, R.W.; Walsh, B.T. Behavioral assessment of the reinforcing effect of exercise in women with anorexia nervosa: Further paradigm development and data. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2010, 43, 611–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leitenberg, H.; Agras, W.S.; Thomson, L.E. A sequential analysis of the effect of selective positive reinforcement in modifying anorexia nervosa. Behav. Res. Ther. 1968, 6, 211–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monti, P.M.; McCrady, B.S.; Barlow, D.H. Effect of positive reinforcement, informational feedback, and contingency contracting on a bulimic anorexic female. Behav. Ther. 1977, 8, 258–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munford, P.R.; Tarlow, G.; Gerner, R.H. An experimental analysis of the interaction of chemotherapy and behavior therapy in anorexia nervosa. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1984, 72, 228–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, R.; Nutzinger, D.O.; Paul, T.; Leplow, B. Dissociated conditional-associative learning in anorexia nervosa. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2002, 24, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olsavsky, A.K.; Shott, M.E.; DeGuzman, M.C.; Frank, G.K. Neural correlates of taste reward value across eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2019, 288, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perkin, G.J.; Surtees, P.G. Wider perspectives in the behavioural treatment of anorexia nervosa: A case study. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 1976, 10, 325–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poole, A.D.; Sanson-Fisher, R.W.; Young, P. A behavioural programme for the management of anorexia nervosa. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 1978, 12, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowland, N.E.; Cervantez, M.; Robertson, K.L. Restricted temporal access to food and anorexia in mice: Microstructure of eating within feeding opportunities. Appetite 2016, 96, 621–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schebendach, J.E.; Klein, D.A.; Foltin, R.W.; Devlin, M.J.; Walsh, B.T. Relative reinforcing value of exercise in inpatients with anorexia nervosa: Model development and pilot data. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2007, 40, 446–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steele, R.L. Anorexia nervosa: A case study. Psychother. Psychosom. 1976, 27, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stumphauzer, J.S. Application of reinforcement contingencies with a 23-year-old anorexic patient. Psychol. Rep. 1969, 24, 109–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Touyz, S.W.; Beumont, P.J.V.; Glaun, D.; Phillips, T.; Cowie, I. A comparison of lenient and strict operant conditioning programmes in refeeding patients with anorexia nervosa. Br. J. Psychiatry 1984, 144, 517–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandereycken, W.; Pieters, G. Short-term weight restoration in anorexia nervosa through operant conditioning. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 1978, 7, 221–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wulliemier, F. Anorexia nervosa: Gauging treatment effectiveness. Psychosomatics 1978, 19, 497–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Search | Strategy | Descriptors and Keywords |
---|---|---|
#1 | Focused on behaviourist learning theory and eating disorders | Descriptors for learning (“learning theory” OR “reinforcement” OR “stimulus-response” OR “classical conditioning” OR “operant conditioning” OR “instrumental conditioning” OR “respondent conditioning” OR “Pavlovian conditioning” OR “instrumental learning”) AND descriptors for eating disorder (“anorexia nervosa”) |
#2 | Focused on modern learning theory and eating disorders | Descriptors for learning (“associative learning” OR “content of learning” OR “modern learning”) AND descriptors for eating disorder (“anorexia nervosa”) |
#3 | Focused on eating behaviour and conditioning | Descriptors for learning (“conditioning theory” OR “conditioning procedure” OR “conditioning learning”) AND descriptors for eating behaviour (“eating disorder” OR “disordered eating”) |
Dysfunctional Association in Memory | R-AN | BP-AN | No Patients | Process | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sweet–fat food-related cue → Catastrophic weight gain | ✓ | ✓ | × | Conditioned fear learning | Dieting, food restriction |
Sweet–fat flavour → Gastrointestinal malaise | ✓ | × | × | Conditioned flavour aversion learning | Food avoidance |
Sweet–fat flavour → Pleasant gustatory sensations | × | ✓ | × | Enhanced learned flavour preference | Overconsumption, binge eating |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Garcia-Burgos, D.; Wilhelm, P.; Vögele, C.; Munsch, S. Food Restriction in Anorexia Nervosa in the Light of Modern Learning Theory: A Narrative Review. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 96. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020096
Garcia-Burgos D, Wilhelm P, Vögele C, Munsch S. Food Restriction in Anorexia Nervosa in the Light of Modern Learning Theory: A Narrative Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(2):96. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020096
Chicago/Turabian StyleGarcia-Burgos, David, Peter Wilhelm, Claus Vögele, and Simone Munsch. 2023. "Food Restriction in Anorexia Nervosa in the Light of Modern Learning Theory: A Narrative Review" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 2: 96. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020096
APA StyleGarcia-Burgos, D., Wilhelm, P., Vögele, C., & Munsch, S. (2023). Food Restriction in Anorexia Nervosa in the Light of Modern Learning Theory: A Narrative Review. Behavioral Sciences, 13(2), 96. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020096