Next Article in Journal
Application of the Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emotional Disorders in a Group Format for Adults: A Scoping Review Protocol
Next Article in Special Issue
Thinking Outside the Nation: Cognitive Flexibility’s Role in National Identity Inclusiveness as a Marker of Majority Group Acculturation
Previous Article in Journal
Spendception: The Psychological Impact of Digital Payments on Consumer Purchase Behavior and Impulse Buying
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Quality of Life Among Latino/a Adults: Examining the Serial Mediation of Network Acculturation, Psychological Acculturation, Social Capital, and Helping-Seeking

by
Adrian J. Archuleta
*,
Stephanie Grace Prost
and
Mona A. Dajani
Kent School of Social Work and Family Science, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(3), 388; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15030388
Submission received: 29 October 2024 / Revised: 4 March 2025 / Accepted: 16 March 2025 / Published: 19 March 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Social and Psychological Determinants of Acculturation)

Abstract

:
Latinos/as are the largest ethnic group in the U.S. and are a continuous source of population growth. Therefore, their health and quality of life are important public health concerns. Acculturation is an important determinant of health for Latinos/as. However, few studies examine models identifying determinants of acculturation along with its relationship to other social and health behaviors. The current study uses social network data from a sample of crowdsourced recruited Latinos/as (N = 300) to examine a structural model between network acculturation, psychological acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and quality of life (QoL). The model posits several paths through which social networks (i.e., network acculturation) relate to acculturation and other model variables. Directly, network acculturation was found to be significantly related to Latino/a enculturation (−0.83, p = 0.002) and White American Acculturation (0.47, p = 0.003). Latino/a enculturation was related to help-seeking (0.21, p = 0.029) and social capital (0.36, p < 0.001), while White American acculturation was only related to social capital (0.35, p = 0.003). Social capital demonstrated a robust relationship with help-seeking (0.48, p = 0.004) and QoL (0.96, p = 0.003). The findings suggest that determinants of acculturation (i.e., network acculturation) are meaningful contributors to psychological acculturation and other variables relating to Latino/as’ QoL.

1. Introduction

Latinos/as represent 19% of the U.S. population and experience tremendous longevity, with life expectancy well into their 80s (U.S. Census Bureau, 2024). Despite this longevity, Latinos/as are likely to work in jobs with increased health risks, are less likely to receive quality medical care, and are more likely to have preexisting health conditions, experience discrimination that hinders medical casre access, and live in communities with environmental concerns (Funk & Lopez, 2022). Increases in longevity and the potential for poorer health elevate the importance of understanding Latinos/as’ quality of life (QoL), a crucial subjective measure related to one’s health beyond disease (WHO, 1998). The WHO (2012) defines QoL as “an individual’s perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and value systems in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns (p. 11)”. A person’s perception of their position in life and their associated satisfaction is driven by complex factors that vary from group to group and the context in which they live and work.
Social relationships are an important facet of a person’s quality of life, with more extensive networks providing greater access to needed resources. Social capital is critical to a person’s health and quality of life as it mitigates stressful experiences, provides known benefits that improve health, and facilitates social mobility (Arachchi & Managi, 2023; Chen et al., 2015; Moore & Kawachi, 2017). Social capital is defined as the aggregated resources obtained through one’s membership in a group (Bourdieu, 1986) and is often distinguished based on network, cognitive, and social integration descriptions, with each articulating how different facets of social capital are developed and maintained through one’s relationships (Moore & Kawachi, 2017). Social integration provides critical access to economic, political, cultural, and social resources that are associated with health determinants that drive inequities between groups (Piachaud et al., 2009).
Acculturation has been identified as a factor associated with Latinos/as’ social integration (Ferguson & Birman, 2016) and access to social capital (Rodriguez-Alcalá et al., 2019; Bhattacharya, 2011; Valencia-Garcia et al., 2012). Acculturation is defined as the cultural changes that occur when two distinct cultural groups interact for a sustained period, resulting in changes in one or both groups (Redfield et al., 1936). Acculturation has been described as degrees of change associated with strategies (e.g., assimilation, integration, separation, and marginalization) enacted by individuals and their communities that create specific acculturative experiences (Berry & Sam, 2016). Changes in acculturation can alter a person’s social networks, including family relationships (Keefe, 1984), which have meaningful implications for the type of social capital available. For example, different aspects of acculturation (e.g., social versus linguistic) and associated experiences (e.g., acculturative stress) shape network size, frequency of contact, trust, and social influence (Archuleta & Perry, 2016). Therefore, differences in social capital characteristics associated with acculturation have important implications for their health and quality of life.
Help-seeking is also important in maintaining one’s physical and mental health. An unwillingness to access help while experiencing psychological distress could exacerbate existing symptoms or result in the loss of life (e.g., unaddressed suicidal ideations). Help-seeking is influenced by various individual, social, and cultural factors (WHO, 2004); however, these factors are under-explored among Latinos/as. Acculturation and social capital (e.g., perceived trust) play critical roles in the help-seeking process, with each often determining the pathways through which care is received. For example, differences in help-seeking are often observed between first- and later-generation ethnocultural groups, with less acculturative groups preferring informal sources (e.g., friends) for assistance (Derr, 2016). Collectively, these determinants of health are important in addressing the health needs of Latino/a adults and likely enhance aspects of a person’s life that increase quality and satisfaction.

1.1. Quality of Life (QoL)

While medical interventions have increased human longevity, they have not eliminated diseases and chronic illnesses that erode the quality of one’s life (Gruenberg, 1977; Olshansky et al., 1991; Rechel et al., 2020). Increasingly, QoL is seen as an important outcome of treatment (Zubaran et al., 2012) that helps assess the effectiveness of health services and associated policies (Basch, 2014; Coulter & Ellins, 2007). A person’s QoL encompasses aspects of physical and psychological health, social relationships, environment, personal beliefs, and independence (WHO, 1998). Quality of life has been examined in relation to a broad range of health conditions, including cancer, mental illness, heart disease, kidney disease, multiple sclerosis, diabetes, fibromyalgia, eye disease, pain, and obesity (Haraldstad et al., 2019). Physical and mental health problems often restrict a person’s ability to function readily, influencing their overall satisfaction with life.
Despite the applicability of quality of life to all age groups, a substantial portion of the literature has focused on the experiences of older adults. However, the compounding effects associated with physical and mental health difficulties earlier in life warrant exploration. For example, among a crowdsourced sample in the United States, Archuleta et al. (2021) found that young and middle adults reported lower QoL scores than reported in the literature for other populations. Acculturation has demonstrated mixed results in predicting QoL among Latinos/as, with assimilation seeming to produce the best outcome (Thoman & Surís, 2004). However, acculturation likely exerts an indirect effect on QoL through other meaningful factors that dictate access to important resources (e.g., income, social integration, health, and mental health literacy), suggesting that more sophisticated models are needed to understand these relationships.

1.2. Acculturation

Cross-cultural psychological theories have been relatively dominant in describing acculturation experiences. Psychological acculturation emphasizes the internal cultural changes that result from intercultural interaction (Fung, 2015). Marín (1992) contends that acculturative changes are experienced at different depths, consisting of superficial (e.g., food, dress, and media use), intermediate (e.g., language use and preference for friends and neighbors), and significant (e.g., values, norms, and identity) levels. According to Berry (2003), individuals develop strategies to preserve a desired amount of their heritage culture while engaging in a particular level of interaction with members of the cultural majority. High and low retention levels and interaction topologically form four strategies (e.g., assimilation, integration, separation, or marginalization) that reflect a person’s social integration and cultural change (Berry & Sam, 2016). For example, with assimilation, individuals desire to engage in high levels of interaction with individuals from the cultural majority while minimizing one’s heritage (Berry, 2006). Conversely, individuals who desire to maintain their cultural heritage while minimizing interaction with the cultural majority use separation as their acculturation strategy (Berry, 2006), reflecting their initial socialization to their heritage culture (i.e., enculturation; Weinreich, 2009). Integration occurs when individuals seek a balance or high levels of cultural retention and interaction with individuals from the cultural majority (Berry, 2003). Lastly, Berry (2006) suggests that individuals who minimize the retention of their cultural heritage but are rebuffed by individuals from the cultural majority are marginalized.
For many groups, integration has been identified as the most adaptive acculturation strategy (Choy et al., 2021), but not for Latinos/as. Historically, separation has often been the most adaptative strategy because of the Latino/a diaspora (i.e., resettlement in traditional locations with dense Latino/a populations) in the U.S. and protective factors (e.g., social support) associated with Latino/a culture (Ferguson & Birman, 2016). More recently, Latinos/as have resettled in U.S. communities with previously small or absent Latino/a populations (Kochhar et al., 2005), which is associated with a greater need for adaptation and integration (Archuleta et al., 2024). Therefore, other acculturation strategies may be more viable as means of adapting than before, requiring continued investigation.
Due to its ubiquitous influence, acculturation has been identified as an essential cultural and social determinant because of its role in altering a person’s behaviors and health (Alegria, 2009). Acculturative stress is often the driving force behind physical and mental health problems, which emerge from difficulties in reconciling cultural changes or conflict occurring within relationships (Berry & Sam, 2016). Social relationships are critical in facilitating acculturation (Berry, 2003; Berry & Sam, 2016; Masgoret & Ward, 2006; Ward & Geeraert, 2016 and may be an important determinant of positive and negative acculturative experiences). However, scholars studying acculturation have called for more radical approaches that take relationships further and consider how the social forces associated with a person’s social networks contribute to changes in acculturation (Ward & Geeraert, 2016).
Incorporation into social networks is seen as a potential marker of structural assimilation, or “large-scale entrance into cliques, clubs, and institutions of host society, on a primary group level” (Gordon, 1964, p. 169). Gordon’s theory of assimilation describes the behavioral and attitudinal changes experienced but includes other potential measures that suggest broader change occurring in social life (e.g., structural assimilation and marital integration). For example, using Gordon’s definition of structural assimilation, Williams and Ortega (1990) examined a person’s neighborhood, organization membership and participation, and friendships. While Gordon’s theory has been criticized for its unidimensionality and inability to explain the experiences of non-European immigrants, modernizing (e.g., exploring social networks) several components may prove useful in explaining facets of acculturation not sufficiently explored in existing acculturation theory.
Social networks and their associated dynamics are important drivers of acculturation processes (Maupome et al., 2016). Network acculturation refers to the interconnected social relationships that form a part of a person’s social network and contribute to their acculturation experiences (Vacca et al., 2018). The maintenance of these relationships requires individual and collective balance to achieve cultural fit (Gómez et al., 2014). Network approaches to acculturation draw on social network and social convoy theories, which describe how individuals form relationships that serve as context for a person’s acculturative experiences (Archuleta et al., 2024). Language use across stressful relationships has been used to measure network acculturation, which demonstrates a negative association with psychological measures of Latinx enculturation (i.e., socialization to one’s heritage culture) and a positive association with White American acculturation, suggesting that network acculturation may serve as a potential determinant for psychological experiences (Archuleta & Dajani, 2021; Archuleta et al., 2024), highlighting the importance of social relationships in determining acculturation experiences.

1.3. Social Capital

Who people are connected to and how they are connected has been increasingly important in health research, as it determines the forms of stress a person is exposed to (e.g., life events and chronic stress) as well as the resources that are available to manage these and other life experiences (Moore & Kawachi, 2017). Social capital is described as a multidimensional construct characterized by trust, a sense of reciprocity, and levels of interconnectedness and integration that aid in mobilizing needed resources (Kawachi & Berkman, 2014; Stone & Hughes, 2002). Different types of networks (e.g., bonding, bridging, and linking) facilitate access to distinct resources (Lin, 2001). Bonding networks often reflect personal ties involving friends, family, and intimate partners, while bridging networks involve weaker ties with coworkers, neighbors, or community members (Marsden, 2011). Bonding ties are more likely to provide access to emotional support, while bridging ties may provide access to opportunities beyond one’s immediate network.
Social relationships are an important bridge between acculturation, social capital, and social networks (Allen et al., 2014; Concha et al., 2014; Keefe, 1984; Rodionov, 2021; Valencia-Garcia et al., 2012). Acculturation has been identified as a factor that changes a person’s relationships and social networks (Keefe, 1984). Valencia-Garcia et al. (2012) found that higher levels of acculturation were associated with greater social capital and reported access to needed services. Park et al. (2023) reported a more conditional relationship with acculturation moderated by social cohesion (i.e., a proxy for social capital) and negative social interaction, with higher acculturation and social cohesion associated with better mental health, and higher negative interaction and acculturation associated with worse mental health. In the context of acculturation, integration into both cultural groups may provide greater access to resources that maximize a person’s social capital. Identifying the role that acculturation plays in altering social capital is critical to understanding how acculturative experiences contribute directly and indirectly to a person’s quality of life.
Social capital is recognized as a key determinant of QoL (Nutakor et al., 2023), suggesting that a person’s social environment offers benefits across areas of life that are important to perceived quality and satisfaction (Buijs et al., 2016). Despite its salutogenic effects, social capital has demonstrated an inconsistent relationship with QoL, often relating to a person’s mental health, but not their physical health (Gao et al., 2018). One overlooked area of research involves the connection between a person’s culture and social capital. Experiences with acculturation have been observed to alter a person’s social networks, which affects access to social capital. For example, Keefe (1984) reported that the familial network size and frequency of interaction of Mexican Americans generationally decreased, suggesting that increased exposure to American culture changes the qualities of a person’s social networks. Changes in social networks are important as they may alter the pathways typically used during mental health crises to identify critical forms of help.

1.4. Help-Seeking

Help-seeking is defined as a person’s intention to pursue assistance when experiencing psychological difficulties or feelings of distress (e.g., suicidal ideations; Wilson et al., 2005). Mental health seeking stems from a person’s recognition that their psychological symptoms would be alleviated by assistance from others (Ayón et al., 2024). The behavioral model of health service utilization emphasizes that individuals are influenced by micro- (e.g., actual and perceived health needs) and macro-level factors (e.g., social determinants of health) that influence proximally and distally a person’s decision to seek help (Andersen, 1995). Individuals’ formal and informal networks are critical in the help-seeking process. Assistance with mental health may be sought through professional mental health providers or nonprofessional services such as family, friends, and online resources (Mohammadifirouzeh et al., 2023). However, help-seeking is affected by several barriers, including difficulty accessing support, lack of trust and fears about confidentiality, stigma, personal attitudes, feelings about disclosure, and a preference for informal support (Clement et al., 2015; Gulliver et al., 2010; Salaheddin & Mason, 2016).
Culture is often overlooked and underexplored in a person’s decision to seek help (McDermott et al., 2017; Teramoto & Edwards, 2014). Saint Arnault’s (2009) model examines the complex interaction between culture and facets of health decision-making, and highlights potential differences in perceptions of health, distress, and wellness that lead a person to seek help. Additionally, Saint Arnault’s model identifies the importance of a person’s social network as a driving mechanism for help-seeking, suggesting that the formulation of social capital (i.e., network size, trust, and knowledge of help-seeking pathways) occurs within a specific cultural context through a person’s social relationships. Experiences with acculturation add to this complexity by altering the potential resources for seeking assistance. For example, increases in acculturation are associated with increases in English proficiency (Wang, 2023) and greater knowledge of formal systems of assistance (Markova et al., 2020), which may facilitate help-seeking. Factors such as English language proficiency, often used to represent acculturation, are closely linked to service utilization (Derr, 2016), suggesting that greater acculturation may be associated with greater formal service use. However, Latinos/as often rely on informal networks (e.g., family, extended family, and friends) for support (Ayón & Naddy, 2013). Therefore, those individuals who are integrated into White American and Latinos/as networks may be best able to maximize their access to resources and seek assistance when needed.

1.5. The Present Study

The complex interplay between acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and QoL requires new models to better describe Latino/a’s experiences and, by extension, to identify necessary support to enhance their lives. Figure 1 provides a conceptual framework depicting the association between study variables. For example, social influences (e.g., intercultural contact and relationships) are identified as factors affecting psychological acculturative experiences (i.e., cultural change and acculturative stress).
Moreover, the framework includes outcome variables indicative of adaptation, which are consistent with QoL. The framework aligns with existing stress process models (see Berry & Sam, 2016) and cross-cultural models (see Ward & Geeraert, 2016) that describe the temporal ordering of acculturation relative to these adaptive outcomes. Network and psychological acculturation then serve as cultural determinants for social capital, help-seeking, and QoL, further shaped by other determinants that influence health and health-related outcomes. The current study seeks to extend existing research by testing a structural model of quality of life that includes social networks as a determinant of psychological acculturation along with social capital and help-seeking. While studies examining acculturation and social capital are increasing, there is a dearth of research incorporating social networks as determinants of acculturation and other social and QoL outcomes for Latino/a adults. Moreover, a limited number of studies have examined the role of social capital in facilitating help-seeking and QoL. The current study tests the following hypotheses to address existing gaps in the literature:
H1. 
Network acculturation will be negatively related to Latino/a enculturation and positively related to White American acculturation.
H2. 
Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation will be positively related to social capital, as greater integration into diverse networks provides greater access to resources.
H3. 
Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation will indirectly affect QoL through social capital and help-seeking but will not be directly related to Latino/a adults’ QoL.
H4. 
White American acculturation will be negatively related to help-seeking, while Latino/a enculturation will be positively related to help-seeking.
H5. 
Help-seeking will be positively related to QoL.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample and Data Collection Procedures

The original survey design focused on the acculturation, negative social exchange networks, and mental health of Latino/a adults. However, a secondary analysis (e.g., examination of existing data to test a different set of hypotheses than initially intended) was conducted using cross-sectional egocentric social network data. Egocentric approaches ask a person (i.e., identified as ego) to list several people in their social network to describe potential network relationships. A crowdsourced population identifying as Latino/a/x or Hispanic between 18 and 65 years of age (N = 300), fluent in English, and living in the United States was recruited through Prolific®. Table 1 provides additional demographic information about the sample.
Prolific® (Prolific Academic Ltd., London, UK) is a crowdsourcing platform that facilitates recruitment for research and employs state-of-the-art software for detecting bots that compromise data quality and study integrity. Prolific® distributes the researcher’s recruiting message and a link to an electronic survey to Prolific® members meeting the study’s criteria. Prolific® recruits individuals from across the United States, making samples more representative than other convenience samples (Palan & Schitter, 2018).
Prolific® facilitates the study by monitoring survey completion and automatically identifying those who have timed out or are taking excessively long to finish the survey. All participants were compensated at a flat rate specified by Prolific®. Members are recruited until the desired sample size for the study is filled. The sample size for the structural equation model was calculated using the Analytics Calculator Index (Soper, 2024). A sample size of N = 177 was recommended, estimating a medium effect size for the model of 0.30, with six latent variables, 35 observed variables, a power of 0.80, and a p-value of 0.05. However, sample sizes between 200 and 300 are considered minimally sufficient to detect meaningful effects in structural equation models (Harrington, 2009; Kline, 2005). Therefore, we recruited a sample of that size.
The construction of the survey and its implementation followed evidence-based practices outlined by the Tailored Design Method (Dillman et al., 2014). This approach uses standardized recruitment methods and evidence-based survey practices (e.g., the content of the recruitment message, the cognitive flow of the survey content, and question types and placement) to minimize survey errors (e.g., nonresponse) and maximize recruitment and retention (Dillman et al., 2014). All procedures were approved by the institutional review board associated with the study’s primary researcher.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Generational Status

Generational status was once used as a proxy for acculturation and is still helpful in demographically understanding where people are situated in their family’s migration history (Phinney, 2003; Thomson & Hoffman-Goetz, 2009). Participants were asked which of the following best described their generational status, with response options ranging from first to fourth generation. The following definitions were provided to participants. First-generation is defined as those participants who report nativity outside of the U.S. Those reporting second generation report nativity in the U.S. but have parents who report nativity outside of the U.S. Third-generation participants are those who are reporting nativity U.S. for themselves and their parents but report nativity outside of the U.S. for their grandparents. Lastly, fourth-generation participants report nativity in the U.S. for themselves, their parents, and their grandparents.

2.2.2. Quality of Life

Several items were combined to measure Latinos/as’ quality of life. Two items from the WHOQOL-BREF (WHO, 1998) measuring a person’s satisfaction with their physical health and rating of their overall quality of life were used. These two items have demonstrated a strong relationship to the physical, psychological, social, and environmental domains of the WHOQOL-BREF (Skevington et al., 2004). An additional item similar to physical health was also created that asked about respondents’ mental health. Response options for the items range from 1 (very dissatisfied/poor) to 5 (very satisfied/excellent). An exploratory factor analysis with these three items indicated a single factor explaining 69% of the variance with loading ranging from 0.81 to 0.84. While reliability and measurement error are closely related, certain forms of reliability or assessments of measurement error may be more pertinent (Mokkink et al., 2023). Composite reliability (CR) assesses whether the construct is reliable, with scores above 0.60 indicating adequate reliability (Ahmad et al., 2016; Fornell & Larcker, 1981). The CR for the current three-item scale is 0.96, suggesting strong reliability.

2.2.3. Acculturation

Network Acculturation

Two measures were used in the current study to represent different aspects of acculturation. Name generators emphasize a person’s network’s structure (e.g., size), followed by additional questions (e.g., name interpreters) that provide information about the network’s composition. Using name generators and interpreters, network measures focusing on language use have been used to measure network aspects of acculturation and as a potential determinant of psychological acculturation (Archuleta & Dajani, 2021; Archuleta et al., 2024). Therefore, the current study used name generators and name interpreters to assess different aspects of respondents’ social networks relative to acculturation. The current study placed greater emphasis on the composition of respondents’ networks, consistent with other network studies examining acculturation. These aspects have demonstrated a unique operationalization that is distinct from traditional psychological measures (see Archuleta et al., 2024). Using a name generator, respondents were asked to list individuals in their lives who were sources of stress. After listing the names, a piped text function in Qualtrics was used to create a follow-up question (i.e., name interpreter) asking respondents, “What language(s) do you speak with X?” with X representing the piped text name of the person identified by the respondent in the name generator. Response options ranged from 1 (all Spanish) to 5 (all English), with 3 representing equal use of Spanish and English. This question was used to create a network acculturation item of language use across all network members by summing and then mean-scoring items. Scores ranged from 1 (all Spanish) to 5 (all English), with higher scores indicating greater English use within one’s network. The CR for the network acculturation variable was 0.75, indicating an adequate level of reliability.

Enculturation and White American Acculturation

Additionally, a modified version of the Latino/a Acculturation Index (Archuleta & Dajani, 2024) was used to measure Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation. The LAI corresponds with traditional psychological measures in acculturation research, consisting of Latino/a enculturation and White American items measuring language use, identity and values, behaviors, and social relationships (Archuleta & Dajani, 2024). A previous validation identified a shortened and full-length version of the LAI. However, neither of these versions of the scale demonstrated adequate model-data fit. The current study sought to identify the strongest measurement model of psychological acculturation using these items, which resulted in a five-item measure of Latino/a enculturation and a five-item measure of White American acculturation. Among the Latino/a enculturation items, Latino/a relationships, traditions, ethnic pride, and traditions were retained. Among White American acculturation items, items reflected preferences for their children’s identity, consumption of American food, friendships with White Americans, and preference for English music. Response options for the items ranged from 0 (strongly disagree) to 10 (strongly agree), with scores ranging from 0 to 50 for the two combined measures. Higher scores in the Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation scales represent higher levels of enculturation and acculturation, respectively. The CR value for the modified version of the Latino/a enculturation scale was 0.86, while the CR value for the modified version of the White American acculturation scale was 0.76.

Social Capital

Several items (i.e., trust, social norms, and support network size) identified as useful measures of social capital (Archuleta et al., 2021; Nugraha et al., 2023) were used in the current study. The items “Most people can be trusted”, “I understand my social environment (e.g., the norms and ways)”, and “Overall, how many people could you contact if you need assistance (e.g., talk with someone, help you move, or give you a ride)?” were combined to create a latent social capital variable. Response options for the first two items ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Additionally, response options for the third item ranged from 1(0) to 7 (more than 5), with scores ranging from 3 to 17. Higher scores represent greater social capital. The CR for the current items was 0.70, indicating adequate reliability.

Help-Seeking

A modified version of the General Help Seeking Questionnaire (Deane et al., 2001) was used to measure a person’s intentions or willingness to seek assistance when experiencing psychological distress (i.e., personal or emotional problems or suicidal ideations). The modified version of the GHSQ consisted of 18 items asking about a respondent’s likelihood of seeking assistance from different informal (e.g., friends and family) and formal (e.g., mental health professional, phone/crisis line, and doctor/general practitioner) sources (Wilson et al., 2005). Response options for the scale items ranged from 1 (extremely unlikely) to 7 (extremely likely). While the complete scale was initially used, the item regarding other sources of help-seeking was missing a substantial amount of data and was removed from analyses. Additionally, the item asking about help negation was negatively related to other items in the measurement model, resulting in poor model fit. Negation items in other studies have been removed from analyses (see Wilson et al., 2005), which was done in the current study, given the items’ effect on model fit. As a result, these items were removed from the analyses.
The emergence of computer-based and app-based sources of assistance prompted the inclusion of an additional item in each scale (see Carroll & Rounsaville, 2010). The new item asked respondents about the likelihood of seeking help online or using a mental health app on their smartphone. Response options provided were consistent with the original scale. Scale scores for the modified version of the scale ranged from 18 to 126, with higher scores indicating a greater likelihood of help-seeking. The factor loading for the item in the general psychological help-seeking scale was 0.42 and 0.69 in the scale asking about suicidal ideations, producing comparable loadings to other items in their respective scales. The CR values for the general psychological help-seeking and suicidal ideations scales were 0.96 and 0.97, respectively, while the overall scale produced a CR of 0.95.

2.3. Analytic Plan

SPSS 29 and AMOS 29 were used to conduct analyses to test study hypotheses. Less than 1% of data were missing in the variables included for analyses. A median value replacement strategy was used to address the missing values in the data set (see Zhang, 2016). Univariate analyses (e.g., measures of central tendency and patterns of responses) were used to describe acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and QoL responses. Pearson’s correlations were computed to test initial associations between variables. Structural equation modeling was used to test the measurement models for latent variables and a conceptual model. Demographic variables may be important controls. However, they may add noise when they are not the focus of analyses or inaccurately provide control when insufficiently measured (Memon et al., 2024). Given that other study variables were the focus, demographic variables were not included in the structural equation model, including generational status, which often duplicates acculturation measures.
Measurement models identify the observed variables relative to a latent construct that delineates a proposed theoretical model, while the structural model tests hypothesized relationships between latent constructs that describe conceptual associations (Kline, 2015). While no single index indicates appropriate model fit, several indices used collectively can point to the appropriateness of the model (Roos & Bauldry, 2022). First, a chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio provides an initial assessment of the model, with ratios less than 2.00 and 3.00 indicating acceptable model fit (Abell et al., 2009). The normed fit index (NFI), relative fit index (RFI), incremental fit index (IFI), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), and comparative fit index (CFI) were used to assess fit. Values of 0.90 or larger suggest a reasonable model fit (Kline, 2005; Byrne, 2001), while values larger than 0.95 indicate a more robust model (T. A. Brown, 2006; Hu & Bentler, 1999).
The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) relaxes the idea of a perfect model and assesses approximal fit (Roos & Bauldry, 2022). Values less than 0.05 suggest a close fit, while values below 0.08 indicate an acceptable fit (Xia & Yang, 2019; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993). The standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) measures the discrepancy in fit between covariance matrices, with values below 0.08 suggesting an acceptable fit (MacCallum et al., 1996; Hu & Bentler, 1999). Modification indices were used to add paths between error variances within measures or between theoretically relevant constructs. For error variances, only paths within subscales were added, given the similarity between item content and to retain the conceptual distinction between the subscales. There are a number of pathways through which acculturation and social capital likely relate to QoL outcomes among Latinos/as. Therefore, several paths between acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and QoL were examined. The estimands function in AMOS was used to specify which direct and indirect paths and sources of serial mediation would be tested in the model. Bootstrapping was used in AMOS to generate estimates and p-values for direct and indirect effects in the model.

3. Results

3.1. Univariate Analysis

Table 2 provides the means and standard deviations for study variables. The average QoL score in the sample was 10.10 (SD = 2.51) out of a possible score of 15. Network language use score, representing network acculturation, averaged 4.01 (SD = 1.02), indicating a higher level of English use. Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation scores were 31.93 (SD = 11.76) and 25.80 (SD = 6.78) out of a possible 50. The sample’s mean social capital level was 11.15 (SD = 2.55) out of a possible 17. To further contextualize study variables, QoL, enculturation, White American acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking were examined descriptively by generational status, a proxy for acculturation. Figure 2 provides the means for each variable by generational status group.

3.2. Bivariate Analyses

Network acculturation was negatively correlated with Latino/a enculturation (r = −0.47, p < 0.001) and positively correlated with White American acculturation (r = 0.25, p < 0.001), but was not correlated with any other study variables. Table 2 provides the correlations between study variables. Latino/a enculturation was positively related to social capital (r = 0.12, p = 0.018), help-seeking (r = 0.25, p < 0.001), and QoL (r = 0.16, p = 0.002), suggesting that greater emersion into Latino/a culture is associated with several health benefits. White American acculturation demonstrated a similar relationship to social capital (r = 0.21, p < 0.001), help-seeking (r = 0.18, p = 0.001), and QoL (r = 0.14, p = 0.007); however, White American acculturation was more strongly related to social capital and weakly related to help-seeking compared to Latino/a enculturation. Social capital was moderately related to help-seeking. At the bivariate level, help-seeking was positively related to QoL (r = 0.34, p < 0.001).

3.3. Structural Equation Modeling

Several goodness of fit indices were examined to assess the fit of the data to the proposed measurement model. Figure 3 provides a structural evaluation model examining study variables with standard estimates for main effects. The chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (χ2/df = 967/498 = 1.94) was below 2.00, suggesting a good-fitting model.
The CFI (0.92), TLI (0.90), and IFI (0.92) were equal to or larger than 0.90, meeting the more liberal threshold. However, the NFI (0.85) and RFI (0.81) were all below 0.90. Similarly, RMSEA (0.06) was slightly below the more liberal threshold of 0.08. Lastly, the SRMR for the model was 0.08.
Both direct and indirect effects were examined in the structural equation model. Table 3 provides the direct effects examining network acculturation, acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking on Latino/a adults’ quality of life. Network acculturation was directly related to Latino/a adults’ enculturation (p = 0.001) and acculturation (p = 0.007). However, network acculturation demonstrated a stronger negative relationship with Latino/a enculturation than its positive relationship with White American acculturation, indicating that network language use associated with social relationships may play a more prominent role in the maintenance of Latino/a culture. Additionally, Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.002) and White American acculturation demonstrated (p = 0.003) a direct relationship with social capital, each contributing equally to accumulating social resources. Of the acculturation-related variables, only Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.029) was significantly related to help-seeking, with higher Latino/a enculturation associated with greater intentions to seek help. Social capital demonstrated a direct relationship with both help-seeking (p = 0.004) and quality of life (p < 0.003), with higher social capital associated with greater intention to seek help, along with greater QoL.
Several indirect effects and serial mediation were observed in the model. Table 4 provides standardized estimates, standard errors, and p-values for the indirect effects and serial mediation tested in the current study. Network acculturation demonstrated a statistically significant indirect effect on social capital through Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.001) and White American acculturation (p = 0.002), suggesting that network acculturation plays a role in maintaining and generating network resources. Multiple paths between network acculturation and help-seeking were examined. Pathways through Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.019) and serial mediation by enculturation and social capital (p = 0.001) were significant. Similarly, the relationship between network acculturation and help-seeking was also serially mediated through White American acculturation and social capital (p = 0.001), identifying multiple pathways through which network acculturation aids in accumulating resources that facilitate mental health help-seeking.
The indirect effects of Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation were examined independently of network acculturation. Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.001) and White American acculturation (p = 0.003) demonstrated a statistically significant effect on help-seeking through social capital. Indirect effects and serial mediation related to QoL were also examined. Network acculturation was significantly related to QoL through Latino/a enculturation and social capital (p = 0.002). Similarly, network acculturation was significantly related to QoL through White American acculturation and social capital (p = 0.003), identifying multiple pathways through which different aspects of acculturation were associated with QoL. Additionally, Latino/a enculturation (p = 0.003) and White American acculturation (p = 0.004) demonstrated significant indirect effects on QoL through social capital independent of network acculturation, indicating that psychological acculturation alone was also a driving factor associated with one’s QoL. No indirect path or serial mediation involving help-seeking was significantly associated with QoL.

4. Discussion

The current study investigated the roles of acculturation and social capital as determinants of help-seeking and QoL. Findings from the study make an important contribution to the acculturation literature by revealing the pathways through which network-related factors likely contribute to Latino/a’s decisions to seek help and facilitate access to social capital. Additionally, the current study is one of few studies that focuses on the contribution of negative relationships to a person’s psychological acculturative experiences. While network acculturation was not directly related to these outcomes, a thorough examination of indirect pathways and serial mediation across variables exposed a complex interplay between these factors, help-seeking, and QoL. While only Latino/a enculturation was directly related to help-seeking, enculturation and acculturation demonstrated indirect effects through social capital. However, help-seeking was not related to Latinos/as’ QoL, likely associated with the psychological difficulties and distress that drive help-seeking intentions.
Findings from the current study add depth to what is known about the relationship between network acculturation and psychological acculturation. Network acculturation demonstrated a stronger negative relationship to Latino/a enculturation compared to its positive relationship with White American acculturation. This suggests that knowing and using English in one’s social networks is more strongly related to whether one identifies with Latinx culture than White American culture. Maintaining one’s heritage language takes effort and commitment, particularly in communities where the language is not widely spoken. The lack of incorporation of Spanish into a person’s conversations and communications with others is connected to their integration into Latino/a culture.
The influence of network-derived acculturation aligns with stress-based acculturation models that identify group-level factors as determinants of a person’s acculturation (Berry & Sam, 2016; Ward & Geeraert, 2016). Here, network acculturation demonstrated a significant negative indirect effect on social capital and QoL through Latino/a enculturation. Interestingly, the opposite was observed through White American acculturation. That is, increases in network acculturation (i.e., English use across one’s network) were associated with greater social capital and quality of life for those more strongly identifying with White American culture. This suggests that those who more strongly identify with Latino/a culture but have a largely English-speaking social network would have less access to needed resources, which results in lower QoL. Conversely, those more strongly embracing White American culture with a largely English-speaking social network would experience greater integration and, therefore, greater access to social capital and quality of life. This finding highlights the role that social networks (i.e., how individuals are connected) play in how individuals acculturate and the importance of cultural fit and serves as a bridge to aspects of acculturation research emphasizing interaction within (i.e., intercultural) and between (i.e., intercultural) groups (e.g., pressure to acculturate).
As in other studies (Valencia-Garcia et al., 2012; Park et al., 2023), experiences with acculturation were related to a person’s social capital. Access to social capital is driven by group membership across several networks (e.g., family, friends, or coworkers), which is contingent on different characteristics seen as central to the group, including culture and race (McPherson et al., 2001). Bourdieu’s (1986) notion of cultural capital provides a bridge for understanding the role of acculturation. Although initially applied in the context of social class, cultural capital argues that cultural knowledge, behaviors, and values serve as substrates for building relationships that afford demonstrators particular privileges within networks. In the current study, individuals who were integrated into Latino/a and White American communities had the greatest amount of social capital, suggesting that integration may be the best acculturation strategy for maximizing potential support. This is consistent with what is observed above, which suggests that a potential lack of cultural congruence with one’s network may result in lower social capital that influences a person’s QoL.
The psychological (e.g., self-esteem) and social (e.g., access to social support) benefits of belonging are well-established in this literature (Keyes, 2017; Seeman, 1996). Cultural similarities likely facilitate membership in different networks by providing a common language, shared values, and engagement in similar activities (e.g., attending the same religious service) that build trust and a sense of reciprocity, and facilitate integration. However, Latino/a enculturation and White American acculturation were not uniformly related to help-seeking in the current study. Latino/a enculturation was directly related to help-seeking, but also demonstrated an indirect effect through social capital. This suggests that integration into Latino/a culture is important for generating resources and increasing a person’s intentions for seeking help. Conversely, White American acculturation was only indirectly related to help-seeking through social capital, indicating that it may be important in accessing needed resources, but not in directly seeking help. Latinos/as’ supportive networks are largely composed of family and friends (Archuleta et al., 2024), and therefore, cultural alignment likely strengthens ties in culturally homogenous networks and facilitates closer relationships that provide avenues for seeking assistance.
Interestingly, help-seeking was unrelated to QoL. A person’s intentions to seek help are associated with symptoms adversely affecting their life (Ayón et al., 2024). It is likely that individuals who are seeking assistance are experiencing psychological distress or need additional resources to manage their symptoms. While alleviating psychological distress and addressing resource gaps is critical, it may not translate to changes in QoL. A distinction has been made in the positive psychology literature that differentiates mental illness from mental health, suggesting that the alleviation of illness does not always translate to improved health (Keyes, 2017). For example, a person may not be satisfied by the improvements or changes in health associated with their help-seeking.
In the current study, social capital was the strongest predictor of QoL. Social capital has been a focus of public health and social epidemiological research for the last 20 years because of its connection to physical and mental health (Chen et al., 2015; Eriksson, 2011; Moore & Kawachi, 2017). Individuals with higher social capital have more extensive social networks that provide access to resources (Bourdieu, 1986; Lin, 2001). In the current study, social capital was a predictor of help-seeking. However, help-seeking was unrelated to QoL, suggesting that the benefits gleaned from social capital that enhance QoL may be unrelated to resources that help manage psychological distress. Social integration has been thought to yield direct benefits that enhance a person’s life that are distinct from other resources generated (R. L. Brown & Ciciurkaite, 2017). A more recent theoretical approach has suggested that social relationships are not simply determinants of health but reflect a distinct aspect of one’s health that contributes to a tripartite model of overall wellness (Doyle & Link, 2024). Poorer social capital may be indicative of strained relationships that are stress-evoking (Archuleta & Nuñez, 2023), while positive relationships may reflect a more robust network capable of producing social health benefits.
Several limitations are worthy of consideration in relation to the findings. First, the fit indices did not reach the anticipated thresholds, suggesting a need to improve the fit of the model. The lack of fit may be associated with several issues. The network measure of acculturation is limited to language use. Increasing the types of network acculturation measures may improve the measurement model for network acculturation and improve the overall model fit. Additionally, an English-speaking population was the focus of the study, which ultimately limited the acculturative experiences included in the analyses. While the AVE and CR were appropriate, the use of an English-speaking sample does pose a threat to the reliability and validity of measures. A more robust representation of acculturative experience will improve measurement and better model people’s experiences of acculturation. Future studies should expand network items to include different aspects of acculturation to determine whether other network indicators demonstrate a significant relationship with psychological acculturation and health outcomes. Moreover, a more linguistically diverse sample should be included to capture the range of acculturative experiences fully and further explore the meaningfulness of network variables. The items used to represent social capital are limited and largely represent personal social capital. Social capital is a complex construct that includes personal and ecological measures. Similarly, QoL represents numerous facets of a person’s life, requiring more extensive measures to best understand the impacts of these study variables on Latinos/a’s lives. Lastly, help-seeking was found to be unrelated to QoL. Help-seeking in the current study was measured by one’s intention to seek assistance, which differs from a person’s actual service use. Therefore, the intention to seek help was used as a proxy for help-seeking. Future research should include a person’s intention to seek help along with objective measures of help-seeking to determine whether help-seeking intentions are associated with QoL through one’s actual service use. Therefore, more robust measures are needed to further assess the relationships between variables examined in the model.

5. Conclusions

As health and quality of life models continue to improve in their descriptive abilities, acculturation becomes further situated as a key factor in accessing assistance and resources that are critical to Latino/a adults. Including network acculturation in research highlights its role as a social determinant of psychological acculturation experiences and other important outcomes that drive life quality. Network indicators of acculturation need to be expanded to include factors relevant to other acculturation domains beyond language (e.g., social relationships, cultural practices and behaviors, and values and beliefs) to understand whether more complete measures of network acculturation behave as anticipated. Future researchers should continue to explore determinants of acculturation, along with the role of acculturation as a predictor of health outcomes, to understand the complex pathways through which acculturative factors relate to health and QoL.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study’s conceptualization, design, and analyses. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University of Louisville (IRB # 24.0344; Approved 30 May 2024) for studies involving humans.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data associated with the current study are available upon reasonable request and in accordance with the granting institutional review board’s rules and regulations.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Abell, N., Springer, D. W., & Kamata, A. (2009). Developing and validating rapid assessment instruments. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahmad, S., Zulkurnain, N., & Khairushalimi, F. (2016). Assessing the validity and reliability of a measurement model in Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). British Journal of Mathematics & Computer Science, 15(3), 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Alegria, M. (2009). The challenge of acculturation measures: What are we missing? A commentary on Thomson & Hoffman-Goetz. Social Science & Medicine, 69, 996–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Allen, J. D., Caspi, C., Yang, M., Leyva, B., Stoddard, A. M., Tamers, S., Tucker-Seeley, R. D., & Sorensen, G. C. (2014). Pathways between acculturation and health behaviors among residents of low-income housing: The mediating role of social and contextual factors. Social Science and Medicine, 123, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Andersen, R. (1995). Revisiting the behavioral model and access to medical care: Does it matter? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 36(1), 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Arachchi, J., & Managi, S. (2023). The role of social capital in subjective quality of life. Nature: Humanities & Social Sciences. Communications, 10, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Archuleta, A. J., & Dajani, M. A. (2021). Exploring network derived indicators of acculturation among college enrolled Latinxs: Examining language use and homophily in support network. Journal of Community Psychology, 49(7), 2403–2423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Archuleta, A. J., & Dajani, M. A. (2024). The Latinx Acculturation Index (LAI): Reexamining the factor structure and convergent validity of an updated acculturation scale. Journal of Latinx Psychology, 13(1), 35–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Archuleta, A. J., & Nuñez, S. C. (2023). It’s mostly relative: Social capital characteristics in negative social exchange and support networks among college-enrolled Latino/a young adults. SN Social Science, 3, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Archuleta, A. J., & Perry, A. R. (2016). Family capital: Examining social capital, family commitment, and acculturation among college-enrolled Mexican-American men and women in the Southwestern United States. Journal of Family Social Work, 19(2), 132–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Archuleta, A. J., Prost, S. G., & Dajani, M. A. (2021). Examining the World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF: Assessing the structural and convergent validity in a general U.S. sample. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 33(1), 64–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Archuleta, A. J., Prost, S. G., & Dajani, M. A. (2024). Acculturation and negative relationships as determinants of well-being: Using the convoy model for understanding the influence of social relationships among Latinx young adults. Journal of Latinx Psychology, 12(1), 48–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ayón, C., Ellis, B. D., Hagan, M. J., Enriquez, L. E., & Offidani-Bertrand, C. (2024). Mental health help-seeking among Latina/o/x undocumented college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 30(3), 434–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ayón, C., & Naddy, M. B. G. (2013). Latino immigrant families’ social support networks: Strengths and limitations during a time of stringent immigration legislation and economic insecurity. Journal of Community Psychology, 41(3), 359–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Basch, E. (2014). New frontiers in patient-reported outcomes: Adverse event reporting, comparative effectiveness, and quality assessment. Annual Review of Medicine, 65, 307–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Berry, J. W. (2003). Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research. American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  17. Berry, J. W. (2006). Contexts of acculturation. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 27–44). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Berry, J. W., & Sam, D. L. (2016). Theoretical perspective. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (2nd ed., pp. 11–29). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bhattacharya, G. (2011). Global contexts, social capital, and acculturative stress: Experiences of Indian immigrant men in New York city. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 13, 756–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bourdieu, P. (1986). Forms of capital. In J. Richardson (Ed.), The Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education (pp. 241–258). Greenwood Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Brown, R. L., & Ciciurkaite, G. (2017). Understanding the connection between social support and mental health. In T. L. Scheid, & E. R. Wright (Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (3rd ed., pp. 207–222). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  23. Buijs, T., Maes, L., Salonna, F., Damme, J. V., Hublet, A., Kebza, V., Costongs, C., Currie, C., & De Clercq, B. (2016). The role of community social capital in the relationship between socioeconomic status and adolescent life satisfaction: Mediating or moderating? Evidence from Czech data. International Journal of Equity Health, 15, 203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  25. Carroll, K. M., & Rounsaville, B. J. (2010). Computer-assisted therapy in psychiatry: Be brave—It’s a new world. Current Psychiatry Reports, 12(5), 426–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Chen, X., Wang, P., Wegner, R., Gong, J., Fang, X., & Kaljee, L. (2015). Measuring social capital investment: Scale development and examination of link to social capital and perceived stress. Social Indicators of Research, 120, 669–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Choy, B., Arunachalam, K., Gupta, S., Taylor, M., & Lee, A. (2021). Systematic review: Acculturation strategies and their impact on the mental health of migrant populations. Public Health in Practice, 2, 100069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Clement, S., Schauman, O., Graham, T., Maggioni, F., Evans-Lacko, S., Bezborodovs, N., Morgan, C., Rüsch, N., Brown, J. S. L., & Thornicroft, G. (2015). What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(1), 11–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Concha, M., Sanchez, M., De la Rosa, M., & Villar, M. E. (2014). A longitudinal study of social capital and acculturation related stress among recent Latino immigrants in South Florida. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 35(4), 469–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Coulter, A., & Ellins, J. (2007). Effectiveness of strategies for informing, educating, and involving patients. British Medical Journal, 335(7609), 24–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Deane, F. P., Wilson, C. J., & Ciarrochi, J. (2001). Suicidal ideation and help-negation: Not just hopelessness or prior help. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(7), 901–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Derr, A. S. (2016). Mental health service use among immigrants in the United States: A systematic review. Psychiatric Services, 67(3), 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Dillman, D. A., Smyth, J. D., & Christian, L. M. (2014). Internet, phone, mail and mixed-mode surveys: The tailored design method (4th ed.). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  34. Doyle, D. M., & Link, B. G. (2024). On social health: History, conceptualization, and population patterning. Health Psychology Review, 18(3), 619–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Eriksson, M. (2011). Social capital and health—Implications for health promotion. Global Health Action, 4, 5611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ferguson, G. M., & Birman, D. (2016). Acculturation in the United States. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 396–416). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fung, K. (2015). Psychological acculturation. Encyclopedia of Immigrant Health. In S. Loue, & M. Sajatovic (Eds.), Encyclopedia of immigrant health (pp. 1233–1234). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Funk, C., & Lopez, M. H. (2022). Hispanic Americans’ trust in and engagement with science. A brief statistical portrait of US Hispanics. Pew Research Center. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2022/06/14/hispanic-americans-trust-in-and-engagement-with-science/ (accessed on 14 June 2022).
  40. Gao, B., Yang, S., Lui, X., Ren, X., Liu, D., & Li, N. (2018). Association between social capital and quality of life among urban residents in less developed cities of western China. Medicine, 97(4), e9656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gómez, E., Urzúa, A., & Glass, C. R. (2014). International student adjustment to college: Social networks, acculturation, and leisure. Journal of Park and Recreation Administration Spring, 32(1), 7–25. [Google Scholar]
  42. Gordon, M. M. (1964). Assimilation in American Life: The role of race, religion, and national origins. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gruenberg, E. M. (1977). The failures of success. The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 83(4), 779–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gulliver, A., Griffiths, K. M., & Christensen, H. (2010). Perceived barriers and facilitators to mental health help-seeking in young people: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 10, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Haraldstad, K., Wahl, A., Andenæ, R., Anderson, M. H., Beisland, E., Borge, C. R., & Engebretsen, E. (2019). A systematic review of quality of life research in medicine and health sciences. Quality of Life, 28, 2641–2650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Harrington, D. (2009). Confirmatory factor analysis. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariances structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Scientific Software International. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kawachi, I., & Berkman, L. (2014). Social cohesion, social capital, and health. In L. F. Berkman, I. Kawachi, & M. M. Glymour (Eds.), Social epidemiology (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Keefe, S. E. (1984). Real and ideal extended familism among Mexican Americans and Anglo Americans: On the meaning of “close” family ties. Human Organization, 43(1), 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Keyes, C. L. M. (2017). The Dual Continua Model: The Foundation of the Sociology of Mental Health and Mental Illness. In T. L. Scheid, & E. R. Wright (Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (3rd ed., pp. 66–81). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  52. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kochhar, R., Suro, R., & Tafoya, S. (2005). The New Latino South: The context and consequences of rapid population growth. Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lin, N. (2001). Social capital: A theory of social structure and action. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  56. MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1, 130–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Marín, G. (1992). Issues in the measurement of acculturation among Hispanics. In K. F. Geisinger (Ed.), Psychological testing of Hispanics (pp. 235–251). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Markova, V., Sandal, G. M., & Pallesen, S. (2020). Immigration, acculturation, and preferred help-seeking sources for depression: Comparison of five ethnic groups. BMC Health Services Research, 20, 648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Marsden, P. V. (2011). Survey Methods for Network Data. In J. Scott, & P. J. Carrington (Eds.), The sage handbook of social network analysis (pp. 370–388). Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  60. Masgoret, A., & Ward, C. (2006). Cultural learning approach to acculturation. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 58–77). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Maupome, G., McConnell, W. R., Perry, B. L., Marion, R., & Wright, E. R. (2016). Psychological and behavioral acculturation in social network of Mexican Americans in the United States and use of dental services. Community Dental Oral Epidemiology, 44, 540–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. McDermott, R. C., Cheng, H., Wong, J., Booth, N., Jones, Z., & Sevig, T. (2017). Hope for help-seeking: A positive psychology perspective of psychological help-seeking intentions. The Counseling Psychology, 45(2), 237–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Memon, M. A., Thurasamy, R., Ting, H., Cheah, J., & Chuah, F. (2024). Control variables: A review and proposed guidelines. Journal of Applied Structural Equation Model, 8(2), 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Mohammadifirouzeh, M., Oh, K. M., Basnyat, I., & Gimm, G. (2023). Factors associated with professional mental help-seeking among U.S. immigrants: A systematic review. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 25(5), 1118–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Mokkink, L. B., Eekhout, I., Boers, M., Van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & de Vet, H. C. W. (2023). Studies on reliability and measurement error of measurements in medicine—From design to statistics explained for medical researchers. Patient Related Outcome Measurement, 14, 193–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Moore, S., & Kawachi, I. (2017). Twenty years of social capital and health research: A glossary. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 17, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Nugraha, A. T., Prayitno, G., Azizi, F. A., Sari, N., Hidayana, I. I., Auliah, A., & Siankwilimba, E. (2023). Structural Equation Model (SEM) of social capital with landowner intention. Economies, 11, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Nutakor, J. A., Zhou, L., Larnyo, E., Addai-Danso, S., & Tripura, D. (2023). Socioeconomic status and quality of life: As assessment of the mediating effect of social capital. Healthcare, 11(5), 749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Olshansky, S. J., Rudberg, M. A., Carnes, B. A., Cassel, C. K., & Brody, J. A. (1991). Trading off longer life for worsening health: The expansion of morbidity hypothesis. Journal of Aging and Health, 3(2), 194–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Palan, S., & Schitter, C. (2018). Prolific. ac—A subject pool for online experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 17, 22–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Park, J., Aranda, M. P., Choi, Y. J., & Jang, Y. (2023). The role of ethnic community social capital in the association between acculturation and psychological distress among older Korean Americans. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 25, 608–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Phinney, J. S. (2003). Ethnic identity and acculturation. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Marín (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research (pp. 63–82). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  74. Piachaud, D., Bennett, F., Nazroo, J., & Popay, J. (2009). Social inclusion and social mobility. UCL Institute of Health Equity. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rechel, B., Jagger, C., & Mckee, M. (2020). Living longer, but in better or worse health? (J. Cylus, C. Normand, J. Figueras, J. North, & C. White, Eds.) European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  76. Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovits, M. (1936). Memorandum for the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38(1), 149–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Rodionov, G. Y. (2021). The relationship between social capital and acculturation expectations of Estonians in Estonia: Perceived threat as a mediator. Культурнo-Истoрическая Психoлoгия, 17(4), 74–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Rodriguez-Alcalá, M. E., Qin, H., & Jeanetta, S. (2019). The role of acculturation and social capital in access to health care: A meta-study on Hispanics in the US. Journal of Community Health, 44, 1224–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Roos, J. M., & Bauldry, S. (2022). Confirmatory factor analysis. Quantitative applications in the social sciences. Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  80. Saint Arnault, D. (2009). Cultural determinants of help seeking: A model for research and practice. Research and Theory for Nursing Practice, 23(4), 259–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Salaheddin, K., & Mason, B. (2016). Identifying barriers to mental health help-seeking among young adults in the U.K.: A cross-sectional survey. British Journal of General Practice, 66(651), e686–e692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Seeman, T. E. (1996). Social ties and health: The benefits of social integration. Annals of Epidemiology, 6(5), 442–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Skevington, S. M., Lofty, M., & O’Connell, K. A. (2004). The World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: Psychometric properties and results of the international field trial: A report from the WHOQOL Group. Quality of Life Research, 13, 299–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Soper, D. S. (2024). Structural equation model sample size calculator [Online Software]. Available online: https://www.analyticscalculators.com (accessed on 1 March 2025).
  85. Stone, W., & Hughes, J. (2002, 30 October–1 November). Measuring social capital: Towards a standardised approach. Australasian Evaluation Society International Conferences, Wollongong, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  86. Teramoto, P. J., & Edwards, L. M. (2014). Perspectives on the intersection of multiculturalism and positive psychology. Springer. [Google Scholar]
  87. Thoman, L. V., & Surís, A. (2004). Acculturation and acculturative stress as predictors of psychological distress and Quality-of-Life functioning in Hispanic psychiatric patients. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 26(3), 293–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Thomson, M. D., & Hoffman-Goetz, L. (2009). Defining and measuring acculturation: A systematic review of public health studies with Hispanic populations in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 69, 983–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. U.S. Census Bureau. (2024). U.S. census bureau quick facts: United States; U.S. Census Bureau QuickFacts.
  90. Vacca, R., Solano, G., Lubbers, M. J., Molina, J. L., & McCarty, C. (2018). A personal network approach to the study of immigrant structural assimilation and transnationalism. Social Networks, 53, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Valencia-Garcia, D., Simoni, J. M., Alegria, M., & Takeuchi, D. T. (2012). Social capital, acculturation, mental health, and perceived access to services among Mexican American Women. Journal of Consult and Clinical Psychology, 80(2), 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Wang, M. (2023). The influence of acculturation on students’ language proficiency. Curriculum and Teaching Methodology, 6(18), 94–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Ward, C., & Geeraert, N. (2016). Advancing acculturation theory and research: The acculturation process in its ecological context. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 98–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Weinreich, P. (2009). “Enculturation” not “acculturation”: Conceptualising and assessing identity process in migrant communities. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 124–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Williams, J. A., Jr., & Ortega, S. T. (1990). Dimensions of ethnic assimilation: An empirical appraisal of Gordon’s typology. Social Science Quarterly, 71(4), 697–710. [Google Scholar]
  96. Wilson, C. J., Deane, F. P., Ciarrochi, J., & Rickwood, D. (2005). Measuring help-seeking intentions: Properties of the general help-seeking questionnaire. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 39(1), 15–28. [Google Scholar]
  97. World Health Organization. (1998). Well-being measures in primary healthcare/The depcare project. WHO Regional Office for Europe. [Google Scholar]
  98. World Health Organization. (2004). Promoting mental health: Concepts, emerging evidence, practice: Summary report. WHO. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9241562943 (accessed on 1 March 2025).
  99. World Health Organization. (2012). WHOQOL user manual. The World Health Organization quality of life (WHOQOL). Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/WHO-HIS-HSI-Rev.2012-3 (accessed on 1 March 2025).
  100. Xia, Y., & Yang, Y. (2019). RMSEA, CFI, and TLI in structural equation modeling with ordered categorical data: The story they tell depends on the estimation methods. Behavioral Research Methods, 51(1), 409–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Zhang, Z. (2016). Multiple imputation with multivariate imputation by chained equation (MICE) package. Annals of Translational Medicine, 4(2), 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Zubaran, C., Emerson, J., Sud, R., Zolfaghari, E., & Foresti, K. (2012). The application of the drug user quality of life scale (DUQOL) in Australia. Health Quality of Life Outcomes, 10, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual model depicting the associations between network and psychological acculturation and social capital, help-seeking, and QoL.
Figure 1. Conceptual model depicting the associations between network and psychological acculturation and social capital, help-seeking, and QoL.
Behavsci 15 00388 g001
Figure 2. Means of network acculturation, Latino/a enculturation, White American acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and QoL by generational status group.
Figure 2. Means of network acculturation, Latino/a enculturation, White American acculturation, social capital, help-seeking, and QoL by generational status group.
Behavsci 15 00388 g002
Figure 3. Structural model examining network and psychological acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking on quality of life (QoL). * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 3. Structural model examining network and psychological acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking on quality of life (QoL). * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Behavsci 15 00388 g003
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 300).
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 300).
M/%
Age
Gender
Men43.3%
Women56.7%
100.00%
Generational Status
1st8.3%
2nd55.0%
3rd23.0%
4th13.3%
Missing0.4%
100%
Relationship Status
Single47.7%
Married26.3%
Divorced1.0%
Living with Partner11.7%
In a Committed Relationship13.3%
100.00%
Highest Level of Education
Less than High School1.7%
High School Diploma16.7%
Vocational Training3.7%
Associate’s degree10.7%
Some College20.0%
Bachelor’s Degree34.3%
Master’s Degree10.3%
PhD, MD, JD. Or other doctorate1.7%
Missing0.90%
100%
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and Pearson’s correlations between study variables.
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and Pearson’s correlations between study variables.
123456
  • Network Acculturation
1
2.
Latino/a Enculturation
−0.47 ***1
3.
WA Acculturation
0.25 ***−0.17 **1
4.
Social Capital
−0.040.12 *0.21 ***1
5.
Help-Seeking
−0.040.25 ***0.18 **0.36 ***1
6.
QoL
−0.040.16 **0.14 **0.53 ***0.34 ***1
M4.01331.9325.8011.1562.6710.10
SD1.0211.766.782.5523.272.51
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Direct effects of network acculturation, Latino/a enculturation, White American acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking on QoL.
Table 3. Direct effects of network acculturation, Latino/a enculturation, White American acculturation, social capital, and help-seeking on QoL.
Standardized EstimateS.E.Critical Ratiop Value
NA→Latino/a Enculturation−0.830.69−3.380.002 **
NA→WA Acculturation 0.470.182.710.003 **
Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital0.360.043.210.002 **
WA Acculturation→Social Capital0.350.152.280.003 *
Social capital→Help-Seeking0.480.263.980.004 **
Latino/a Enculturation→Help-Seeking0.210.052.670.029 **
WA Acculturation→Help-Seeking0.130.161.420.226
Social Capital→QoL0.960.354.070.003 **
Help-Seeking→QoL−0.150.12−0.700.277
Latino/a Enculturation→QoL0.010.05−0.030.923
WA Acculturation→QoL−0.040.15−0.320.821
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; NA = network acculturation; WA = White American.
Table 4. Indirect effects and serial mediation between study variables.
Table 4. Indirect effects and serial mediation between study variables.
Indirect Paths/Serial MediationStandardized EstimateS.E.p Value
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital −0.290.150.001 **
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Help-Seeking −0.290.210.019 *
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking −0.230.160.001 **
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→QoL−0.430.310.002 **
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→QoL−0.010.170.911
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Help-Seeking→QoL0.040.050.143
NA→Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→Help-seeking→QoL 0.030.100.203
NA→WA Acculturation→QoL−0.020.090.836
NA→WA Acculturation→Social Capital0.160.070.002 **
NA→WA Acculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking0.130.080.001 **
NA→WA Acculturation→Help-Seeking0.100.080.161
NA→WA Acculturation→Social Capital→QoL0.240.130.003 **
NA→WA Acculturation→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.010.020.288
NA→WA Acculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking→QoL −0.020.040.398
Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking 0.100.050.001 **
Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→QoL 0.190.090.003 **
Latino/a Enculturation→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.020.020.192
Latino/a Enculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.020.030.212
WA Acculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking 0.270.230.003 **
WA Acculturation→Social Capital→QoL 0.500.390.004 **
WA Acculturation→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.010.050.327
WA Acculturation→Social Capital→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.020.100.398
Social Capital→Help-Seeking→QoL−0.100.270.249
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; NA = network acculturation; WA = White America.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Archuleta, A.J.; Prost, S.G.; Dajani, M.A. Quality of Life Among Latino/a Adults: Examining the Serial Mediation of Network Acculturation, Psychological Acculturation, Social Capital, and Helping-Seeking. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 388. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15030388

AMA Style

Archuleta AJ, Prost SG, Dajani MA. Quality of Life Among Latino/a Adults: Examining the Serial Mediation of Network Acculturation, Psychological Acculturation, Social Capital, and Helping-Seeking. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(3):388. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15030388

Chicago/Turabian Style

Archuleta, Adrian J., Stephanie Grace Prost, and Mona A. Dajani. 2025. "Quality of Life Among Latino/a Adults: Examining the Serial Mediation of Network Acculturation, Psychological Acculturation, Social Capital, and Helping-Seeking" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 3: 388. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15030388

APA Style

Archuleta, A. J., Prost, S. G., & Dajani, M. A. (2025). Quality of Life Among Latino/a Adults: Examining the Serial Mediation of Network Acculturation, Psychological Acculturation, Social Capital, and Helping-Seeking. Behavioral Sciences, 15(3), 388. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15030388

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop