Next Article in Journal
Grief Intervention in Suicide Loss Survivors through Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
The Skill of Positive Solitude Moderates the Relationship between 24 Character Strengths and Flourishing in the Second Half of Life
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Sexual Double Standard toward Non-Heterosexual Populations: Evaluations of Sexually Active Gay Men and Lesbian Women
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Associations of Implicit and Explicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement and Sexual Assertiveness with Sexual and Interactional Competence in Emerging Adults

by
Andrea Grauvogl
*,
Ron Pat-El
and
Jacques J. D. M. van Lankveld
Faculty of Psychology, Open University of the Netherlands, P.O. Box 2960, 6401 DL Heerlen, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(9), 790; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090790
Submission received: 6 June 2024 / Revised: 2 September 2024 / Accepted: 5 September 2024 / Published: 9 September 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sexual Double Standard: Prejudice in Gender and Sexual Orientation)

Abstract

:
In this study, among emerging adults, we investigated the interrelationships of explicit and implicit measures of sexual assertiveness (SA) and sexual double standard endorsement (SDS) on the one hand, and different aspects of sexual and interactional competence (SAIC) on the other hand, using Partial Least Squares Path Modeling (PLS-PM) of cross-sectional data. Participants were 159 sexually active, heterosexual individuals in the Netherlands between 18 and 25 years. No exclusion criteria were used. The Sexual Competence and Interaction Competence in Youth and lifetime number of sexual partners were used to measure SAIC. Explicit SA was measured using the Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness, while Explicit SDS was assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Sexual Standards in Youth. Two implicit association tests were performed to measure implicit SA and SDS. Participants accessed these computerized reaction time tasks via a secure online data collection platform. Results showed a strong association between the latent factors of sexual attitudes and SAIC. Greater SA and lower SDS were associated with a greater competence level. No gender effects were found.

1. Introduction

In this study, among emerging adults, we investigated the interrelationships of explicit and implicit measures of sexual assertiveness (SA) and sexual double standard endorsement on the one hand, and different aspects of sexual and interactional competence (SAIC) on the other hand. The terms sexual competence and interactional competence refer to different but related constructs in the field of sexuality. Recent conceptualizations of sexual competence focus on various aspects of sexual behavior [1,2,3,4]. Sexual competence consists of cognitive factors and skills aimed at solving problems that arise in different sexual contexts, including communication ability, refusal and resistance, conflict resolution, and interpersonal negotiation skills [5,6]. While these authors conceive of sexual competence as consisting of personal characteristics and skills that act causally to achieve sexual health, others view sexual competence more as the outcome state of such characteristics and skills. For instance, Palmer et al. [3] describe sexual competence at first intercourse as encompassing the aspects of protection by contraception, consensuality (willingness of both partners), autonomy (intercourse was not due to external influences such as alcohol or peer pressure), and acceptability of timing as perceived by both individuals. In line with the WHO definition of sexual health [7], some definitions of sexual competence also include the cognitive, emotional and interactional skills to achieve the experience of sexual pleasure and satisfaction [2,8,9]. These recent conceptualizations are now widely accepted among researchers, although they do not necessarily coincide with lay definitions of sexual competence in youth [10,11].
Among researchers, both psychological and interactional skills are seen as relevant to sexual competence [1,4]. Psychological skills refer to the ability to identify one’s own sexual needs and limits. Interactional skills include the ability to communicate one’s needs and limits and to adequately assess and appraise the partner’s responses, including the partner’s consent. Gold and colleagues [4] emphasized the importance of interactional competence as an intermediary to realizing one’s sexual competence in sexual interactions. Interactional competence includes communicative and social skills, and mental and behavioral strategies that serve to arrange sexual encounters with a partner in a mutually satisfactory manner [1]. Although the descriptions of sexual competence and interactional competence clearly show considerable overlap, interactional competence refers more to action aspects in an interaction context, including sexual communication, while sexual competence points to individual psychological factors and protective outcomes of sexual interactions.
SAIC are thus conceptualized as consisting of a wide range of cognitive and behavioral skills. In the present study, we selected four constituent factors to represent SAIC: sexual communication, pleasurable sexuality, contraceptive use, and limited lifetime number of sexual partners. Three of these factors are measured using the Sexual Competence and Interaction Competence in Youth (SCICY) [12], and represent sexual communication skills, positive feelings about sex, and behavioral competence with condom use. A high lifetime number of sexual intercourse partners is seen as entailing increased risk of sexually transmitted infections, including HIV, and as a threat to sexual health in adolescents and emerging adults [13,14]. The number of lifetime sexual partners is measured with a single question.
For the purpose of developing interventions aimed at enhancing young people’s SAIC, it is important to increase our knowledge of (modifiable) factors related to SAIC [15]. Some determinants of SAIC have previously been investigated. Knowledge of one’s sexual preferences [6,16], but not general sexual knowledge, and social and behavioral competence [6] were found to predict sexual health in emerging adults. Relationship factors, including the partner’s autonomy-supportive versus controlling behavior, were also found to affect sexual competence [17]. The predictors of sexual health outcomes in these studies showed only modest effect sizes, explaining up to 33% of the variance in the full prediction model [16], leaving room for additional explanatory factors. In addition to sexual knowledge and relational factors, a wide array of other determinants has been suggested, including among others: school-based education on sexuality and relationships [11], general level of education [18], sexual literacy [2], sexual assertiveness [9,19,20] and sexual double standard endorsement [21]. In this study we focused on the latter two factors as potential predictors of SAIC.

1.1. Sexual Double Standard Endorsement

The term “sexual double standard” (SDS) is used to describe a set of normative expectations for engaging in romantic and sexual behavior, which differ for young men and women [22]. A central aspect of the SDS is gender differences in sexual assertiveness (SA); young men are expected to be sexually active, dominant, and take sexual initiative, whereas young women are expected to be sexually reserved, submissive, and passive [20,23]. Endorsement of the SDS is associated with various negative sexual health outcomes [21]. In young men, SDS was found to be associated with greater acceptance of rape myths [24], beliefs that dating violence is acceptable, and sexually violent behavior [25,26,27]. For both young men and women, it is related to early sexual debut [28,29] and higher STI/HIV infection risk [30,31]. The impact of SDS seems particularly negative for girls [21]. The sexual passivity implicated in SDS predicts more negative and fewer positive emotions related to sex in young women [20], as well as lower sexual satisfaction [32,33] and more sexual problems. In a Spanish study, men but not women who favored egalitarian gender roles in sexuality reported greater sexual satisfaction [34]. In a study among mostly African American adolescents, girls had more gender equitable attitudes [23]. Participants with more equitable gender norms reported perpetrating significantly less relationship abuse. Gender equitable attitudes were also significantly correlated with condom negotiation self-efficacy.
Over time, the SDS concept has evolved; some researchers argue that the SDS no longer exists [35,36], or only under specific conditions [37]. Other researchers observed a reverse SDS [38,39,40,41], although traditional sexual script adherence was recently found to still impact behavior [42]. Based on previous research outcomes that imply multiple associations of SDS with sexual health outcomes, we considered it an important candidate for inclusion in the current study.

1.2. Sexual Assertiveness

Assertiveness in sexual encounters is considered an important correlate of SAIC [9,19,20]. Greater SA was associated with greater sexual satisfaction among female undergraduate students in the US [43], although romantic and casual sexual relationships in youth did not differ in the actors’ level of SA in a Dutch study [9]. The hypothesized interrelatedness of SA and SDS was tested in a cross-sectional study among Ecuadorian emerging adults aged 18 to 30 [18]. SA was positively associated with overall mental well-being and relationship satisfaction in women but not in men. In turn, SA was negatively predicted by SDS, especially among less educated women and men. In a study in Germany, Klein et al. [42] conceptualized SDS as acceptance vs. rejection of traditional gender scripts and performed an online vignette study in which adult male participants were instructed to imagine themselves single in a bar situation with a woman. The scenario described the woman as displaying either assertive, timid, or controlling sexual behavior. Confirming the traditional gender script and SDS, men were found to perceive women who behaved sexually assertively as nonconformist. Their judgment of the assertive woman was less positive than that of women who expressed sexual timidity or control. The authors concluded from the findings of a second study using the same design and methods among both women and men that it was SA, not sexual script deviation, that determined their evaluations of the imagined target [42]. SA, therefore, seems a strong candidate for inclusion in the present study of correlates of SAIC in young adults. To our knowledge, no study has yet examined the associations between both SDS and SA and SAIC simultaneously.

1.3. Explicit Versus Implicit Factors

In addition to deliberate attitudes and intentions, attitudes that operate at an automatic cognitive level may be relevant in SAIC. Automatic cognitions, assessed with the help of indirect measures, have been found to predict behavior in various fields of human functioning, including sexual functioning (e.g., [44]), condom (non-)use [45,46], aggressive behavior [47], alcohol use (e.g., [48]), physical activity [49], and psychopathology (e.g., [50]). There is broad consensus that measuring implicit associations is able to bypass introspective access and socially desirable biases [51]. In several reviews and meta-analyses, implicit associations were found to add limited but significant proportions of explained variance to directly measured deliberate attitudes [49,52]. Thus, implicit measurements may provide different and additional [51], although not necessarily more (or less) accurate, information on SA and SDS. Good theoretical understanding of the determinants of SAIC, and sufficient empirical support of this theory, are building blocks for future efficacious interventions [53]. In the present study, we aim to answer the following research question: What are the interrelationships between SAIC on the one hand, and SDS and SA on the other hand? We also explored the bivariate associations between the key variables, and whether the associations differed in young women vs. men.

2. Method

This study is part of a larger research project. Among others, previous studies in this project investigated the psychometric aspects of two implicit association tests for measuring implicit sexual assertiveness and implicit sexual double standard endorsement, which were required for conducting the present study (see the Instruments section below).

2.1. Sample

Undergraduate psychology students at the Open Universiteit recruited participants from their personal circles of acquaintances. Sexually active, heterosexual, emerging adults between the ages of 18 and 25 with a good command of the Dutch language were eligible for participation. No exclusion criteria were used.

2.2. Instruments

Demographics. Participants reported their age (years), gender (female, male, other), and sexual orientation (on a five-point scale ranging from ‘1 = exclusively attracted to men’ to ‘5 = exclusively attracted to women’).
Sexual and Interactional Competence. The latent construct of SAIC was comprised four factors: sexual communication skills, emotional well-being regarding sexuality, and behavioral competence with condom use, all measured using the Sexual Competence and Interaction Competence in Youth (SCICY) [12], as well as the lifetime number of sexual partners. The SCICY is a 26-item self-report questionnaire aimed at measuring sexual and interaction competence in adolescents. It is organized into eight subscales, three of which were selected for the present purpose: sexual communication skills (6 items), positive feelings about sex (5 items), and behavioral competence with condom use (2 items). Responses are given using 5-point Likert scales. The answer options differ per item group. For items in the selected subscales that were formulated as statements, answer options were: 1 “I always agree”, 2 “I mostly agree”, 3 “I sometimes agree/disagree”, 4 “I mostly disagree”, and 5 “I always disagree”. For items asking participants to report behavioral initiatives, the answer options were: 1 “I would always dare to do that”, 2 “I would usually dare to do that”, 3 “I would/wouldn’t dare to do that sometimes”, 4 “I wouldn’t dare to do that most of the time”, and 5 “I would never dare to do that”. For self-descriptive items, answer options were 1 “Never applies to me”, 2 “Usually does not apply to me”, 3 “Sometimes applies/does not apply to me”, 4 “Usually applies to me”, 5 “Always applies to me”. Higher scores represent higher levels of communication skills, positive feelings about sex, and competence with contraceptive use. The subscales were found to have adequate internal consistency (alphas ranging from 0.77 to 0.84), and moderate to excellent test–retest reliability (communication about sex: r = 0.76, p < 0.001; positive feelings about sex: r = 0.54, p < 0.001; contraceptive use: r = 1.00, p < 0.001) [12].
Lifetime Number of Sexual Partners was reported by participants as their response to a single question: “How many people have you had sex with (approximately) in your life? By sex, we mean everything from touching or caressing genitals to sexual penetration”. Responses were recorded as integers. Participants were allowed to refrain from answering the question without providing a reason.
Implicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement. An IAT [54] was designed to measure Implicit SDS (SDS-IAT) [41]. Target category labels were ‘male’ versus ‘female’, and attribute category labels were ‘sexually active’ versus ‘sexually passive’. Stimulus words were presented in the middle of the screen. Words representing the target categories were Dutch male (i.e., Bram, Tim, Rob, Jan) and female names (i.e., Emma, Lieke, Julia, Roos). Words representing the ‘sexually active’ attribute category were ‘sexual’, ‘exciting’, ‘experienced’, and ‘daring’ (in Dutch: ‘seksueel’, ‘opwindend’, ‘ervaren’, ‘uitdagend’). Words representing the ‘sexually passive’ attribute category were ‘biding’, ‘reserved’, ‘cautious’, and ‘modest’ (in Dutch: ‘afwachtend’, ‘terughoudend’, ‘voorzichtig’, ‘bescheiden’). The SDS-IAT was found to possess adequate psychometric properties [41]. Attenuation-corrected alphas (range: 0.65–0.70) demonstrated acceptable internal consistency. A multitrait-multimethod analysis of the SDS-IAT demonstrated low convergent validity with Explicit SDS, supporting theoretical expectations. Divergent validity was confirmed by the absence of significant correlations with conceptually unrelated concepts, except for extraversion in female participants.
Implicit Sexual Assertiveness. An IAT was designed to measure Implicit SA (SA-IAT) [55]. Attribute categories were ‘I’ (in Dutch: ‘ik’) versus ‘Other’ (in Dutch: ‘een ander’), and target categories were ‘sexually assertive’ (in Dutch: ‘seksueel assertief’) versus ‘sexually compliant’ (in Dutch: ‘seksueel meegaand’). Stimuli presented in the center of the screen were either words representing the ‘I’ attribute category (‘I’, ‘me’, ‘self’, ‘mine’ [in Dutch: ‘ik, mij, zelf, mijn’]), or the ‘other’ attribute category (you, their, your [in Dutch: ‘je, jij, hun, jouw’]), or words associated with the ‘sexually assertive’ (confident, initiative-taking, leading, adventurous [in Dutch: ‘zelfverzekerd, initiatiefrijk, leidend, avontuurlijk’]) versus ‘sexually compliant’ (following, agreeable, subordinate, dependent [in Dutch: ‘volgend, toegeeflijk, onderdanig, afhankelijk’]) target categories. The SA-IAT was found to possess adequate psychometric properties [55]. Attenuation-corrected alphas (range: 0.61–0.70) demonstrated acceptable internal consistency. A multitrait-multimethod analysis of the SA-IAT demonstrated low convergent validity with Explicit SA, supporting theoretical expectations. Divergent validity was confirmed by the absence of significant correlations of Implicit SA with conceptually unrelated concepts, except for extraversion in the full sample and the female subsample.
The labels of the target and attribute categories in both IATs were permanently visible in the upper-left and upper-right corners of the screen. Correct responses were defined as key presses with which stimuli were placed into the category they were a priori considered to represent. After a correct response, the next stimulus was presented after a 250 ms interval. After an incorrect response, a red X replaced the stimulus and remained on the screen until the correct key was pressed. The IATs were organized into five blocks. To familiarize participants with the procedure, they started with a practice run of 16 trials presenting only stimuli from the target category (e.g., gender in the SDS-IAT: ‘male’ versus ’female’; block 1). Next was a practice block of 16 trials (block 2), in which both target and attribute stimuli were presented, followed by a test block of 48 trials (block 3). In these two blocks, one of two possible combinations of target and attribute categories (e.g., female + sexually passive; male + sexually active) was mapped to the response keys (‘z’ and ‘m’ on a QWERTY keyboard). In the final blocks, a practice block (block 4) and a test block (block 5), including the same number of trials, the reverse combination was presented (e.g., male + sexually passive; female + sexually active). Two versions of each IAT were made. They differed in the order of presentation of blocks 2 + 3 and 4 + 5, allowing for the investigation of potential order effects. Random allocation ensured that half of the participants started with each version. Testing for order effects revealed a within-IAT order effect for the SDS-IAT, but not for the SA-IAT. No between-IAT order effects were found.
Explicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement. Explicit SDS was assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Sexual Standards in Youth (SASSY) [56]. The 19-item SASSY was designed as a multifaceted measure of SDS in young people and was found to have a single underlying factor. An example item is ‘Sometimes a boy should apply some pressure to a girl to get what he wants sexually’. Answers were given on a 6-point scale ranging from ‘1 = Completely disagree’ to ‘6 = Completely agree’. In previous research, good reliability was found among Dutch adolescents (α = 0.90) [56] and young adults (Study 1: α = 0.88, Study 2: α = 0.89) [56]. A mean score was calculated from all items. Higher scores represent a higher level of SDS. In the present sample a Cronbach’s α of 0.85 indicated good internal consistency.
Explicit Sexual Assertiveness. To assess Explicit SA, a selection of eight items from the Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness (HISA) was used [57,58,59]. Items were selected while taking the factor structure of the scale into account, ensuring that items from both the ‘initiation’ (3 items) and ‘no shyness/refusal’ dimensions (5 items) were included [60]. An example item from the ‘initiation’ dimension is ‘I think I am open with my partner about my sexual needs’. An example item from the ‘no shyness/refusal’ dimension is ‘It is hard for me to say no, even when I do not want sex’. Answers were given on a 5-point scale ranging from ‘0 = Never’ to ’4 = Always’. A mean score was calculated from all items. Higher scores represent a higher level of SA. Satisfactory reliability for both the ‘initiation’ (α = 0.77) and the ‘no shyness/refusal’ (α = 0.72) subscales was found in the present sample.

2.3. Procedure

Ethical approval was obtained from the University Ethics Committee (filed under reference FETC-14024). Participant anonymity and data security were ensured using the university’s secure online data acquisition platform. Participants completed the questionnaires and performed the computer tasks in the comfort of their own home using an online research platform to reduce bias due to social demands. Online assessment of IAT has been found to produce robust findings that do not differ from assessments in a lab setting [61,62]. Participants first completed either the SDS-IAT or an IAT assessing Implicit SA. The order of both IATs was randomly assigned. After completing the IATs, participants completed the questionnaires of the study in a fixed order. Completing the study took about 30 min. After finishing the final task, participants received an email with a debriefing message and a digital gift voucher worth EUR 10.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The D600 algorithm was employed to calculate scores for Implicit SDS and Implicit SA. Only test block data were used. Reaction times (RTs) below 400 ms were discarded, and those higher than 2500 ms were replaced with 2500 before calculating the mean RTs. Error trial RTs were replaced with the mean RT of the participant’s correct responses in the same block in which the error occurred, plus a 600 ms penalty. The D600 index score was calculated as the difference between the mean RTs, divided by the pooled standard deviation with the exception of the attribute practice block. Negative scores reflect higher Implicit SDS, while positive scores reflect higher Implicit SA.
Prior to analysis, the data were screened for errors in data entries, missing values, and abnormal distributions and values. In the dataset, missing values were identified in the following variables: SA-IAT (26 missing), SDS-IAT (27 missing), lifetime number of sex partners (21 missing), Explicit SA (8 missing), Explicit SDS (6 missing), sexual communication skills (8 missing), positive feelings about sex (8 missing), and behavioral competence with condom use (8 missing). To mitigate the impact of missing data on the analysis, the mice package [63] in R version 4.2.2 [64] was employed. The predictive mean matching method was used for imputation, generating five multiple imputations to ensure a robust and reliable dataset for subsequent analyses.
To test the relationship between the study’s latent constructs, we utilized Partial Least Squares Path Modeling (PLS-PM), employing the PLS-PM package in R [64]. PLS-PM is a non-parametric statistical technique used for structural equation modeling (SEM) that differs from covariance-style SEM by focusing on the relationships between latent variables and observed indicators [65]. Unlike traditional covariance-based SEM, which emphasizes covariances and variances among variables, PLS-PM is a variance-based approach that prioritizes explaining the variance in the endogenous constructs. This makes PLS-PM particularly well-suited for modeling complex relationships in small sample sizes or situations with high collinearity. Additionally, PLS-PM is advantageous for formative measurement models, where latent constructs are considered to be caused by their indicators rather than causing them. This flexibility in handling formative constructs distinguishes PLS-PM, allowing researchers to effectively model and understand situations where variables are more causally driven by their observed indicators.
The analysis encompassed two latent variables with a formative measurement model. The dataset, consisting of 159 cases and 8 manifest variables, was standardized. The PLSPM was executed with a centroid weighting scheme and a maximum of 100 iterations. The two latent variables, sexual attitudes and SAIC, were defined as exogenous and endogenous blocks, respectively. Unidimensionality assessments revealed satisfactory internal consistency reliability for both blocks, with composite reliability ( α ) and eigenvalues supporting the robustness of the latent constructs.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics and Preliminary Analyses

Data were collected from a convenience sample of 159 participants (Nwomen = 98; Nmen = 55; Nmissing = 6) living in the Netherlands or Flanders, Belgium. Demographic features, their categorization, and scores on sexual variables are shown in Table 1. The mean ages of female (22.2 ± 1.9 years) and male participants (21.8 ± 1.9 years) were not significantly different. Moderate and high education levels prevailed and were not significantly different between genders. Male participants were significantly more often single or dating than female participants, while the latter were more often in a committed relationship. Response patterns differed between women and men regarding their sexual attraction to the other versus their own gender and their relationship status. A larger proportion of female participants reported feeling ‘mostly’ attracted to the other gender compared to male participants.
Descriptive statistics of the study variables and 95% CIs for differences between young women and men on these variables are presented in Table 2. Young men reported greater sexual communication skills, more positive feelings about sex, and higher explicit sexual assertiveness (SA) but lower Implicit SA. Compared to young women, men exhibited stronger implicit endorsement of the sexual double standard (SDS), whereas women endorsed a reverse Implicit SDS. No significant differences were found in Explicit SDS between genders.
The three factors of the SAIC construct measured using the SCICY, correlated significantly in the full sample (rs between 0.292 and 0.411) and in the female subsample (rs between 0.299 and 0.486; see Table 3). In the male subsample, only sexual communication skills and behavioral competence with condom use were found to correlate (r = 0.292). Lifetime number of sexual partners did not correlate with the other factors. Sexual communication skills, positive feelings about sex, and behavioral competence with condom use correlated positively with Explicit SA in the full sample, while positive feelings about sex correlated negatively with Explicit SDS, indicating that stronger SDS (negative scores) came with stronger positive feelings. This correlation was stronger in young men than in young women. Lifetime number of sexual partners correlated negatively with behavioral competence with condom use in young men, but this association was absent in young women. A negative correlation between Explicit SDS and Explicit SA was found, indicating that greater SDS came with greater SA, with the correlation being significant only in the young women’s subsample.

3.2. Hypothesis Testing

We employed Partial Least Squares Path Modeling (PLS-PM) to investigate predictive relationships between sexual attitudes and SAIC.
Two cases with high counts of lifetime sexual experiences (36 and 60, respectively) were identified. The PLS-PM analysis was conducted twice: once including all cases and once excluding these two extreme cases. The difference in results was negligible, leading us to present the analysis including all cases.

3.3. The Outer Model

In PLS-PM, the outer model examines the relationships between observed variables (manifest variables) and the underlying latent constructs they represent. These relationships help us understand how well the observed variables contribute to and define the latent constructs, ultimately aiding in the meaningful interpretation of the relationships between the latent constructs.

3.4. Sexual Attitudes

The outer model shows how different factors contributed to sexual attitudes Specifically, Explicit SA emerged as a crucial positive indicator of sexual attitudes (weight: 0.916), meaning that higher values of Explicit SA are associated with higher Sexual Attitudes. In contrast, both Explicit SDS and Implicit SDS are weak negative indicators (weight: −0.222 and −0.212, respectively), indicating a slight decrease in sexual attitudes with higher levels of these factors. Implicit Sexual Assertiveness (Implicit SA) has a very weak positive impact (weight: 0.144). Overall, sexual attitudes were primarily indicated by Explicit SA.

3.5. Sexual and Interactional Competence

For Sexual and Interactional Competence (SAIC), positive feelings about sex played the most significant role (weight: 1.07). Sexual communication skills (weight: −0.182) and the number of sexual partners (weight: −0.136) had minimal influence. Notably, competence with contraceptive use had almost no impact and was negatively associated (weight: −0.0001). Thus, SAIC was primarily determined by positive feelings about sex.

3.6. Cross-Loadings

Cross-loadings examine how well each observed variable (manifest variable) is associated with the latent constructs they are supposed to measure, as well as with other latent constructs. They help verify the specificity and validity of the constructs. In this model, it pertains to how observed variables that were indicators for sexual attitudes also related to latent SAIC, and vice versa.
Cross-loadings showed (in Figure 1) that the observed variables correlated highly with their intended construct and had low correlations with other constructs, confirming their specificity. An exception was Explicit SA, which showed a strong negative association with sexual attitudes (−0.949) and a modest positive association with SAIC (0.559). Similarly, positive feelings about sex were strongly linked with SAIC and had a modest negative association with Sexual Attitudes. Other cross-loadings were generally low (<0.3) and lower than the loadings on the intended constructs.

3.7. Inner Model

The inner model, which can be seen as the “regression” part of the PLS-PM, examines how sexual attitudes predict SAIC. There was a strong negative relationship (β = −0.589, t(1) = −9.13, p < 0.001), indicating that higher sexual attitudes were linked to lower SAIC.
Combining information from the inner and outer models helps interpret this negative relationship. Given the dominant role of positive feelings about sex in the SAIC construct, with a high weight (1.07), and that sexual attitudes are primarily indicated by explicit SA (weight = 0.916), the negative relationship between SA and SAIC can be largely described as that individuals with higher sexual attitudes, driven by higher explicit sexual assertiveness, tend to have lower SAIC, largely because those more assertive individuals have less positive feelings about sex.

3.8. Multigroup Analysis

Table 3 displays correlations between variables, highlighting differences in the strength of these relationships between men and women. To better understand these differences, we conducted a separate analysis for each group (men and women) using our statistical method (PLS-PM).
The results are presented in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Overall, the models for men and women were similar, but there was a stronger negative path coefficient of sexual attitudes on SAIC in young men (β = −0.77) compared to young women (β = −0.56). Figure 4 illustrates these differences, particularly in how sexual communication skills, Implicit SA, Implicit SDS, and the lifetime number of sexual partners contribute to the models.
Here are some key points:
-
Explicit Sexual Assertiveness (ESA): ESA was a crucial positive indicator for sexual attitudes in both genders. For women, the weight was very high (0.916), and for men, it was even slightly higher (0.931). This shows that ESA played a significant role in shaping sexual attitudes in both men and women.
-
Positive feelings about sex: This variable had a dominant positive contribution to SAIC for both genders, with a weight of 1.086 for women and 0.931 for men, showing that positive feelings about sex played a significant role in SAIC for both men and women.
-
Lifetime number of sexual partners: For young men, having more sexual partners had a small positive contribution to SAIC (0.254). For young women, it had a small negative contribution (−0.213).
-
Sexual communication skills: These skills have a small positive contribution to young women’s SAIC (0.28) but a very small negative contribution for young men (−0.10).
-
Implicit Sexual Double Standard (ISDS): This variable had a negative contribution to SA for women (−0.385) but a positive contribution for men (0.221).
-
Implicit Sexual Assertiveness (ISA): ISA showed a positive contribution to SA for women (0.244) but a negative contribution for men (−0.293).
-
Condom competence: This factor had a minimal positive impact on SAIC for both genders, with a weight of 0.066 for women and 0.125 for men, indicating a relatively minor role in shaping SAIC.

4. Discussion

In this study we examined the interrelationships between sexual attitudes, encompassing SDS and SA on the one hand, and SAIC on the other. Sexual attitudes were measured both as deliberate cognitions and as automatic/implicit associations. We also explored the bivariate associations between the key variables.
In support of our expectations, the association between the latent constructs of sexual attitudes and SAIC (the ‘inner model’) was significant and showed a substantial effect size. The use of PLS-PM enabled us to test a formative measurement model, assuming that the latent construct of SAIC is caused by the latent construct of Sexual Attitudes, in line with general models of behavior regulation that assign a causal role to context-specific attitudes [66,67,68].
Positive feelings about sex made the largest contribution to the latent construct of SAIC, followed by behavioral competence with condom use and sexual communication skills, In contrast, the lifetime number of sexual partners made a very limited contribution, differing from findings in earlier research [13,14].
The contributions of the selected variables to the latent construct of sexual attitudes were found to vary considerably. The largest contribution was made by Explicit SA, followed by Explicit SDS, while the contributions of both implicit variables were small. The findings with regard to explicit attitudes aligned with earlier research on the associations of attitudes and SAIC [18,30,31,34,42,43].
The contribution of the lifetime number of sexual partners to the definition of SAIC was found to be low in the present study. The literature on sexual health in adolescents and emerging adults has suggested that a high number of sexual partners during their lifetime poses a threat to sexual health because—obviously—this increases the risk of acquiring sexually transmitted infections, including HIV, as a result of increased exposure to pathogens [13,14]. Indeed, a high number of sexual partners in adolescence was found to be related to increased risk of acquiring STIs and unintended pregnancy, as well as to absent parental monitoring, contact with delinquent peers, and problematic behavior in studies by Van Ryzin, Johnson [14] and Valois, Oeltmann [13]. However, these researchers did not assess other aspects of SAIC, including Positive feelings about sex, allowing for the possibility that in their studies, the lifetime number of sexual partners was also not related to other, experiential and behavioral aspects of SAIC.
The very low contribution of both implicit attitudes contrasted with our expectations. Previous research demonstrated that implicit associations with sexual stimuli predicted several aspects of sexuality, including sexual functioning [44] and condom use [45]. Unfortunately, the present study does not provide empirical evidence to explain these findings. However, it can be speculated that implicit attitudes toward sexual stimuli influence sexual behavior and sexual responses under conditions in which the attentional capacity required to make informed decisions is greatly reduced by ‘the heat of the moment’ [46,69,70,71]. Lower cognitive processing capacity greatly increases the possible influence of implicit associations on these outcomes [70,71], as the more serial processing of deliberate attitudes requires sufficient attention and ample processing capacity, whereas automatic cognizing is capable of processing parallel streams of information [72], although the absence of this effect has also been found [73]. Implicit attitudes regarding SDS and SA might play a role in earlier phases of behavioral preparation where attitudes guide the perception and interpretation of one’s situation [74], and in which cognitive processing capacity is less taxed.

Strengths and Limitations

The present study used explicit, as well as implicit instruments to measure SDS and SA and included a relatively large sample size. The inclusion of young men in this study is a strength, as the differential effect of sexual attitudes on SAIC in young men, as compared with young women, has, to our knowledge, received scarce research attention (but see [23,75]). Although the employed statistical approach allows testing formative models, in which latent constructs are assumed to be caused by their predictors rather than causing them, the cross-sectional design of the study does not enable strong causal claims. Future studies using a prospective design are warranted to support the indications of a causal path between sexual attitudes and SAIC.

5. Conclusions

We conclude that the present cross-sectional study suggests the existence of a causal path between sexual attitudes and sexual and interactional competence in emerging adults. This is observed most prominently as the effect of sexual attitudes on the level of positive feelings about sex, followed by the effects of sexual attitudes on behavioral competence with condom use and on sexual communication skills. The largest contribution to the latent construct of sexual attitudes is made by explicit sexual assertiveness, followed by explicit endorsement of the sexual double standard, while implicit measures of these constructs contributed very little.

Author Contributions

Methodology, R.P.-E.; Formal analysis, R.P.-E. and J.J.D.M.v.L.; Writing—original draft, A.G. and J.J.D.M.v.L.; Writing—review & editing, A.G., R.P.-E. and J.J.D.M.v.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of University of Utrecht (protocol code FETC-14024, date of approval: 5 February 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The original data presented in the study are openly available in https://osf.io/upxns/?view_only=f437af4db174453eb798367f5d376904 (accessed on 4 September 2024).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Vanwesenbeeck, I.; van Zessen, G.; Ingham, R.; Jaramazovič, E.; Stevens, D. Factors and processes in heterosexual competence and risk: An integrated review of the evidence. Psychol. Health 1999, 14, 25–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lucke, S. Redefining Sexual Competence. In Critical Sexual Literacy; Anthem Press: London, UK, 2021; p. 287. [Google Scholar]
  3. Palmer, M.J.; Clarke, L.; Ploubidis, G.B.; Mercer, C.H.; Gibson, L.J.; Johnson, A.M.; Copas, A.J.; Wellings, K. Is ‘sexual competence’ at first heterosexual intercourse associated with subsequent sexual health status? J. Sex Res. 2017, 54, 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Gold, S.R.; Letourneau, E.J.; O’Donohue, W. Sexual interaction skills. In Handbook of Psychological Skills Training: Clinical Techniques and Applications; O’Donohue, W., Krasner, L., Eds.; Allyn & Bacon: Needham Heights, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 229–246. [Google Scholar]
  5. Blondeel, K.; Toskin, I.; Mirandola, M.; Gios, L.; Temmerman, M. Sexual competence as an indicator of sexual health, results from SIALON II, a European multi-country bio-behavioral survey among men who have sex with men: Karel Blondeel. Eur. J. Public Health 2016, 26, ckw173.004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. House, L.D.; Bates, J.; Markham, C.M.; Lesesne, C. Competence as a predictor of sexual and reproductive health outcomes for youth: A systematic review. J. Adolesc. Health 2010, 46, S7–S22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. WHO. Sexual Health; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  8. Sanchez, D.T.; Crocker, J. How Investment in Gender Ideals Affects Well-Being: The Role of External Contingencies of Self-Worth. Psychol. Women Q. 2005, 29, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. van de Bongardt, D.; de Graaf, H. Youth’s socio-sexual competences with romantic and casual sexual partners. J. Sex Res. 2020, 57, 1166–1179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Tomaszewska, P.; Schuster, I.; Krahé, B. Young adults’ understanding of sexual competence: A qualitative study with German university students. Sex. Res. Soc. Policy A J. NSRC 2022, 20, 145–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hirst, J. Developing sexual competence? Exploring strategies for the provision of effective sexualities and relationships education. Sex Educ. 2008, 8, 399–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Grauvogl, A.; Peters, M.L.; Evers, S.M.A.A.; van Lankveld, J.J.D.M. A new instrument to measure Sexual Competence and Interaction Competence in Youth: Psychometric properties in female adolescents. J. Sex Marital Ther. 2015, 41, 544–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Valois, R.F.; Oeltmann, J.E.; Waller, J.; Hussey, J.R. Relationship between number of sexual intercourse partners and selected health risk behaviors among public high school adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 1999, 25, 328–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Van Ryzin, M.J.; Johnson, A.B.; Leve, L.D.; Kim, H.K. The number of sexual partners and health-risking sexual behavior: Prediction from high school entry to high school exit. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2011, 40, 939–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kok, G.; Gottlieb, N.H.; Peters, G.-J.Y.; Mullen, P.D.; Parcel, G.S.; Ruiter, R.A.C.; Fernández, M.E.; Markham, C.; Bartholomew, L.K. A taxonomy of behaviour change methods: An intervention mapping approach. Health Psychol. Rev. 2016, 10, 297–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Seifen, T.; Shaw, C.M.; Smith, C.V.; Johnson, L.R. The more you know: Sexual knowledge as a predictor of sexual well-being. J. Sex Marital Ther. 2022, 48, 779–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Boislard-Pépin, M.-A.; Green-Demers, I.; Pelletier, L.; Chartrand, J.; Séguin Lévesque, C. L’Impact du style interpersonnel du partenaire sur la compétence, la motivation et la satisfaction sexuelles = The impact of the interpersonal style of the sexual partner on sexual competence, motivation, and satisfaction. Rev. Québécoise Psychol. 2002, 23, 105–121. [Google Scholar]
  18. López-Alvarado, S.; Prekatsounaki, S.; Van Parys, H.; Enzlin, P. Sexual assertiveness and its correlates in emerging adults: An exploratory study in cuenca (ecuador). Int. J. Sex. Health 2022, 34, 679–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Couture, S.; Fernet, M.; Hébert, M.; Guyon, R.; Lévesque, S.; Paradis, A. ‘I just want to feel good without making you feel bad’: Sexual assertiveness negotiation in adolescent romantic relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2023, 52, 3063–3079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Emmerink, P.M.; van den Eijnden, R.J.J.M.; Vanwesenbeeck, I.; ter Bogt, T.F.M. The relationship between endorsement of the sexual double standard and sexual cognitions and emotions. Sex Roles 2016, 75, 363–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sanchez, D.T.; Fetterolf, J.C.; Rudman, L.A. Eroticizing inequality in the United States: The consequences and determinants of traditional gender role adherence in intimate relationships. J. Sex Res. 2012, 49, 168–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Vanwesenbeeck, I. Doing gender in sex and sex research. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2009, 38, 883–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hill, A.L.; Miller, E.; Switzer, G.E.; Abebe, K.Z.; Chang, J.C.; Pulerwitz, J.; Brush, L.D.; Hill, A.V. Gender equitable attitudes among adolescents: A validation study and associations with sexual health behaviors. Adolesc. Res. Rev. 2022, 7, 523–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Truman, D.M.; Tokar, D.M.; Fischer, A.R. Dimensions of masculinity: Relations to date rape supportive attitudes and sexual aggression in dating situations. J. Couns. Dev. 1996, 74, 555–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Zvi, L. The double standard toward female and male victims of non-consensual dissemination of intimate images. J. Interpers. Violence 2022, 37, NP20146–NP20167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Jamshed, N.; Stewart, A.L.; Overstreet, N.M. Social dominance, sexual double standards, and violence against women in tight and loose cultures. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2022, 53, 1145–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shen, A.C.-T.; Chiu, M.Y.-L.; Gao, J. Predictors of dating violence among Chinese adolescents: The role of gender-role beliefs and justification of violence. J. Interpers. Violence 2012, 27, 1066–1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kreager, D.A.; Staff, J.; Gauthier, R.; Lefkowitz, E.S.; Feinberg, M.E. The double standard at sexual debut: Gender, sexual behavior and adolescent peer acceptance. Sex Roles A J. Res. 2016, 75, 377–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Part, K.; Rahu, K.; Rahu, M.; Karro, H. Gender differences in factors associated with sexual intercourse among Estonian adolescents. Scand. J. Public Health 2011, 39, 389–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Smith, G.; Mysak, K.; Michael, S. Sexual double standards and sexually transmitted illnesses: Social rejection and stigmatization of women. Sex Roles A J. Res. 2008, 58, 391–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bermudez, M.P.; Castro, A.; Gude, F.; Buela-Casal, G. Relationship power in the couple and sexual double standard as predictors of the risk of sexually transmitted infections and HIV: Multicultural and gender differences. Curr. HIV Res. 2010, 8, 172–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Amaro, H.D.; Alvarez, M.-J.; Ferreira, J.A. Sexual gender roles and stereotypes and the sexual double standard in sexual satisfaction among Portuguese college students: An exploratory study. Psychol. Sex. 2022, 13, 1350–1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kiefer, A.K.; Sanchez, D.T. Scripting sexual passivity: A gender role perspective. Pers. Rel. 2007, 14, 269–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Study of sexual satisfaction in different typologies of adherence to the sexual double standard. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 609571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Marks, M.J.; Fraley, R.C. The sexual double standard: Fact or fiction? Sex Roles 2005, 52, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Marks, M.J.; Fraley, R.C. Confirmation bias and the sexual double standard. Sex Roles 2006, 54, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jonason, P.K.; Marks, M.J. Common vs. uncommon sexual acts: Evidence for the sexual double standard. Sex Roles 2009, 60, 357–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Milhausen, R.R.; Herold, E.S. Does the sexual double standard still exist? Perceptions of university women. J. Sex Res. 1999, 36, 361–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Milhausen, R.R.; Herold, E.S. Reconceptualizing the sexual double standard. J. Psychol. Hum. Sex. 2001, 13, 63–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sakaluk, J.K.; Milhausen, R.R. Factors influencing university students’ explicit and implicit sexual double standards. J. Sex Res. 2012, 49, 464–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. van Lankveld, J.; Emmerink, P.M.J.; van den Eijnden, R.J.J.M.; ter Bogt, T.F.M.; Pat-El, R.J.; Vanwesenbeeck, I. An implicit measure of sexual double standard endorsement in emerging adults: Reliability and validity aspects. Sex. Cult. Interdiscip. Q. 2022, 26, 729–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Klein, V.; Imhoff, R.; Reininger, K.M.; Briken, P. Perceptions of sexual script deviation in women and men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2019, 48, 631–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Smith, C.V. In pursuit of ‘good’ sex: Self-determination and the sexual experience. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2007, 24, 69–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. van Lankveld, J.; Wolfs, K.; Grauvogl, A. Gender differences in the relationship of sexual functioning with implicit and explicit sex liking and sex wanting: A community sample study. J. Sex Res. 2018, 57, 860–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Czopp, A.M.; Monteith, M.J.; Zimmerman, R.S.; Lyman, D.R. Implicit attitudes as potential protection from risky sex: Predicting condom use with the IAT. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 26, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  46. Wolfs, K.; Bos, A.E.R.; Mevissen, F.E.F.; Peters, G.-J.Y.; Lankveld, J.J.D.M. Sexual arousal and implicit and explicit determinants of condom use intentions. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2019, 48, 469–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wiers, R.W.; Beckers, L.; Houben, K.; Hofmann, W. A short fuse after alcohol: Implicit power associations predict aggressiveness after alcohol consumption in young heavy drinkers with limited executive control. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 2009, 93, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Thush, C.; Wiers, R.W. Explicit and implicit alcohol-related cognitions and the prediction of future drinking in adolescents. Addict. Behav. 2007, 32, 1367–1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chevance, G.; Bernard, P.; Chamberland, P.E.; Rebar, A. The association between implicit attitudes toward physical activity and physical activity behaviour: A systematic review and correlational meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2019, 13, 248–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Roefs, A.; Huijding, J.; Smulders, F.T.; MacLeod, C.M.; de Jong, P.J.; Wiers, R.W.; Jansen, A.T. Implicit measures of association in psychopathology research. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, 149–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Nosek, B.A.; Greenwald, A.G.; Banaji, M.R. The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes; Bargh, J.A., Ed.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 265–292. [Google Scholar]
  52. Lindgren, K.P.; Neighbors, C.; Gasser, M.L.; Ramirez, J.J.; Cvencek, D. A review of implicit and explicit substance self-concept as a predictor of alcohol and tobacco use and misuse. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abus. 2017, 43, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Bartholomew, L.K.; Parcel, G.S.; Kok, G.J.; Gottlieb, N.H.; Fernandez, M.E. Planning Health Promotion Programs: An Intervention Mapping Approach, 3rd ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  54. Greenwald, A.G.; McGhee, D.E.; Schwartz, J.L.K. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. van Lankveld, J.; van den Eijnden, R.; Emmerink, P.; ter Bogt, T.; Pat-El, R.; Vanwesenbeeck, I. An indirect measure of implicit sexual assertiveness: Reliability and validity of the sexual assertiveness IAT in young adults in the Netherlands. J. Sex Marital Ther. 2022, 48, 628–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Emmerink, P.M.; van den Eijnden, R.J.J.M.; ter Bogt, T.F.M.; Vanwesenbeeck, I. A scale for the assessment of sexual standards among youth: Psychometric properties. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2017, 46, 1699–1709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hurlbert, D.F. The role of assertiveness in female sexuality: A comparative study between sexually assertive and sexually nonassertive women. J. Sex Marital Ther. 1991, 17, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Pierce, A.P.; Hurlbert, M.K. Test-retest reliability of the Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1999, 88, 31–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Santos-Iglesias, P.; Sierra, J.C. Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness: A study of psychometric properties in a Spanish sample. Psychol. Rep. 2010, 107, 39–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Santos-Iglesias, P.; Vallejo-Medina, P.; Sierra, J.C. Equivalencia y baremos del Hurlbert Index of sexual assertiveness entre hombres y mujeres españoles. An. Psicol. 2014, 30, 232–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Houben, K.; Wiers, R.W. Measuring implicit alcohol associations via the Internet: Validation of Web-based implicit association tests. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2008, 40, 1134–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R.; Greenwald, A.G. Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 2002, 6, 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. van Buuren, S.; Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. Mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 45, 1–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  65. Hanafiah, M.H. Formative vs. reflective measurement model: Guidelines for structural equation modeling research. Int. J. Anal. Appl. 2020, 18, 876–889. [Google Scholar]
  66. Ajzen, I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In Action Control: From Cognition to Behavior; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  67. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Predicting and Changing Behavior: The Reasoned Action Approach; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  68. Albarracin, D.; Johnson, B.T.; Fishbein, M.; Muellerleile, P.A. Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 142–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ariely, D.; Loewenstein, G. The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2006, 19, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Hofmann, W.; Friese, M.; Strack, F. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 4, 162–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Strack, F.; Deutsch, R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 8, 220–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Salemink, E.; Friese, M.; Drake, E.; Mackintosh, B.; Hoppitt, L. Indicators of implicit and explicit social anxiety influence threat-related interpretive bias as a function of working memory capacity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Van Bockstaele, B.; Tibboel, H.; Larsen, H.; Wiers, R.W.; Bögels, S.M.; Salemink, E. Dual processes in fear and anxiety: No effects of cognitive load on the predictive value of implicit measures. Cogn. Emot. 2021, 35, 859–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Fazio, R.H.; Roskos-Ewoldsen, D.R. Acting as We Feel: When and How Attitudes Guide Behavior. In Persuasion: Psychological Insights and Perspectives, 2nd ed.; Brock, T.C., Green, M.C., Eds.; Sage Publications, Inc: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2005; pp. 41–62. [Google Scholar]
  75. Smulders, L.; Bos, H.; Stams, G.; Konijn, C.; Koolhof, R.; Assink, M. Measuring sexual interaction competence in adolescents: A validation study of the sexual interaction and relationship questionnaire (SIRQ). Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2024, 163, 107813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Path model in the full sample.
Figure 1. Path model in the full sample.
Behavsci 14 00790 g001
Figure 2. Path model in the female subsample.
Figure 2. Path model in the female subsample.
Behavsci 14 00790 g002
Figure 3. Path model in the male subsample.
Figure 3. Path model in the male subsample.
Behavsci 14 00790 g003
Figure 4. Comparison of path loadings in the female and male subsamples. ESA: Explicit Sexual Assertiveness; ESDS: Explicit Sexual Double Standard endorsement; ISA: Implicit Sexual Assertiveness; ISDS: Implicit Sexual Double Standard endorsement.
Figure 4. Comparison of path loadings in the female and male subsamples. ESA: Explicit Sexual Assertiveness; ESDS: Explicit Sexual Double Standard endorsement; ISA: Implicit Sexual Assertiveness; ISDS: Implicit Sexual Double Standard endorsement.
Behavsci 14 00790 g004
Table 1. Demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics.
Men (M(SD); %)Women (M(SD); %)
Age21.8 (1.9)22.2 (1.9)
Sexually Attracted To 1
  Only Men9.169.8
  Mostly Men1.824.0
  Men and Women3.64.2
  Mostly Women10.90.0
  Only Women72.71.0
  I Don’t Know (Yet)0.01
  I Don’t Want to Disclose1.80.0
Education Level
  Lower6.32.2
  Intermediate70.860.7
  Higher 22.937.1
Relationship Status 2
  Single38.229.2
  Dating20.011.3
  Committed Relationship40.054.3
  Married1.82.0
1: χ2(6) = 116.798, p < 0.001; 2: χ2(3) = 10.020, p < 0.05.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations among young men and women.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations among young men and women.
Full Sample
(n = 126–153)
Young Men
(n = 43–55)
Young Women
(n = 82–98)
95% CI
Gender Difference
MSDMSDMSDLowerUpper
Sexual communication skills (SCICY) ***2.800.453.020.332.680.460.210.47
Positive feelings about sex (SCICY) *4.210.844.360.584.110.950.030.52
Behavioral competence with condom use (SCICY)3.851.183.931.143.811.21−0.280.52
Lifetime number of sexual partners6.543.997.127.076.216.97−1.563.37
Implicit SDS Endorsement (IAT-SDS; seconds) ***−0.010.39−0.160.400.070.35−0.37−0.10
Implicit Sexual Assertiveness (SA-IAT; seconds) *−0.120.39−0.250.39−0.060.37−0.32−0.04
Explicit SDS Endorsement (SASSY)2.230.572.320.662.180.51−0.070.34
Explicit Sexual Assertiveness (HISA) *2.930.663.060.532.850.720.000.41
* p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001; SCICY: Sexual Competence and Interaction Competence in Youth; SASSY: Scale for the Assessment of Sexual Standards in Youth; HISA: Adapted Hurlbert Index of Sexual Assertiveness.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations in the full sample and the female and male subsamples.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations in the full sample and the female and male subsamples.
12345678
Full Sample (N = 115–153)
1 Sexual communication skills (SCICY)-
2 Positive feelings about sex (SCICY)0.411 c-
3 Behavioral competence with condom use (SCICY)0.292 c0.341 c-
4 Lifetime number of sexual partners0.1060.125−0.095-
5 Implicit SDS Endorsement (IAT-SDS; seconds)−0.023−0.1110.1050.094-
6 Implicit Sexual Assertiveness (SA-IAT; seconds)−0.067−0.0140.030−0.1370.077-
7 Explicit SDS Endorsement (SASSY)−0.001−0.199 a−0.116−0.042−0.148−0.006-
8 Explicit Sexual Assertiveness (HISA)0.161 a0.539 c0.204 a0.007−0.024−0.150−0.207 a-
Female (Top Right: N = 74–98) and Male (Bottom Left: N = 41–55) Subsamples
1 Sexual communication skills (SCICY)-0.486 c0.299 b0.1890.057−0.0450.0320.197
2 Positive feelings about sex (SCICY)−0.024-0.380 c0.159−0.1060.057−0.1880.483 c
3 Behavioral competence with condom use (SCICY)0.292 a0.235-0.0130.0470.046−0.0980.251 a
4 Lifetime number of sexual partners−0.149−0.013−0.311 a-0.191−0.071−0.114−0.049
5 Implicit SDS Endorsement (IAT-SDS; seconds)0.1610.0360.2360.010-−0.158−0.0470.123
6 Implicit Sexual Assertiveness (SA-IAT; seconds)0.163−0.0660.012−0.2420.281-0.067−0.169
7 Explicit SDS Endorsement (SASSY)−0.203−0.337 a−0.1610.050−0.231−0.056-−0.245 a
8 Explicit Sexual Assertiveness (HISA)−0.1630.710 c0.0700.105−0.1920.028−0.219-
a = p < 0.05; b = p < 0.01; c = p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Grauvogl, A.; Pat-El, R.; van Lankveld, J.J.D.M. Associations of Implicit and Explicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement and Sexual Assertiveness with Sexual and Interactional Competence in Emerging Adults. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 790. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090790

AMA Style

Grauvogl A, Pat-El R, van Lankveld JJDM. Associations of Implicit and Explicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement and Sexual Assertiveness with Sexual and Interactional Competence in Emerging Adults. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(9):790. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090790

Chicago/Turabian Style

Grauvogl, Andrea, Ron Pat-El, and Jacques J. D. M. van Lankveld. 2024. "Associations of Implicit and Explicit Sexual Double Standard Endorsement and Sexual Assertiveness with Sexual and Interactional Competence in Emerging Adults" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 9: 790. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090790

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop