The Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Development of Intertemporal Decision Making
3. Group Intertemporal Decision Making
3.1. Influence of the Social Environment on Intertemporal Decision Making
3.2. Decision Outcomes of Group Intertemporal Decision Making
3.2.1. Comparison of Group and Individual Intertemporal Decision-Making Outcomes
3.2.2. Factors Influencing Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Outcomes
3.3. Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process
3.3.1. Interpersonal Interactive Process
3.3.2. Information-Processing Processes
4. Future Research Prospects: Based on the “Two Processes” of Group Intertemporal Decision Making
4.1. Mechanism of Differences between Group and Individual Intertemporal Decision Making in Terms of Interpersonal Interaction Processes
4.2. Mechanism of Group Intertemporal Decision Making in Terms of Information Processing
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Frederick, S.; Loewenstein, G.; O’donoghue, T. Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. J. Econ. Lit. 2002, 40, 351–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Read, D.; McDonald, R.; He, L. Intertemporal choice: Choosing for the future. In The Cambridge Handbook of Psychology and Economic Behavior; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2018; pp. 167–197. [Google Scholar]
- Sun, D.Q.; Xin, Z.Q. Research paradigm and decision quality evaluation for group decision making. Psychol. Tech. Appl. 2017, 5, 628–637. [Google Scholar]
- Hamada, D.; Nakayama, M.; Saiki, J. Wisdom of crowds and collective decision-making in a survival situation with complex information integration. Cognit. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020, 5, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laughlin, P.R.; Hatch, E.C.; Silver, J.S.; Boh, L. Groups perform better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers problems: Effects of group size. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 90, 644–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, P. Science and value: Risk decision-making mechanism and its optimization in the context of the COVID-19. Gov. Stud. 2020, 36, 51–58. [Google Scholar]
- Green, L.; Myerson, J. Exponential versus hyperbolic discounting of delayed outcomes: Risk and waiting time. Am. Zool. 1996, 36, 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Estle, S.J.; Green, L.; Myerson, J.; Holt, D.D. Discounting of monetary and directly consumable rewards. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 58–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, H.; Guo, Y.; Yu, Q. Self-control makes the difference: The psychological mechanism of dual processing model on internet addicts’ unusual behavior in intertemporal choice. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 101, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, C.M.; Hu, F.P.; Zhu, L.F. Introducing upfront losses as well as gains decreases impatience in intertemporal choices with rewards. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2014, 9, 297–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malliet, P.; Reynès, F.; Landa, G. Assessing short-term and long-term economic and environmental effects of the COVID-19 crisis in France. Environ. Resource Econ. 2020, 76, 867–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Du, T.Y.; Hu, X.Y.; Yang, J.; Li, L.Y.; Wang, T.T. Low socioeconomic status and intertemporal choice: The mechanism of “psychological-shift” from the perspective of threat. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 30, 1894–1904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Wal, A.J.; van Horen, F.; Grinstein, A. Temporal myopia in sustainable behavior under uncertainty. Int. J. Res. Market. 2018, 35, 378–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, X.J.; Chen, J.F.; Fu, J.H.; Li, S.; Liang, Z.Y. Time preferences for public health and health behavior. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 28, 1926–1938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koffman, J.; Gross, J.; Etkind, S.N. Uncertainty and COVID-19: How are we to respond? J. R. Soc. Med. 2020, 113, 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szczygielski, J.J.; Charteris, A.; Bwanya, P.R. The impact and role of COVID-19 uncertainty: A global industry analysis. Int. Rev. Financ. Anal. 2022, 80, 101837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, X.W.; Wu, Z.Y.; He, Q.H. A mini-review on how the COVID-19 pandemic affected intertemporal choice. Psychoradiology 2023, 3, kkad021. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, D.; Nie, X.; Zhou, Y.; Ye, J.; Yu, P.; Hu, Y.; Jin, X. The influence of COVID-19 on intertemporal choices in the health and economy domains. Heliyon 2024, 10, e31801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geng, X.W.; Zhang, K.; Liu, Y. The influence of public crisis on university students’ intertemporal choice: The moderate of subjective socioeconomic status. Chin. J. Appl. Psychol. 2022, 28, 449–456. [Google Scholar]
- Bulley, A.; Miloyan, B.; Pepper, G.V.; Gullo, M.J.; Henry, J.D.; Suddendorf, T. Cuing both positive and negative episodic foresight reduces delay discounting but does not affect risk-taking. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2019, 72, 1998–2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, Y.P.; Jiang, C.M.; Hu, T.Y.; Sun, H.Y. Effects of emotion on intertemporal decision-making: Explanation from the single dimension priority model. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2022, 54, 122–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loya, J.M.; Roth, M.R.; Yi, R. An examination of group size and valence in delay discounting of shared outcomes. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 673–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Read, D.; Olivola, C.Y.; Hardisty, D.J. The value of nothing: Asymmetric attention to opportunity costs drives intertemporal decision making. Manag. Sci. 2017, 63, 4277–4297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paul, A.; Samuelson, A. Note on measurement of utility. Rev. Econ. Stud. 1937, 4, 155–161. [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein, G.; Prelec, D. Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. Q. J. Econ. 1992, 107, 573–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laibson, D. Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Q. J. Econ. 1997, 112, 443–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benzion, U.; Rapoport, A.; Yagil, J. Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Manag. Sci. 1989, 35, 270–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prelec, D.; Loewenstein, G. Decision making over time and under uncertainty: A common approach. Manag. Sci. 1991, 37, 770–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thaler, R.H.; Shefrin, H.M. An economic theory of self-control. J. Polit. Econ. 1981, 89, 392–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, S.; Su, Y.; Sun, Y. The effect of pseudo-immediacy on intertemporal choices. J. Risk Res. 2010, 13, 781–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Y.; Li, S. The effect of risk on intertemporal choice. J. Risk Res. 2010, 13, 805–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.F.; Gigerenzer, G.; Todd, P.M. Fast and frugal heuristics: Simple decision rules based on bounded rationality and ecological rationality. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 26, 56–59. [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer, G.; Goldstein, D.G. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 650–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholten, M.; Read, D. The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 925–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Li, S. A behavioral choice model when computational ability matters. Appl. Intell. 2004, 20, 147–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Farrar, Straus and Giroux: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Costa, D.; Arantes, J.; Keating, J. A dual-process approach to cooperative decision-making under uncertainty. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0265759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grayot, J.D. Dual process theories in behavioral economics and neuroeconomics: A critical review. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 2020, 11, 105–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diederich, A.; Zhao, W.J. A dynamic dual process model of intertemporal choice. Span. J. Psychol. 2019, 22, E54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, C.C.; Chen, B.; Liu, L.X.; Yuan, X.X.; Wang, Z.J. Does standers-by always see more than gamesters? A review on the self-other decision making differences. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 21, 879–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonas, E.; Frey, D. Information search and presentation in advisor–client interactions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2003, 91, 154–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polman, E. Information distortion in self-other decision making. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 46, 432–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albrecht, K.; Volz, K.G.; Sutter, M.; Laibson, D.I.; Von Cramon, D.Y. What is for me is not for you: Brain correlates of intertemporal choice for self and other. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 2011, 6, 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Oliveira, A.C.; Jacobson, S. (Im) patience by proxy: Making intertemporal decisions for others. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2021, 182, 83–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, H.Y.; Cui, L.P.; Li, D. Negative discounting: The self-other discrepancy in intertemporal choice. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 39, 970–976. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Z.J.; Kuang, Y.; Tang, H.Y.; Gao, C.; Chen, A.; Chan, K.Q. Are decisions made by group representatives more risk averse? The effect of sense of responsibility. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2018, 31, 311–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weatherly, J.N.; Ruthig, J.C. Degree of delay discounting as a function of who receives the outcome and the discounter’s perceived level of social support. Curr. Psychol. 2013, 32, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, K.; Chein, J.; Steinberg, L. Adolescents in peer groups make more prudent decisions when a slightly older adult is present. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 27, 322–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilman, J.M.; Curran, M.T.; Calderon, V.; Stoeckel, L.E.; Evins, A.E. Impulsive social influence increases impulsive choices on a temporal discounting task in young adults. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e101570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kedia, G.; Brohmer, H.; Scholten, M.; Corcoran, K. Improving self-control: The influence of role models on intertemporal choices. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bixter, M.T.; Trimber, E.M.; Luhmann, C.C. Are intertemporal preferences contagious? Evidence from collaborative decision making. Mem. Cognit. 2017, 45, 837–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sun, M.H. The Influence of Group Discussion and Group Intertemporal Tendencies Composition on Intertemporal Decision-Making. Master’s Thesis, Ludong University, Yantai, China, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Bixter, M.T.; Rogers, W.A. Age-related differences in delay discounting: Immediate reward, reward magnitude, and social influence. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2019, 32, 471–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bixter, M.T.; Luhmann, C.C. Delay discounting in dyads and small groups: Group leadership, status information, and actor-partner interdependence. J Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 86, 103902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, T.Y. Group Intertemporal Decision Making in the Domains of Money and Environment. Master’s Thesis, Zhejiang University, Yantai, China, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Tsuruta, M.; Inukai, K. How are individual time preferences aggregated in groups? A laboratory experiment on intertemporal group decision-making. Front. Appl. Math. Stat. 2018, 4, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Schwenke, D.; Wehner, P.; Scherbaum, S. Effects of individual and dyadic decision-making and normative reference on delay discounting decisions. Cognit. Res. Princ. Implic. 2022, 7, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batteux, E.; Ferguson, E.; Tunney, R.J. Do our risk preferences change when we make decisions for others? A meta-analysis of self-other differences in decisions involving risk. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0216566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glätzle-Rützler, D.; Lergetporer, P.; Sutter, M. Collective intertemporal decisions and heterogeneity in groups. Games Econ. Behav. 2021, 130, 131–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bi, P.C. Effects of leadership style on group decision making process and outcome: A laboratory experiment. Bus. Manag. J. 2010, 32, 80–84. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, L.P.; Xin, Z.Q.; Sun, D.Q. The evaluation methods of group decision-making and their applications among college students and community residents. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 28, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granovetter, M. Threshold models of collective behavior. Am. J. Sociol. 1978, 83, 1420–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwenke, D.; Dshemuchadse, M.; Vesper, C.; Bleichner, M.G.; Scherbaum, S. Let’s decide together: Differences between individual and joint delay discounting. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumann, M.R.; Bonner, B.L. The effects of variability and expectations on utilization of member expertise and group performance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2004, 93, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, D.A.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J.; Pliner, P. Self-presentational conflict in social eating situations: A normative perspective. Appetite 2001, 36, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Senecal, N.; Wang, T.; Thompson, E.; Kable, J.W. Normative arguments from experts and peers reduce delay discounting. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2012, 7, 568–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fukutomi, M.; Ito, N.; Mitani, Y. How Group Size and Decision Rules Impact Risk Preferences: Comparing group and individual settings in lottery-choice experiments. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2022, 98, 101873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morone, A.; Nuzzo, S.; Temerario, T. Decision process and preferences over risk under the “endogenous decision rule”: Results from a group experiment. Econ. Bus. Lett. 2021, 10, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schippers, M.C.; Rus, D.C. Majority decision-making works best under conditions of leadership ambiguity and shared task representations. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 2305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeng, J.H.; He, G.B. Advances in research on constructive group decision making process. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 11, 686–691. [Google Scholar]
- Xiao, Y.; Zhang, H.; Basadur, T.M. Does information sharing always improve team decision making? An examination of the hidden profile condition in new product development. J. Bus. Res. 2016, 69, 587–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vuong, Q.H. Mindsponge Theory; Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Sciendo: Warsaw, Poland, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Vuong, Q.H.; Nguyen, M.H. Better Economics for the Earth: A Lesson from Quantum and Information Theories; AISDL: Hanoi, Vietnam, 2024. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sun, H.-Y.; Xiao, Y.-T.; Yang, S.-S. The Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 815. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090815
Sun H-Y, Xiao Y-T, Yang S-S. The Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(9):815. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090815
Chicago/Turabian StyleSun, Hong-Yue, Yi-Ting Xiao, and Shan-Shan Yang. 2024. "The Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 9: 815. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090815
APA StyleSun, H. -Y., Xiao, Y. -T., & Yang, S. -S. (2024). The Group Intertemporal Decision-Making Process. Behavioral Sciences, 14(9), 815. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090815