Next Article in Journal
Behavioural Systems Mapping of Solid Waste Management in Kisumu, Kenya, to Understand the Role of Behaviour in a Health and Sustainability Problem
Next Article in Special Issue
Correction: Muñoz-Aucapiña et al. (2025). Psychometric Validation of the Dating Violence Questionnaire (DVQ-R) in Ecuadorians. Behavioral Sciences, 15(1), 68
Previous Article in Journal
Building a Positive Work Environment: The Role of Psychological Empowerment in Engagement and Intention to Leave
Previous Article in Special Issue
Psychometric Validation of the Dating Violence Questionnaire (DVQ-R) in Ecuadorians
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

What Is the Link of Closeness and Jealousy in Romantic Relationships?

by
Ana María Fernández
1,*,
Maria Teresa Barbato
1,
Pamela Barone
2,
Belén Zavalla
1,
Diana Rivera-Ottenberger
3 and
Mónica Guzmán-González
4
1
Laboratorio de Evolución y Relaciones Interpersonales, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Santiago 9170022, Chile
2
Department of Psychology, Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122 Palma, Spain
3
School of Psychology, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago 7820436, Chile
4
School of Psychology, Universidad Católica del Norte, Antofagasta 1270709, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(2), 132; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020132
Submission received: 3 December 2024 / Revised: 7 January 2025 / Accepted: 23 January 2025 / Published: 26 January 2025

Abstract

:
From an evolutionary perspective, love and attachment foster closeness, while jealousy ensures exclusivity in romantic relationships. This study examined the links between jealousy and affective aspects of love, hypothesizing positive associations despite their apparent opposition. An online sample of 265 individuals in Chile and Spain completed measures of digital jealousy, closeness, love, felt loved, and attachment. Results revealed higher jealousy in Chile than in Spain. Across both countries, anxious attachment and closeness were significant predictors of jealousy, explaining nearly 30% of its variance. In Chile, feeling loved negatively predicted jealousy, suggesting that reassurance of the romantic bond may reduce jealousy in this cultural context. Notably, affective closeness—conceptualized as the inclusion of the self in the other—emerged as a novel predictor of jealousy, extending beyond the established role of anxious attachment. These findings underscore the nuanced interplay between cultural context, affective closeness, and attachment in shaping jealousy.

1. Introduction

1.1. Love and Attachment

Love is a complex and multifaceted emotion that plays a crucial role in promoting enduring social bonds, especially within romantic relationships (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986). From an evolutionary perspective, romantic love is a mechanism that encourages long-term commitment, facilitating caregiving and protection of offspring, both of which are essential for survival and reproductive success (Carter & Porges, 2013; Sorokowski et al., 2017). Love extends beyond romantic partnerships, influencing family and friendship dynamics, where consistent emotional closeness and actions that convey love are essential (Xia et al., 2023). In this context, the emotional connection one shares with others is crucial for feeling loved, not just within romantic relationships but across various forms of social bonds.
The theory of attachment supports this understanding by emphasizing that early relational experiences with caregivers shape mental models that influence adult relationships (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2017). These models underline how trust, emotional security, and mutual support within relationships foster emotional closeness, making such connections indispensable (Bowlby, 1982; Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Attachment bonds are fundamental not only in parent–child relationships but also in adult romantic relationships, reinforcing that emotional security and attachment are key to sustaining love over time (Eastwick, 2016; Ein-Dor, 2015). Yet, attachment theory is just one lens through which love can be understood. In addition, Fredrickson’s (2013) theory of “positivity resonance” broadens the view of love, proposing it as a dynamic, momentary connection rooted in shared positive emotions, biobehavioral synchrony, and mutual care. This theory aligns with the idea that emotional closeness integrates a partner into one’s sense of self, strengthening the bond (Aron et al., 1992). Secure attachment further reinforces positive emotional resonance, supporting the notion that love involves a complex interplay of emotional security and mutual care (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2017).
Despite the wide range of components that characterize love, the most commonly utilized framework for its measurement is Sternberg’s Triangular Theory of Love. This theory portrays love in three dimensions: intimacy, passion, and commitment, which can be ranked to convey different types of love (Sternberg, 1986). Intimacy refers to feelings of closeness and emotional connection, passion relates to physical attraction and desire, and commitment involves the decision to maintain a relationship over time. Stenberg’s model has significantly advanced the characterization of romantic relationships by providing a comprehensive structure to understand the dynamics of love cross-culturally (Cassepp-Borges et al., 2023; Kowal et al., 2024).
Moreover, the dimension of intimacy emphasizes that feelings of being loved are deeply rooted in mutual understanding and responsiveness between partners. This dimension can be measured through aspects of momentary connection, fostering deeper bonds and empathy within the relationship (Fredrickson, 2013; Gonzaga et al., 2006; Reis & Aron, 2008).
However, there is a complementary perspective on the nature of love, emphasizing the role of neuropsychological and emotional responses in shaping romantic experiences. For example, the concept of “feeling loved” highlights individual perceptions of love based on momentary emotional interactions, which may not always align with the structural components of intimacy, passion, and commitment (Ellis et al., 2022; Heshmati et al., 2017; Reis & Aron, 2008). The dynamics of love appear to be shaped not only by long-term relational structures but also by transient, emergent experiences of closeness and affection (Rykkje et al., 2015).
Recent research underscores the importance of measuring the subjective experience of “feeling loved” as a critical dimension in understanding relationships (Sasaki et al., 2023). This perspective enriches relational bonds by emphasizing the nuanced, reciprocal, and momentary aspects of love. Integrating this measurement with established theories deepens our understanding of how love operates across various relational contexts.
On the other hand, several studies have revealed that the way romantic love is experienced and expressed varies significantly across cultures and is shaped by societal norms and traditions (Karandashev, 2015). In Western cultures, love is often expressed explicitly through verbal affirmations and grand gestures, while in Chinese and Filipino traditions, it is more commonly conveyed through implicit actions and subtle behaviors (Nadal, 2012). Emotional investment also shows cultural differences, with North Americans tending to express higher levels of affection and passion compared to the more reserved approaches typical in East Asia (Schmitt et al., 2009). Moreover, cultural attitudes toward love differ, as more individualistic societies, such as Spain compared to Chile (Insights, 2020) (https://www.hofstede-insights.com/), emphasize autonomy and passionate connections, whereas collectivistic cultures prioritize familial bonds and societal harmony (Dion & Dion, 2006).
Across the majority of these studies, the extensive use of instruments enhances the ability to assess phenomena effectively, yet it remains unclear what specific aspects or characteristics of love are being evaluated. For instance, some cross-cultural studies on love employ standardized tools such as emotional investment scales to assess levels of affection, passion, and compassion across cultures (Schmitt et al., 2009). Similarly, De Munck et al. (2011) examined dimensions like altruism and physical attraction using self-report measures, while Sprecher et al. (1994) explored cross-cultural differences in the intensity of love through direct surveys. Longitudinal studies, such as those by Ingersoll-Dayton et al. (1996), have analyzed the evolution of romantic relationships over time, capturing shifts from initial intimacy to later emotional development in various cultural contexts. These tools, combined with ethnographic approaches, provide valuable insights into how universal emotions like love are shaped by cultural norms, revealing both shared and unique elements across societies.
Love not only fosters emotional security, mutual care, and intimacy but also evokes a range of powerful emotions that differentiate and define human relationships, from infancy through adulthood (Bode, 2023; Kowal et al., 2024). Among them, jealousy stands out as a pivotal emotion, intricately tied to the dynamics of close relationships and deeply influencing how love is experienced and expressed (Sullivan, 2021).

1.2. Jealousy

Jealousy represents a pivotal emotion in close human relationships, primarily examined in children within the framework of attachment theory (Bowlby, 1982; Fernández et al., 2022; Hart, 2010), as well as in the contexts of friendships and sibling dynamics (Rodriguez et al., 2015; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2017). Although often characterized as a troublesome emotion (Rodriguez et al., 2015), jealousy also plays a complementary role to love, serving to preserve romantic bonds (Fernández et al., 2023). From an evolutionary perspective, jealousy functions as a defense mechanism aimed at safeguarding relationships from perceived threats (Chung & Harris, 2018), particularly infidelity or emotional disengagement (Buss, 2013; Schützwohl, 2008). Commonly regarded as a reaction to the fear of losing a partner’s affection (Buunk, 1997), jealousy fulfills an adaptive function by protecting the emotional and reproductive resources essential for sustaining long-term relationships (Buss, 2018). In this regard, love and jealousy operate synergistically to stabilize romantic partnerships (Conroy-Beam et al., 2015; Fletcher, 2015).
Research on jealousy has concentrated on its evolutionary and functional aspects, while also exploring the cultural and sexual variations that influence its expression. Cultural dimensions, such as collectivism and individualism, have been shown to influence how jealousy is experienced. Zandbergen and Brown (2015) found that collectivist societies prioritize relational harmony, often perceiving jealousy in the context of maintaining group stability, whereas individualistic cultures emphasize personal autonomy, framing jealousy as a threat to individual relationships. These cultural differences have been measured using tools like the Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism Scale (Singelis et al., 1995), which provides valuable insights into the broader social frameworks that modulate emotional responses to jealousy.
In the context of romantic relationships, jealousy has been identified as a crucial emotion for protecting mating bonds (Buunk et al., 2019; Fernandez, 2017). It has primarily been studied through hypothetical infidelity scenarios and retrospective accounts of distress related to actual infidelity (Buss et al., 1992; Buss, 2018; Schützwohl, 2008). For example, men tend to exhibit greater distress by sexual infidelity, aligning with evolutionary concerns about paternity certainty, while women report heightened emotional jealousy, often linked to perceived threats to emotional commitment (Buss et al., 1992; Buunk & Fernandez, 2020; Larsen et al., 2021). These patterns have been captured using tools like the modified Emotional and Sexual Jealousy Scale (Buss et al., 1992) and the Self-Report Jealousy Scale (Bringle et al., 1979), which quantify reactions to emotional and sexual threats. Experimental methods, including recalling personal betrayal experiences, exposure to attractive rivals, dramatized depictions in media (e.g., Cheaters), and subliminal priming with infidelity-related cues, have further highlighted the nuanced ways in which jealousy is elicited and expressed (Kuhle, 2011; Massar & Buunk, 2010; Sabini & Green, 2004).
Jealousy poses a significant threat to intimate relationships, as it is often considered a maladaptive and pathological emotion rooted in insecurity and negative self-esteem (Chin et al., 2017; Buss & Abrams, 2017). Previous research has linked jealousy to traits like narcissism, psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and low self-worth (Costa et al., 2015; Massar et al., 2017; Seeman, 2016). Despite its association with psychological distress and interpersonal conflict, jealousy remains universally prevalent, even in cultural contexts where traditional explanations—such as male dominance, aggression, or female emotional vulnerability—fail to fully explain its persistence and intensity (Kruger et al., 2013; Scelza, 2014). This paradox highlights the complexity of jealousy as an emotional phenomenon.
Qualitative approaches have complemented these findings by uncovering deeper themes, such as infidelity, expectations of time and commitment, self-esteem, and the influence of social media (Zandbergen & Brown, 2015). Additionally, innovative methods like economic games, which manipulate resource allocation between romantic partners and rivals, have provided new ways to study jealousy by offering real-time insights into emotional responses (Barbato et al., 2024). The rise of social media has introduced another layer, amplifying relational threats and rivalries in the digital sphere, and making it an increasingly important area of study (Tandon et al., 2021; Tukachinsky Forster, 2022).

1.3. Relationship Between Love and Jealousy

Therefore, both love and jealousy play complementary roles in promoting and preserving pair bonds (Chung & Harris, 2018). Love fosters prosocial behaviors and mutual benefits between reproductive partners (Fletcher, 2015), while jealousy helps prevent the loss of these benefits in the face of potential interlopers (Buss, 2018; Harris, 2003). These emotions together support the maintenance of long-term commitments, contributing to the evolutionary success of pair bonding by ensuring the continuity of shared resources, care, and reproductive fitness (Conroy-Beam et al., 2015; Cosmides & Tooby, 2013).
The relationship between love and jealousy is influenced by various factors such as attachment style (Attridge, 2013; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), given the convergence of both constructs in the development of close bonds (Fernández et al., 2023). Evidence suggests that high attachment anxiety is strongly associated with increased jealousy (Sullivan, 2021). More specifically, individuals with anxious attachment often experience heightened suspicion, insecurity about their partner’s availability, and concerns about potential abandonment, all of which can trigger jealousy (Collins & Feeney, 2000; Deng et al., 2023; Güçlü et al., 2017; Montalvini et al., 2014; Ng & Hou, 2017). In contrast, attachment avoidance—a tendency toward emotional independence due to early neglect or a lack of sensitive care—leads to self-reliance and discomfort with affection, often resulting in reduced jealousy (Lafontaine et al., 2018; Montalvini et al., 2014).
Therefore, the present research aims to explore the interplay between jealousy, emotional closeness, and love across two distinct cultural contexts: Chile and Spain. Drawing on attachment theory and theories of romantic love, this study examines how jealousy relates to key relational variables that influence the stability and dynamics of romantic relationships. Specifically, it is hypothesized that jealousy will show positive associations with emotional closeness, love (including its dimensions of intimacy, passion, and commitment), and attachment anxiety, as these variables are intertwined in shaping individuals’ emotional responses. By exploring these dynamics, this study seeks to shed light on the role jealousy plays within the broader context of love, and how attachment, emotional closeness, and cultural factors shape the experience and expression of jealousy in romantic relationships.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study employed a cross-sectional design with a convenience sample of 265 romantically involved individuals who completed all measures online using the PsyToolkit platform (Stoet, 2010). Participants were recruited through advertisements on social networks, resulting in a sample composed predominantly of heterosexual individuals (74.3%), of whom 71.9% were women. Only 29.2% of participants reported having children.
The sample included participants from Chile (40%) and Spain (60%). In the Chilean subsample, 78% were women compared to 72% in the Spanish subsample. There was a significantly shorter relationship duration in months among participants from Chile (M = 46.97, SD = 6.46) compared to those from Spain (M = 89.18, SD = 15.63; t231.255 = −2.494, p < 0.05). No significant differences were found between the two countries in terms of age or sex across any of the assessed variables (Fs < 1.43, ps > 0.05).

2.2. Measures

Sociodemographic questions. Participants provided information about their age, sex, employment status, duration of their current relationship, and number of children.
Jealousy. The digital jealousy scale (DJS) was used to measure jealousy in digital contexts (Gubler et al., 2023). This 9-item scale covers cognitive, affective, and behavioral aspects of jealousy. Participants were instructed to think about their current relationship and indicate their level of agreement with each statement on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 6 = strongly agree). Example items include “it worries me when a new woman/man appears on my partner’s friends list” or “I look over my partner’s shoulder when I know they are texting with someone else”.
The original scale was developed in both German and English. For the current study, the items were translated into Spanish from the English version by a bilingual team, and a back-translation process was employed to ensure accuracy. In the original study, the DJS showed high construct reliability (McDonald’s ω = 0.89 in the German sample and McDonald’s ω = 0.90 in the English sample). Cronbach’s alpha for the adapted DJS was 0.90. Gubler et al. (2023) found that digital jealousy and romantic jealousy (measured by a scale that does not refer to the context of social media) showed a very strong positive correlation (r = 0.86).
Love. The short version of the Triangular Love Scale (TLS-15) was used to measure love (Kowal et al., 2024). Based on Sternberg’s triangular theory of love, the scale evaluates three core components, intimacy, passion, and commitment, commonly measured in romantic relationships. The abbreviated version consists of 15 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never; 5 = always). Example items of each subscale include “I have a warm relationship with my partner” (intimacy), “I find my partner to be very personally attractive” (passion), and “I view my commitment to my partner as a solid one” (commitment).
The Spanish version of the TLS-15 has been previously validated and adapted for both Latin American and Spanish populations, showing high reliability in both contexts. In the Latin American sample, the reliability scores were intimacy: α = 0.92, ω = 0.94; passion: α = 0.89, ω = 0.91; and commitment: α = 0.90, ω = 0.93. In the Spanish sample, the scores were intimacy: α = 0.91, ω = 0.93; passion: α = 0.88, ω = 0.90; and commitment: α = 0.90, ω = 0.90 (Kowal et al., 2024). For the present sample, the reliability scores for the TLS-15 were the following: intimacy: α = 0.86; passion: α = 0.81; and commitment: α = 0.83.
Closeness. An adapted version of the inclusion of other in the self (IOS) scale (Aron et al., 1992), a valid measure of perceived closeness between individuals (Gächter et al., 2015), was used. This single-item scale employs visual representations of overlapping circles to illustrate varying degrees of identification with the partner and affective closeness. Participants are asked to select the pair of circles that best represents their relationship with their romantic partner.
Felt loved. The Felt-Loved scale was used to assess how loved participants felt about their partners (Sasaki et al., 2023). Participants rated the following three items on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 7 = very much): “I feel cared for/loved by my partner”; “I feel accepted/valued by my partner”; and “I feel understood/validated by my partner.” The original study averaged the items to create an overall measure of felt love (α = 0.94; Sasaki et al., 2023). Cronbach’s alpha for the current sample was 0.84.
Attachment. The Revised Adult Attachment Scale (RAAS), Close Relationship version (Collins, 1996), was used to assess adult attachment styles. This 18-item scale measures emotional closeness or distance in relationships using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all characteristic of me; 5 = very characteristic of me). It evaluates three dimensions of attachment: closeness, dependence, and attachment-related anxiety and avoidance. Example items of each subscale are “I find it relatively easy to get close to people” (close); “I find it difficult to allow myself to depend on others” (depend); and “I often worry that other people don’t really love me” (attachment-related anxiety and avoidance)
The validated Spanish version of the scale was used (Fernández & Dufey, 2015). Only the dimensions of anxiety (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.90) and avoidance (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81) are reported in the current study.

2.3. Procedure

Data collection occurred between April and July 2024 using an online platform, where participants completed the questionnaires mentioned above. Following APA ethical standards, informed consent was obtained from all participants before participation. The study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the first author’s university, ensuring adherence to confidentiality and ethical guidelines throughout the research process.

2.4. Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated to summarize the characteristics of the sample. Mean differences across countries were examined using independent t-tests, and effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d.
Pearson correlations were performed to explore associations among the affective variables. Additionally, stepwise multiple regression analyses were conducted to identify the best predictors of jealousy. Separate regressions were conducted for the overall sample and by country (Chile and Spain) to examine potential cultural differences in the predictors of jealousy. All analyses were performed using SPSS 25 and JAMOVI 2.4.5. with significance levels set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

All participants in the sample were in a committed relationship when they completed the scales. Additionally, they reported having had on average 2.21 romantic relationships (SD = 1.24), having an average of 0.56 children (SD = 0.99).
The normality of the data was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. The results indicated that all variables, including jealousy (W = 0.165, p = 0.000), IOS (W = 0.165, p = 0.000), intimacy (W = 0.193, p = 0.000), passion (W = 0.207, p = 0.000), commitment (W = 0.113, p = 0.000), felt loved (W = 208, p = 0.000), and anxious attachment (W = 0.125, p = 0.000), did not meet the normality criterion, while avoidant attachment (W = 0.053, p = 0.079) was normally distributed.
Descriptive statistics by country are presented in Table 1, with Chilean participants reporting significantly more jealousy than the Spanish (t = 3.653, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.47).
When inquiring about the only demographic variable that was different across countries, the correlation between time in the relationship and the affective variables yielded that, in the Chileans, this was directly correlated to age (r = 0.61, p < 0.001), and inversely associated with jealousy (r = −0.31, p < 0.01) and anxious (r = −0.38, p < 0.01) and avoidant attachment (r = −0.23, p < 0.01). In the Spanish participants, only age, again, was positively correlated with time in the relationship (r = 0.41, p < 0.001).
Correlational analyses in the whole sample among the affective variables showed that jealousy was significantly and directly associated with IOS and anxious and avoidant attachment and inversely correlated with the dimensions of intimacy and commitment from the Love Scale, as well as the Felt-Loved scale (See Table 2).
A stepwise regression predicting jealousy from the affective variables yielded anxious attachment, IOS, and felt loved as the best predictors of jealousy (R2 = 0.28, F3, 254 = 33.264. p < 001). Jealousy was positively predicted by anxious attachment (β = 0.325. t = 5.772, p < 0.001) and IOS (β = 0.279, t = 5.082, p < 0.001) and negatively predicted by felt loved (β = −0.176, t = −2.910, p < 0.001).
As can be seen in Figure 1, two additional stepwise regressions were run predicting jealousy by country. In Chile, 41.9% of the variance in jealousy (F3, 89 = 20.650, p < 001) was positively explained by IOS (β = 0.395. t = 4.433, p < 0.001) and anxious attachment (β = 0.218, t = 2.364, p < 0.05) and negatively predicted by felt loved (β = −0.251, t = −2.827, p < 0.05). In Spain, 21.9% of the variance in jealousy (F2, 165 = 23.157, p < 0.001) was predicted by anxious attachment (β = 0.400, t = 5.749, p < 0.001) and IOS (β = 0.188, t = 2.705, p < 0.01).

4. Discussion

This study posits that love and jealousy are emotions that help protect a significant romantic relationship, rather than being reverse affective variables, as might commonly be expected. Therefore, jealousy was predicted to be associated with relationship closeness and insecure attachment, a hypothesis that was supported by our results from two Spanish-speaking samples, one from South America and the other from Europe.
The results reveal distinct patterns in how demographic and affective variables interact across the two countries. For Chilean participants, relationship duration correlates significantly with a broader range of variables, including age, jealousy, and attachment styles. This suggests a more complex interplay between relationship duration and emotional dynamics in Chile. Longer relationships may foster lower attachment insecurity and jealousy, which aligns with the classical literature that links jealousy to insecure attachment (Rodriguez et al., 2015). Additionally, shorter relationship duration may signal lower commitment. In contrast, among Spanish participants, relationship duration was found to correlate exclusively with age. This possibly reflects a distinct cultural approach to relationship dynamics, where the passage of time exerts less influence on affective experiences. Furthermore, the absence of a significant association between relationship duration and jealousy in the Spanish sample may be explained by a more individualistic orientation in the Spanish sample than in the Chilean group. It could also be linked to a higher prevalence of avoidant attachment styles in more individualistic societies (Sakman & Sümer, 2024). Supporting this interpretation, Deng et al. (2023) reported that individuals with avoidant tendencies tend to experience less pervasive jealousy in Russia.
Jealousy is consistently associated with anxious attachment and the perception of the inclusion of other in the self (IOS) across both samples, highlighting the universal role of attachment insecurity and relational closeness in this affection. However, the perception of being loved (felt loved) emerged as a protective factor against jealousy, particularly in the Chilean sample. This outcome suggests that feelings of relational security may mitigate jealousy more strongly in Chile, reflecting cultural nuances in emotional regulation or relational expectations (Collins & Feeney, 2000), which could be related to the shorter relationship duration of this group compared to the Spanish. For example, Sasaki et al. (2023) found that feeling loved fosters emotional security and mutual appreciation within a couple, serving as a protective factor for relationship stability. This variable may act as a buffer against negative emotions, reducing their potential to disrupt relational harmony. By reinforcing positive affect and strengthening emotional bonds, feeling loved helps couples navigate challenges more effectively, reducing conflict escalation, enhancing resilience, and mitigating romantic jealousy. Alternatively, the shorter relationship duration observed in the Chilean sample could be associated with an enhanced sensibility to jealousy, as limited romantic stability is known to increase jealousy (Chursina, 2023).
The variance in jealousy explained by the regression models differs notably between countries, with the Chilean data explaining nearly twice the variance observed in Spain. This disparity underscores the potentially greater emotional complexity or cultural significance of jealousy in Chilean relationships. The stronger role of feeling loved among Chilean participants highlights the protective influence of emotional validation and perceived affection, which seems less critical in the Spanish sample (see Watkins et al., 2019).
The findings suggest that jealousy, rather than being merely an indicator of insecurity or distrust, may reflect the emotional intensity characteristic of close relationships. In the Chilean sample, the perception of feeling loved served as a significant protective factor, stressing the role of relational security in mitigating jealousy. Greater emotional closeness appears to moderate jealousy, as reciprocal connections within relationships enable individuals to manage these emotions more effectively (Attridge, 2013; Sasaki et al., 2023).
The difference in the impact of feeling loved between Chile and Spain may stem from the varying emphasis on emotional connection within these cultural contexts. Individualistic societies often prioritize autonomy and passionate connections, while collectivist cultures place greater value on family ties and social harmony (Nadal, 2012). These cultural differences extend to responses to jealousy, particularly in situations involving sexual infidelity, where factors such as commitment expectations and the influence of social media play significant roles (Zandbergen & Brown, 2015). In Chile, shorter average relationship durations may reflect distinct relational expectations shaped by cultural or generational factors. For instance, younger generations or individuals from collectivist cultures may approach relationship maintenance behaviors with heightened sensitivity to emotional security and perceived trustworthiness. Previous research suggests that relational expectations evolve with cultural norms and age groups, influencing thresholds for acceptable behavior on social media (Tandon et al., 2021). These evolving norms could explain why shorter relationships in Chile exhibit greater sensitivity to feeling loved, compared to the longer and potentially more stable relationships observed in Spain.
The findings point to the importance of considering cultural context when examining jealousy and its predictors. In Chile, jealousy seems deeply intertwined with affective security and emotional closeness, whereas, in Spain, attachment anxiety and perceived relational closeness (IOS) play dominant roles, with a weaker association with feeling loved. These distinctions may reflect cultural differences in attachment processes, relational priorities, or societal norms surrounding romantic relationships. For example, Sakman and Sümer (2024) found that there is an association between attachment anxiety and interdependence—the more individuals view themselves in relation to others, the more attachment anxiety they experience—which may underlie greater feelings of jealousy in the Chilean context since there is higher relational closeness and possibly interdependence.
While this study illuminates key predictors of jealousy, the cross-sectional nature, small sample size, and convenience sampling limit causal interpretations. Additionally, jealousy was measured using a digital jealousy scale, which may not fully capture the broader context of jealousy in romantic relationships. Another limitation of this study was that the distribution of sexes within the sample was not balanced, which could influence the generalizability of the findings. Furthermore, as most participants were in heterosexual relationships, we were unable to analyze potential differences in affective variables based on relationship type, like in polyamorous or fluid relationships.
Future research should explore longitudinal designs to track changes in affective variables over time and examine how cultural factors shape emotional regulation in relationships. Additionally, investigating other cultural contexts would further elucidate the universality or variability of these dynamics. It is also important to use more comprehensive measures of jealousy that extend beyond digital contexts, capturing a wider array of jealousy experiences. Moreover, forthcoming studies should aim for more balanced samples across sexes as well as more diverse samples that include various relationship types, allowing for meaningful comparisons across these groups. Power analyses to justify sample sizes and ensure robust and generalizable findings should also be incorporated. Finally, the non-pathological and subtle nature of normal relationship jealousy that was studied should not be confused with other kinds of destructive relationship affects and behaviors within romantic relationships (i.e., Rodriguez et al., 2015), which were not the focus of the research.

5. Conclusions

This study reaffirms the idea that love and jealousy may be evolved protective factors of romantic relationships. It stresses the complex, culturally nuanced nature of jealousy in romantic relationships. The findings emphasize the interplay of attachment, relational closeness, and feeling loved, highlighting the need to consider both individual and cultural factors in understanding emotional experiences within romantic partnerships.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.M.F., M.T.B., D.R.-O., M.G.-G. and P.B.; methodology, A.M.F. and P.B.; software, B.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, A.M.F. and M.T.B.; writing—review and editing, A.M.F., B.Z., M.T.B., P.B., M.G.-G. and D.R.-O. visualization, B.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Project “AYUDANTE Dicyt_032456FT”, Vicerrectoría de Investigación, Innovación y Creación, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, and the Fondecyt Project #1181114 of the Government of Chile.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ETHICS COMMITTEE of UNIVERSIDAD DE SANTIAGO DE CHILE (protocol code 443, 21 June 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data analyzed during this study are not publicly available due to confidentiality agreements with participants but are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Attridge, M. (2013). Jealousy and relationship closeness: Exploring the good (reactive) and bad (suspicious) sides of romantic jealousy. SAGE Open, 3(1), 2158244013476054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barbato, M. T., Fernández, A. M., Rodriguez-Sickert, C., Muñoz, J. A., Polo, P., & Buss, D. (2024). Jealousy as predicted by allocation and reception of resources in an economic game. Evolutionary Psychology, 22(4), 14747049241289232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Bode, A. (2023). Romantic love evolved by co-opting mother-infant bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1176067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss. (Attachment (2nd ed.), Vol. 1). Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bringle, R. G., Roach, S., Andler, C., & Evenbeck, S. E. (1979). Measuring the intensity of jealous reactions. Journal of Supplemental Abstract Service, 9, 23–24. [Google Scholar]
  8. Buss, D. M. (2013). Sexual jealousy. Psihologijske Teme, 22(2), 155–182. [Google Scholar]
  9. Buss, D. M. (2018). Sexual and emotional infidelity: Evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Buss, D. M., & Abrams, M. (2017). Jealousy, infidelity, and the difficulty of diagnosing pathology: A CBT approach to coping with sexual betrayal and the green-eyed monster. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 35, 150–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychological Science, 3(4), 251–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Buunk, B. P. (1997). Personality, birth order and attachment styles as related to various types of jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(6), 997–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Buunk, A. P., & Fernandez, A. M. (2020). Don’t cheat like I did: Possessive jealousy and infidelity in close relationships. Interpersona, 14(2), 211–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Buunk, A. P., Massar, K., Dijkstra, P., & Fernández, A. M. (2019). Intersexual and intrasexual competition and their relation to jealousy. In The Oxford handbook on evolutionary psychology and behavioral endocrinology (pp. 125–236). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Carter, C. S., & Porges, S. W. (2013). The biochemistry of love: An oxytocin hypothesis. EMBO Reports, 14(1), 12–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Cassepp-Borges, V., Gonzales, J. E., Frazier, A., & Ferrer, E. (2023). Love and relationship satisfaction as a function of romantic relationship stages. Trends in Psychology, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chin, K., Atkinson, B. E., Raheb, H., Harris, E., & Vernon, P. A. (2017). The dark side of romantic jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chung, M., & Harris, C. R. (2018). Jealousy as a specific emotion: The dynamic functional model. Emotion Review, 10(4), 272–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chursina, A. V. (2023). The impact of romantic attachment styles on jealousy in young adults. Psychology in Russia, 16(3), 222–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 810–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theory perspective on support seeking and caregiving in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(6), 1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Conroy-Beam, D., Goetz, C. D., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Why do humans form long-term mateships? An evolutionary game-theoretic model. In J. Olson, & M. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–39). Academic Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 201–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Costa, A. L., Sophia, E. C., Sanches, C., Tavares, H., & Zilberman, M. L. (2015). Pathological jealousy: Romantic relationship characteristics, emotional and personality aspects, and social adjustment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. De Munck, V. C., Korotayev, A., de Munck, J., & Khaltourina, D. (2011). Cross-cultural analysis of models of romantic love among US residents, Russians, and Lithuanians. Cross-Cultural Research, 45(2), 128–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Deng, M., Tadesse, E., Khalid, S., Zhang, W., Song, J., & Gao, C. (2023). The influence of insecure attachment on undergraduates’ jealousy: The mediating effect of self-differentiation. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1153866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dion, K. K., & Dion, K. L. (2006). Individualism, collectivism, and the psychology of love. In R. J. Sternberg, & K. Weis (Eds.), The new psychology of love (pp. 298–312). Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Eastwick, P. W. (2016). The emerging integration of close relationships research and evolutionary psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ein-Dor, T. (2015). Attachment dispositions and human defensive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 112–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ellis, O., Heshmati, S., & Oravecz, Z. (2022). What makes early adults feel loved? Cultural consensus of felt love experiences in early adulthood. Applied Developmental Science, 28(2), 166–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Fernandez, A. M. (2017). Sexual jealousy among women. In T. Shackelford, & V. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science (pp. 1–8). Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fernández, A. M., Acevedo, Y., Baeza, C. G., Dufey, M., & Puga, I. (2022). Jealousy protest to a social rival compared to a nonsocial rival in Chilean infants 10–20 months’ old. Infancy, 27(5), 997–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Fernández, A. M., Barbato, M. T., Cordero, B., & Acevedo, Y. (2023). What’s love got to do with jealousy? Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1249556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Fernández, A. M., & Dufey, M. (2015). Adaptação da escala dimensional revisada de apego adulto de Collins para o contexto chileno. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28, 242–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fletcher, G. J. (2015). Accuracy and bias of judgments in romantic relationships. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 292–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 1–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Gächter, S., Starmer, C., & Tufano, F. (2015). Measuring the closeness of relationships: A comprehensive evaluation of the ‘inclusion of the other in the self’ scale. PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0129478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Gonzaga, G. C., Turner, R. A., Keltner, D., Campos, B., & Altemus, M. (2006). Romantic love and sexual desire in close relationships. Emotion, 6(2), 163–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Gubler, D. A., Schlegel, K., Richter, M., Kapanci, T., & Troche, S. J. (2023). The green-eyed monster in social media—Development and validation of a digital jealousy scale. Psychological Test Adaptation and Development, 4(1), 13–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Güçlü, O., Şenormancı, Ö., Şenormancı, G., & Köktürk, F. (2017). Gender differences in romantic jealousy and attachment styles. Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 27(4), 359–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Harris, C. R. (2003). Factors associated with jealousy over real and imagined infidelity: An examination of the social-cognitive and evolutionary psychology perspectives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27(4), 319–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hart, S. L. (2010). The ontogenesis of jealousy in the first year of life: A theory of jealousy as a biologically-based dimension of temperament. In S. L. Hart, & M. Legerstee (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy: Theory, research, and multidisciplinary approaches (pp. 57–82). Wiley Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 9(4), 383–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Heshmati, S., Oravecz, Z., Pressman, S., Batchelder, W. H., Muth, C., & Vandekerckhove, J. (2017). What does it mean to feel loved: Cultural consensus and individual differences in felt love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36(1), 214–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Campbell, R., Kurokawa, Y., & Saito, M. (1996). Separateness and togetherness: Interdependence over the life course in Japanese and American marriages. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13(3), 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Insights, H. (2020). National culture. Available online: https://www.hofstede-insights.com/ (accessed on 22 December 2024).
  48. Karandashev, V. (2015). A Cultural Perspective on Romantic Love. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 5(4), 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kowal, M., Sorokowski, P., Dinić, B. M., Pisanski, K., Gjoneska, B., Frederick, D. A., Pfuhl, G., Milfont, T. L., Bode, A., Aguilar, L., García, F. E., Roberts, S. C., Abad-Villaverde, B., Kavčič, T., Miroshnik, K. G., Ndukaihe, I. L. G., Šafárová, K., Valentova, J. V., Aavik, T., . . . Sternberg, R. J. (2024). Validation of the short version (TLS-15) of the Triangular Love Scale (TLS-45) across 37 languages. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 53(2), 839–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kruger, D. J., Fisher, M. L., Edelstein, R. S., Chopik, W. J., Fitzgerald, C. J., & Strout, S. L. (2013). Was that cheating? Perceptions vary by sex, attachment anxiety, and behavior. Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 159–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Kuhle, B. (2011). Did you have sex with him? Do you love her? An in vivo test of sex differences in jealous interrogations. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(8), 1044–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lafontaine, M. F., Guzmán-González, M., Péloquin, K., & Levesque, C. (2018). I am not in your shoes: Low perspective taking mediating the relation among attachment insecurities and physical intimate partner violence in Chilean university students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(22), 3439–3458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Larsen, P. H. H., Bendixen, M., Grøntvedt, T. V., Kessler, A. M., & Kennair, L. E. O. (2021). Investigating the emergence of sex differences in jealousy responses in a large community sample from an evolutionary perspective. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 6485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Massar, K., & Buunk, A. P. (2010). Judging a book by its cover: Jealousy after subliminal priming with attractive and unattractive faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 634–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Massar, K., Winters, C. L., Lenz, S., & Jonason, P. K. (2017). Green-eyed snakes: The associations between psychopathy, jealousy, and jealousy induction. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 164–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2017). Adult Attachment and compassion. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), Oxford handbooks online (pp. 79–90). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Montalvini, P. R., Lucero, M., & López, G. B. (2014). Estilos de apego y su relación con el patrón alimenticio de restricción-sobrealimentación en sujetos dietantes crónicos. Revista Chilena de Neuropsicología, 9(1–2), 8–11. [Google Scholar]
  58. Nadal, K. L. (2012). Mahal: Expressing love in Filipino and Filipino American families. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of love. Volume 3: Meaning and culture (pp. 23–26). Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ng, S. M., & Hou, W. K. (2017). Contentment duration mediates the associations between anxious attachment style and psychological distress. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Reis, H. T., & Aron, A. (2008). Love: What is it, why does it matter, and how does it operate? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Rodriguez, L. M., DiBello, A. M., Øverup, C. S., & Neighbors, C. (2015). The price of distrust: Trust, anxious attachment, jealousy, and partner abuse. Partner Abuse, 6(3), 298–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Rykkje, L., Eriksson, K., & Råholm, M.-B. (2015). Love in connectedness: A theoretical study. SAGE Open, 5(1), 2158244015571186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sabini, J., & Green, M. C. (2004). Emotional responses to sexual and emotional infidelity: Constants and differences across genders, samples, and methods. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1375–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Sakman, E., & Sümer, N. (2024). Cultural correlates of adult attachment dimensions: Comparing the US and Turkey. Cross-Cultural Research, 58(2–3), 208–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Sasaki, E., Overall, N. C., Reis, H. T., Righetti, F., Chang, V. T., Low, R. S. T., Henderson, A. M. E., McRae, C. S., Cross, E. J., Jayamaha, S. D., Maniaci, M. R., & Reid, C. J. (2023). Feeling loved as a strong link in relationship interactions: Partners who feel loved may buffer destructive behavior by actors who feel unloved. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 125(2), 367–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Scelza, B. A. (2014). Jealousy in a small-scale, natural fertility population: The roles of paternity, investment and love in jealous response. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 103–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Schmitt, D. P., Youn, G., Bond, B., Brooks, S., Frye, H., Johnson, S., Klesman, J., Peplinski, C., Sampias, J., Sherrill, M., & Stoka, C. (2009). When will I feel love? The effects of culture, personality, and gender on the psychological tendency to love. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5), 830–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Schützwohl, A. (2008). The intentional object of romantic jealousy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(2), 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Seeman, M. V. (2016). Pathological jealousy: An interactive condition. Psychiatry, 79(4), 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29(3), 240–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., Butovskaya, M., Karwowski, M., Groyecka, A., Wojciszke, B., & Pawłowski, B. (2017). Love influences reproductive success in humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Sprecher, S., Sullivan, Q., & Hatfield, E. (1994). Mate selection preferences: Gender differences examined in a national sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 1074–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93(2), 119–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Stoet, G. (2010). PsyToolkit: A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 1096–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Sullivan, K. T. (2021). Attachment style and jealousy in the digital age: Do attitudes about online communication matter? Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 678542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Tandon, A., Dhir, A., & Mäntymäki, M. (2021). Jealousy due to social media? A systematic literature review and framework of social media-induced jealousy. Internet Research, 31(5), 1541–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Tukachinsky Forster, R. (2022). The green side of parasocial romantic relationships: An exploratory investigation of parasocial jealousy. Psychology of Popular Media, 12(3), 279–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Watkins, C. D., Leongómez, J. D., Bovet, J., Żelaźniewicz, A., Korbmacher, M., Varella, M. A. C., Fernandez, A. M., Wagstaff, B., & Bolgan, S. (2019). National income inequality predicts cultural variation in mouth to mouth kissing. Scientific Reports, 9, 6698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Xia, M., Chen, Y., & Dunne, S. (2023). What makes people feel loved? An exploratory study on core elements of love across family, romantic, and friend relationships. Family Process, 63(3), 1304–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Zandbergen, D. L., & Brown, S. G. (2015). Culture and gender differences in romantic jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 122–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Predictors of jealousy by country. Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Predictors of jealousy by country. Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Behavsci 15 00132 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and effect sizes for the variables by country.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and effect sizes for the variables by country.
ChileSpaindp
MSDMSD
Jealousy2.481.271.980.910.470.001
Intimacy22.273.0321.802.840.160.208
Passion19.874.1719.003.900.220.090
Commitment22.033.7221.942.940.030.828
IOS3.744.003.734.000.010.954
Felt Loved6.241.046.100.950.150.253
Anxious Attachment2.371.072.370.830.000.997
Avoidant Attachment2.340.642.420.580.120.340
Note: n = 265. Values of 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 for Cohen’s d (d) are commonly considered to be indicative of small, medium, and large effects, respectively.
Table 2. Correlation between jealousy and the affective variables.
Table 2. Correlation between jealousy and the affective variables.
Variables2345678
1. Jealousy−0.127 *0.076−0.173 *0.364 ***−0.240 ***0.430 ***0.178 **
2. Intimacy 0.667 ***0.708 ***0.0130.815 ***−0.174 **−0.175 **
3. Passion 0.587 ***0.0950.577 ***0.019−0.042
4. Commitment 0.0360.677 ***−0.194 **−0.122
5. IOS −0.054−0.236 **−0.169 **
6. Felt loved 0.243 **0.071
7. Anxious Att. 0.445 ***
8. Avoidant Att. -----
Note. The table presents Pearson correlation coefficients (r) between jealousy and the affective variables: love (including the dimensions intimacy, passion, and commitment), closeness (assessed using IOS), felt loved, and attachment (anxious attachment and avoidant attachment). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fernández, A.M.; Barbato, M.T.; Barone, P.; Zavalla, B.; Rivera-Ottenberger, D.; Guzmán-González, M. What Is the Link of Closeness and Jealousy in Romantic Relationships? Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020132

AMA Style

Fernández AM, Barbato MT, Barone P, Zavalla B, Rivera-Ottenberger D, Guzmán-González M. What Is the Link of Closeness and Jealousy in Romantic Relationships? Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(2):132. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020132

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fernández, Ana María, Maria Teresa Barbato, Pamela Barone, Belén Zavalla, Diana Rivera-Ottenberger, and Mónica Guzmán-González. 2025. "What Is the Link of Closeness and Jealousy in Romantic Relationships?" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 2: 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020132

APA Style

Fernández, A. M., Barbato, M. T., Barone, P., Zavalla, B., Rivera-Ottenberger, D., & Guzmán-González, M. (2025). What Is the Link of Closeness and Jealousy in Romantic Relationships? Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020132

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop