Bystanders to Bias: Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions Affects Men’s and Women’s Outcomes in STEM Small Group Contexts
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Gendered Microaggressions as Subtle Expressions of Gender Stereotypes
1.2. Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions
1.3. Activating Social Identity Threat
1.4. Gender Differences in Effects of Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions
1.5. Stereotyping Concerns as a Mechanism
1.6. Overview of the Studies and Hypotheses
2. Study 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Procedure and Materials
Clip Number | Microaggression Version | Control Version | |
---|---|---|---|
Stereotypic Interaction | Stereotype Reflected | Non-Stereotypic Interaction | |
1 | A man asks a woman to take the secretarial role of note-taker. | Women primarily support men’s work in STEM and adopt stereotypic roles such as secretary. | A man volunteers to take notes. |
2 | Students discuss being in research studies. | None; neutral interaction | Students discuss being in research studies. |
3 | A woman’s idea is ignored until a man repeats it and is given credit for it. | Men are more credible sources of good ideas in STEM than women. | A woman’s idea is discussed and accepted. |
4 | Students discuss their summer vacations. | None; neutral interaction | Students discuss their summer vacations. |
5 | A woman volunteers ideas but the men speak over her. | Women’s STEM contributions are not as important as men’s. | A woman volunteers ideas without being spoken over. |
6 | A man expresses surprise that a woman is in a more advanced calculus class than he is in. | It is unusual and unexpected for women to be highly competent in STEM. | A woman states she is in an advanced calculus class without comment from others. |
7 | Students discuss their internet research. | None; neutral interaction | Students discuss their internet research. |
8 | A man explains a concept to a woman after she states that she is already familiar with the concept. | Women are assumed to be less knowledgeable than men. | A man and a woman discuss a concept that they are both familiar with. |
9 | A woman reads the project instructions and requirements to the group. | None; neutral interaction | A woman reads the project instructions and requirements to the group. |
2.2. Measures
2.3. Results
2.3.1. Two-Way ANOVAs
2.3.2. Multigroup Path Analysis
2.4. Study 1 Discussion
3. Studies 2a and 2b
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Procedure
3.1.3. Measures
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Two-Way ANOVAs
3.2.2. Mixed ANOVA—Gender Stereotyping Concerns
3.2.3. Multigroup Path Analysis
3.3. Study 2a and Study 2b Discussion
4. General Discussion
4.1. Limitations and Future Directions
4.2. Contributions to Theory
4.3. Contributions to Practice
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | In each study, we over-recruited women to get a sizable sample of female computer science and engineering majors. In the online Supplementary Materials, Section S3, we report the number of men and women in the microaggression and control conditions for each study. |
2 | If two participants were scheduled for a lab session at the same time, they were told they would be joining separate groups, and one of them would go to another room. |
3 | Additionally, in each study, we included a manipulation check at the end to assess the participants’ perceptions of the videos as displaying respectful or biased interactions (see online Supplementary Materials, Section S7). In each study, the participants rated the neutral videos as more respectful and less gender-biased than the microaggression videos. |
4 | In the main text, we present results that were measured in each study and that we believe are important group work outcomes in the computer science and engineering context (engineering recall, stereotyping concerns, and enthusiasm for group work). Additionally, each study included exploratory control variables and moderators that did not significantly impact our interpretation of the results. Study 1 included exploratory outcomes that were not included in follow-up studies, and Study 2b was part of an omnibus data collection (of both male-dominated and gender-balanced STEM majors) and, thus, it included measures not of relevance to the current study. See the online Supplementary Materials, Section S10, for the full list of measures assessed in each study. |
5 | In Studies 2a and 2b, we again assess general gender stereotyping concerns. Results with the general gender stereotyping concern measure can be found in the online Supplementary Materials, Section S8. |
6 | Study 2b was part of a larger omnibus study and data collection effort involving both male-dominated and gender-balanced STEM majors (N = 568). In the current study, we only report on computer science and engineering college students (who identified as such in our internal survey). |
7 | In Studies 2a and 2b, we again measured general stereotyping concerns. Contrary to Study 1, both studies revealed a significant condition-by-gender interaction, with the effect of the condition on general stereotyping concerns being stronger for women. Moreover, both studies revealed a significant indirect effect of witnessing microaggressions on decreased enthusiasm through general stereotyping concerns for women, but not men (see online Supplementary Materials, Section S8 for full reporting of results). This further indicates that in small group work STEM contexts, stereotyping concerns may be of more relevance and more consequential to women than men. |
References
- Amodeo, A. L., Esposito, C., & Bacchini, D. (2020). Heterosexist microaggressions, student academic experience and perception of campus climate: Findings from an Italian higher education context. PLoS ONE, 15(4), e0231580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected: Divergent goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 248–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloodhart, B., Balgopal, M. M., Casper, A. M. A., Sample McMeeking, L. B., & Fischer, E. V. (2020). Outperforming yet undervalued: Undergraduate women in STEM. PLoS ONE, 15(6), e0234685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruin, J. (2006). STATA programs for data analysis (ANOVA tools). UCLA, Statistical Consulting Group. Available online: https://stats.oarc.ucla.edu/stata/ado/analysis/ (accessed on 9 February 2025).
- Champely, S. (2020). pwr: Basic functions for power analysis: R package version 1.3-0. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pwr (accessed on 9 February 2025).
- Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient belonging: How stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer science. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clifford, S., & Jerit, J. (2014). Is there a cost to convenience? An experimental comparison of data quality in laboratory and online studies. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 1(2), 120–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Cortland, C. I., & Kinias, Z. (2019). Stereotype threat and women’s work satisfaction: The importance of role models. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 7(1), 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dasgupta, N., Scircle, M. M., & Hunsinger, M. (2015). Female peers in small work groups enhance women’s motivation, verbal participation, and career aspirations in engineering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(16), 4988–4993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emerson, K. T., & Murphy, M. C. (2015). A company I can trust? Organizational lay theories moderate stereotype threat for women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(2), 295–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Finley, A. J., & Penningroth, S. L. (2015). Online versus in-lab: Pros and cons of an online prospective memory experiment. In A. M. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 113, pp. 135–162). Nova Science Publishers, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Frantz, C. M., Cuddy, A. J., Burnett, M., Ray, H., & Hart, A. (2004). A threat in the computer: The race implicit association test as a stereotype threat experience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 1611–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeman, T. M., Anderman, L. H., & Jensen, J. M. (2007). Sense of belonging in college freshmen at the classroom and campus levels. Journal of Experimental Education, 75(3), 203–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidet, P., Kubitz, K., & McCormack, W. T. (2014). Analysis of the team-based learning literature: TBL comes of age. Journal on Excellence in College Teaching, 25(3–4), 303–333. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [PubMed Central]
- Hutchison, K. (2020). Four types of gender bias affecting women surgeons and their cumulative impact. Journal of Medical Ethics, 46(4), 236–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kassambara, A. (2018). Comparing multiple means in R. Datanovia. Available online: https://www.datanovia.com/en/courses/comparing-multiple-means-in-r/ (accessed on 9 February 2025).
- Kim, J. Y., & Meister, A. (2023). Microaggressions, interrupted: The experience and effects of gender microaggressions for women in STEM. Journal of Business Ethics, 185(3), 513–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lilienfeld, S. O. (2017). Microaggressions: Strong claims, inadequate evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(1), 138–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logel, C., Walton, G. M., Spencer, S. J., Iserman, E. C., von Hippel, W., & Bell, A. E. (2009). Interacting with sexist men triggers social identity threat among female engineers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Long, J. S. (Ed.). (2001). From scarcity to visibility: Gender differences in the careers of doctoral scientists and engineers. National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
- Love, B., Hodge, A., Grandgenett, N., & Swift, A. W. (2014). Student learning and perceptions in a flipped linear algebra course. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 45(3), 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahalik, J. R., Good, G. E., Tager, D., Levant, R. F., & Mackowiak, C. (2012). Developing a taxonomy of helpful and harmful practices for clinical work with boys and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59(4), 591–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meadows, L. A., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2013, June 23–26). The influence of gender stereotypes on role adoption in student teams [Paper # 6744]. American Society for Engineering Education Annual Conference, Atlanta, GA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T. A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, D. L. (2024). Understanding subtle gender bias recognition among witnesses in a STEM undergraduate context [Open Access Dissertation, Michigan Technological University]. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18(10), 879–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nadal, K. L., Hamit, S., Lyons, O., Weinberg, A., & Corman, L. (2013). Gender microaggressions: Perceptions, processes, and coping mechanisms of women. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of business success (pp. 193–220). Praeger. [Google Scholar]
- National Academy of Sciences, National Academy of Engineering & Institute of Medicine. (2007). Beyond bias and barriers: Fulfilling the potential of women in academic science and engineering. National Academies Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- National Science Foundation. (1994). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 1994 (NSF 94-333). National Science Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- National Science Foundation. (2021). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2021 (NSF 21-321). National Science Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002a). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002b). Math= male, me= female, therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Onyeador, I. N., Hudson, S. K. T., & Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2021). Moving beyond implicit bias training: Policy insights for increasing organizational diversity. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8(1), 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peñalver, J., Salanova, M., Martínez, I. M., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2019). Happy-productive groups: How positive affect links to performance through social resources. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 14(3), 377–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietri, E. S., Hennes, E. P., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Bailey, A. H., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Handelsman, J. (2019). Addressing unintended consequences of gender diversity interventions on women’s sense of belonging in STEM. Sex Roles, 80, 527–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramsey, L. R., Betz, D. E., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2013). The effects of an academic environment intervention on science identification among women in STEM. Social Psychology of Education, 16(3), 377–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 316–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sekaquaptewa, D. (2019). Gender-based microaggressions in STEM settings. NCID Currents, 1(1). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2003). Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(1), 68–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Severiens, S. E., & Schmidt, H. G. (2009). Academic and social integration and study progress in problem based learning. Higher Education, 58, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shelton, J. N., West, T. V., & Trail, T. E. (2010). Concerns about appearing prejudiced: Implications for anxiety during daily interracial interactions. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(3), 329–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, K. A. (1996). Cooperative learning: Making” groupwork” work. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 67, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi: How stereotypes affect us and what we can do. WW Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
- Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sue, D. W. (2010). Microaggressions in everyday life: Race, gender, and sexual orientation. John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Sue, D. W. (2019). Microaggressions and student activism: Harmless impact and victimhood controversies. In Microaggression theory: Influence and implications. Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, V. J., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Stereotype threat undermines academic learning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 1055–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valian, V. (1998). Why so slow? MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vorauer, J. D., Main, K. J., & O’Connell, G. B. (1998). How do individuals expect to be viewed by members of lower status groups? Content and implications of meta-stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 917–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vorauer, J. D., & Turpie, C. A. (2004). Disruptive effects of vigilance on dominant group members’ treatment of outgroup members: Choking versus shining under pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 384–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watkins, N. L., LaBarrie, T. L., & Appio, L. M. (2010). Black undergraduates’ experiences with perceived racial microaggressions in predominantly White colleges and universities. In D. W. Sue (Ed.), Microaggressions and marginality: Manifestation, dynamics, and impact (pp. 25–58). Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, J. C., Phillips, K. W., & Hall, E. V. (2014). Double jeopardy? Gender bias against women in science. UC Hastings College of Law. Available online: https://worklifelaw.org/publications/Double-Jeopardy-Report_v6_full_web-sm.pdf (accessed on 9 February 2025).
- Wong, G., Derthick, A. O., David, E. J. R., Saw, A., & Okazaki, S. (2014). The what, the why, and the how: A review of racial microaggressions research in psychology. Race and Social Problems, 6(2), 181–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, L. N., & Lewis, J. A. (2020). Is physical activity a buffer? Gendered racial microaggressions and anxiety among African American women. Journal of Black Psychology, 46(2–3), 122–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, Y., & Carroll, D. W. (2018). Gendered microaggressions in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Leadership and Research in Education, 4, 28–45. [Google Scholar]
- Yee, L., Noble-Tolla, M., Bohrer, A., Krivkovich, A., Robinson, N., Nadeau, M. C., Robinson, K., Starikova, I., Thomas, R., Cooper, M., & Konar, E. (2018). Women in the Workplace 2018. Lean In and McKinsey & Company. Available online: https://womenintheworkplace.com/ (accessed on 9 February 2025).
Study 2a | Study 2b | |
---|---|---|
Initial Participants | 412 | 272 |
Excluded Participants | ||
Failed attention checks (e.g., Please select somewhat disagree for this statement) | 22 | 19 |
Indicated data of poor quality | 37 | – |
Did not identify as current college students | 46 | – |
Not engineering or computer science majors | 27 | – |
Did not identify as men or women | – | 10 |
Did not identify as US residents during last 5 years | – | 3 |
Final number of participants | 306 | 243 |
Country of Residence | ||
US | 230 | 243 |
UK | 72 | 0 |
Other | 4 | 0 |
Gender | ||
Men | 176 | 122 |
Women | 130 | 121 |
Age | ||
Mean age | 22.07 years (SD = 4.96) | 22.00 years (SD = 6.13) |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
White | 52% | 53% |
Asian/Asian American | 24% | 24% |
Black/African American | 8% | 6% |
Latino/Hispanic | 7% | 5% |
Middle Eastern or North African | 1% | 1% |
Native American | <1% | <1% |
Selected more than one race/ethnicity | 5% | 9% |
Other | <1% | <1% |
Study 2a | Study 2b | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
F(1, 302) | p | F(1, 239) | p | |
Condition | 63.49 | <0.001 | 28.48 | <0.001 |
Gender | 27.63 | <0.001 | 16.85 | <0.001 |
Stereotyping concern type | 39.05 | <0.001 | 18.40 | <0.001 |
Condition × Gender | 2.02 | 0.157 | 11.08 | 0.001 |
Gender × Stereotyping Concern | 243.38 | <0.001 | 244.23 | <0.001 |
Condition × Stereotyping Concern | 7.10 | 0.006 | 7.39 | 0.007 |
Condition × Gender × Stereotyping Concern | 30.73 | <0.001 | 29.24 | <0.001 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Vossoughi, N.; Burley, L.C.; Foley, R.P.; Meadows, L.A.; Sekaquaptewa, D. Bystanders to Bias: Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions Affects Men’s and Women’s Outcomes in STEM Small Group Contexts. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 215. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020215
Vossoughi N, Burley LC, Foley RP, Meadows LA, Sekaquaptewa D. Bystanders to Bias: Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions Affects Men’s and Women’s Outcomes in STEM Small Group Contexts. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(2):215. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020215
Chicago/Turabian StyleVossoughi, Nadia, Logan C. Burley, Ryan P. Foley, Lorelle A. Meadows, and Denise Sekaquaptewa. 2025. "Bystanders to Bias: Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions Affects Men’s and Women’s Outcomes in STEM Small Group Contexts" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 2: 215. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020215
APA StyleVossoughi, N., Burley, L. C., Foley, R. P., Meadows, L. A., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2025). Bystanders to Bias: Witnessing Gendered Microaggressions Affects Men’s and Women’s Outcomes in STEM Small Group Contexts. Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 215. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020215