Next Article in Journal
Vehicle Delay Model Applied to Dynamic and Static Traffic Impact Analysis of Large Parking Lots
Next Article in Special Issue
Composition of the Wild Rabbit Nest and Its Implication for Domestic Rabbit Breeding
Previous Article in Journal
A Deep Convolutional Neural Network-Based Multi-Class Image Classification for Automatic Wafer Map Failure Recognition in Semiconductor Manufacturing
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Observing Brief and Irregular Behaviour of Animals—The Validity of Short Observation Periods

1
Institute of Animal Breeding and Husbandry, Christian-Albrechts-University, Olshausenstraße 40, D-24098 Kiel, Germany
2
Institute for Animal Hygiene, Animal Welfare and Farm Animal Behaviour, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Bischhofsholer Damm 15, D-30173 Hannover, Germany
3
Unit for Biomathematics and Data Processing, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Justus Liebig University, Frankfurter Str. 95, D-35392 Giessen, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2021, 11(20), 9770; https://doi.org/10.3390/app11209770
Submission received: 27 September 2021 / Revised: 14 October 2021 / Accepted: 16 October 2021 / Published: 19 October 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Animal Behavior in Intensive Culture Environment)

Abstract

:
There are efficient sampling methods to accurately estimate behaviour with a moderate or long duration. For short behaviour, observing animals continuously is recommended although there is no recommended minimum observation time. In most studies, sampling method and observation time per day is determined by practical considerations. Thus, this study analysed the validity of behavioural observations in different observation periods using continuous sampling (CS) or time sampling (TS) based on biting behaviour. Tail-biting and ear-biting of weaned piglets in six pens were continuously observed for 12 h per day for 4 days to form a reference. Shorter observation periods of CS and TS were simulated by taking subsets of this reference. The amount of behaviour per hour of each observation period was compared to the reference and to other observation period of the same kind and length. Four different measurements were defined to calculate accuracy scores (AS; 0–1; higher values are better). Comparison to the reference shows better AS for observation periods with longer observation time in total (0.5 h of CS: 0.2; 6 h of CS: 0.6). Additionally, TS covers longer time periods without decreasing AS. However, focus on activity time results in an overestimation of irregular behaviour. Comparing AS among observation periods of the same kind and length show overall low agreement. This study indicated problems of different observation periods of CS and TS to accurately estimate behaviour. Therefore, validity of behavioural observations should be analysed in greater detail to determine optimal sampling methods.

1. Introduction

A valid database is important for every scientific study. In most cases, studying animal behaviour relies on animal observations to acquire this database. However, behavioural observations are highly time-consuming. Since the time used for observations is often restricted by practical considerations, different observation techniques are used to estimate the behaviour as effectively as possible by reducing the required analysis time [1]. For example, instead of using continuous sampling, which observes the animals continuously for a certain time-period, time sampling only observes the animals for portions of time with breaks in between. Here, the animals are still observed continuously but not for the complete time-period. Focal sampling observes only one animal continuously to facilitate the observation, and scan sampling periodically analyses snapshots after a determined time interval [2]. As each method has advantages and disadvantages, it is important to choose the appropriate method regarding the behaviour, situation and hypothesis being investigated to achieve a valid database. Thus, several studies have evaluated the validity of sampling methods of different behaviour. It has been shown that focal sampling can estimate behaviour correctly which do not differ significantly between the animals [3] and scan sampling is a very efficient method to estimate behaviour with a long duration such as resting behaviour [4,5,6,7,8]. For behaviour with a shorter duration, however, the time between intervals has to be adjusted, i.e., shortened, to ensure validity. Thus, it can be difficult to observe social interactions as this behaviour often varies between the animals and only has a short duration. For example, biting behaviour in pigs takes only a few seconds and is easily missed by scan sampling if the time interval between scans is too long. However, shortening the interval as far as to accurately estimate biting behaviour (e.g., scan sampling every 5 s) would be less efficient than using continuous sampling. Thus, time sampling or continuous sampling is recommended to observe short behaviour such as biting behaviour in pigs [5,9,10,11,12], although there is no recommendation regarding the required observation length. The observation time per day is often determined by practical factors and rarely exceeds 2 h per day [13,14,15,16,17,18].
Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyse the validity of different observation periods of different length comparing continuous sampling and time sampling on the basis of tail-biting behaviour and ear-biting behaviour of pigs.

2. Materials and Methods

This study used behavioural observations of a tail-biting behaviour study in pigs [19], which investigated tail-biting behaviour in weaned piglets from November 2016 until April 2017 on the agricultural research farm “Futterkamp”, which is part of the Chamber of Agriculture of Schleswig-Holstein, Germany. There were six conventional pens with 24 piglets per pen, which were weaned after 28 days and then video recorded for 40 days using one AXIS M3024-LVE Network Camera per pen produced by Axis Communications. The piglets were individually marked to allow identification in the videos. Additionally, tail-lesions were scored twice a week using the “German Pig Scoring Key” (German designation: Deutscher Schweine Boniturschlüssel) [20]. If a pig was identified as sick or injured, it was treated in the pen or removed to a hospital pen. The day of a tail-biting outbreak was defined for each pen individually as the first scoring day on which at least one large tail-lesion (larger than the diameter of the tail) was documented. If a tail-biting outbreak was identified, further pen enrichment was added to the pen and biting pigs were removed. From this day on, the previous four days were analysed using continuous event sampling during hours of light (6:00 to 18:00). One trained observer analysed the tail- and ear-biting behaviour of the pigs. This behaviour was defined as manipulating, sucking or chewing on the tail or ear of another piglet [15]. The time, the initiator and the receiver of the behaviour was documented. These behavioural observations of the 12 h observation period formed the reference. For the analysis of this study, it was assumed that these represented reality without missing true events or adding false events. All smaller observation periods were later compared to this reference.
To simulate shorter observation periods of continuous sampling or time sampling, the dataset of the continuous observation of 12 h per day was used as a starting point for subsampling. See Table 1 and Figure 1 for an overview of the observation periods used. The 12 h (06:00 to 18:00) were evenly divided into smaller time intervals forming shorter observation periods (6 h, 3 h, 1 h and 0.5 h) of continuous sampling. However, the observation periods 3 h, 1 h and 0.5 h were limited to the activity times of the pigs (08:30 to 11:30 in the morning and 14:30 to 17:30 in the afternoon). The activity time was defined as time periods with the most biting behaviour per hour and it was determined beforehand. Observation periods of the same length did not overlap. For example, the first observation period of 1 h started 08:30 and ended 09:30, the second observation period of 1 h started 9:30 and ended 10:30, etcetera. This resulted in one 12 h observation period, two 6 h observation periods, two 3 h observation periods, six 1 h observation periods and twelve 0.5 h observation periods per day. Additionally, the first 10 min of each 30 min were used to simulate time sampling. Here, the observation periods covered the 3 h of activity time in the morning or in the afternoon (6 × 10 min), a combination of both activity times (2 × 6 × 10 min), 6 h in the morning or afternoon (12 × 10 min) and the whole day during the 12 h of light (24 × 10 min). Again, observation periods of the same length did not overlap. For example, the observation period of 6 × 10 min started at 08:30 and analysed the first 10 min of each 30 min for the following 3 h (08:30–08:40, 09:00–09:10, 09:30–09:40, 10:00–10:10, 10:30–10:40 and 11:00–11:10). The second observation of 6 × 10 min started at 08:40 and analysed the first 10 min of each 30 min for the following 3 h (08:40–08:50, 09:10–09:20, 09:40 –09:50, 10:10–10:20, 10:40–10:50 and 11:10–11:20), etcetera. This resulted in three 24 × 10 min observation periods, six 12 × 10 min observation periods, three 2 × 6 × 10 min observation periods and six 6 × 10 min observation periods per day.
The behaviour illustrated was counted within each observation period for each pig and each behaviour (initiating tail-biting behaviour, receiving tail-biting behaviour, initiating ear-biting behaviour and receiving ear-biting behaviour) separately. To simplify the comparison between different lengths of observation periods, the amount of behaviour illustrated was adjusted to the amount of shown behaviour per hour. Then, all observation periods were compared twice for each behaviour separately: Comparison 1; compared all observation periods to the continuous observation period of 12 h to analyse the agreement to the reference, i.e., the representation of reality. Comparison 2; compared all observation periods of the same kind (continuous sampling or time sampling) and length with each other to analyse the agreement between the observation periods. It was known that the amount of biting behaviour shown increased over the four observation days [19]. Thus, differences between the days would arise from the developing tail-biting outbreak and not from measurement errors of the chosen sampling method or observation period. Because this study focused on the differences of observation periods and sampling methods, each day was used as an independent unit and all comparisons were limited to the observation periods within a day. Different measurements of accuracy were used for the comparisons.
On the one hand, a measurement relied on the numerical agreement. The amount of behaviour of a pig shown in one observation period was compared to the amount of behaviour of the same pig shown in the other observation period. However, a tolerance range was applied, because it was highly unlikely to observe an animal showing behaviour in two observation periods for the exact same amount of time. The robustness of the database of this study was analysed beforehand for the effect of missing data. It could be shown that observation periods of different length were robust against missing data if an error rate of 10% was not exceeded. Based on these results, the tolerance range was set to ±10% as values within this tolerance range could still be used for an agreement. Other tolerance ranges were also tested. Wider tolerance ranges increased the resulting accuracy scores and their variance; however, the ratio between the mean accuracy scores of the different observation periods did not change. Still, the tolerance range was set to ±10%. Therefore, the agreement was either 1 (the amount of behaviour of a pig shown in one observation period was within the range of the amount of behaviour ±10% shown in the second observation period) or 0 (the amount of behaviour of a pig was not within the range of the amount of behaviour ±10% shown in the second observation period). The percentage of agreements (PA) was then calculated per pen.
On the other hand, measurements relied on the stability of the ranking order within a pen. The measurement Overlap Top 1 checked whether the animal(s) in the highest ranking in an observation period had the highest rank in the second observation period as well. This was defined as
OTop 1 = [U∩V]/[U∪V],
where U is the set of animals of the highest ranking in one observation period and V is the set of animals of the highest ranking in the other observation period [21]. Similarly, the measurement Overlap Top 3 checked whether the animals in the highest three rankings of one observation period were among the highest three rankings of the second observation period. This was defined as
OTop 3 = [U∩V]/[U∪V],
where U is the set of animals in the highest three rankings of one observation period and V is the set of animals in the highest three rankings of the other observation period [21].
The last measurement was the squared Spearman correlation coefficient, which analyses the changes in the whole ranking order between one observation period and the other. It can be interpreted as the proportion of variance in the ranking order of one observation period accounted for by the observed ranks of the other observation period [21].

3. Results

The analysis of the 288 h of video footage produced a total of 18,364 observations divided into 9950 tail-biting behaviour events and 8414 ear-biting behaviour events. The mean, standard deviation and range of the four types of behaviour analysed (initiating tail-biting behaviour, receiving tail-biting behaviour, initiating ear-biting behaviour and receiving ear-biting behaviour) according to the observation periods used are shown in Table 2. There is a wide span between the mean and extreme values.
The results of comparison 1 (comparison of different observation periods to the reference of 12 h continuous observation) are shown in Figure 2a–d according to behaviour analysed and measurement of accuracy used. Generally, the accuracy scores range from 0 to 1, whereas the mean accuracy scores range from 0.03 to 0.77. The differences between continuous sampling observation periods and time sampling observation periods come mainly from an overall trend of longer observation periods producing better accuracy scores. Apart from this, the accuracy scores are on a similar level. The accuracy scores of the different behaviours are on a similar level as well, although initiating tail-biting behaviour has slightly better scores. Comparing the different measurements of accuracy shows better accuracy scores for the measurement based on the ranking order.
In Figure 3a–d, the results of comparison 2 (comparison between the observation periods of the same kind and length) are displayed. Here, the mean accuracy scores range from 0.05 to 0.47. Analogous to the comparison 1, the initiating tail-biting behaviour produces slightly better accuracy scores. Additionally, the measurement PA produces the best accuracy scores for observation periods of 1 h or shorter.

4. Discussion

This study investigated the validity of behavioural observations of biting behaviour in pigs. Observation periods of different length using continuous sampling or time sampling were compared to the observations of 12 h continuous sampling, which were set as the reference. The validity evaluates whether a measurement accurately measures reality [22]. Thus, the biting behaviour of pigs was analysed in as much detail as possible by a thoroughly trained observer for 12 h per day to approximately represent reality. Therefore, the observations were defined as an accurate representation of reality without missing events or adding false events to be able to test subsets of shorter observation periods for their validity.
In the comparison of different observation periods to the reference of 12 h continuous observation, the mean accuracy scores differed according to the observation period used. However, the differences in total analysis time accounted for this effect more than for example the differences between continuous sampling and time sampling. Here, analysing the video footage for a longer time in total resulted in better accuracy scores. Comparing observation periods of continuous sampling and time sampling that cover the same time-period (e.g., the activity time), it becomes clear that there is a loss of information resulting in worse accuracy scores of the time sampling observation periods, whereas the accuracy scores are at a similar level if observation periods of continuous sampling and time sampling which invested the same analysis time (e.g., 1 h during the activity time) are compared. Thus, time sampling is a trade-off as it accepts a loss in information in exchange for saving analysis time. However, this trade-off can be beneficial to use the limited time, which can be invested in observations more efficiently, because it allows for longer time periods without increasing the required analysis time and without decreasing accuracy, which is more important.
Comparing the accuracy scores of the behaviour analysed (initiating tail-biting behaviour, receiving tail-biting behaviour, initiating ear-biting behaviour and receiving ear-biting behaviour) shows small differences, but they follow the same trends according to measurement of accuracy and observation period used. However, the accuracy scores of initiating tail-biting behaviour are slightly better for the measurements based on the ranking order (Top 1, Top 3 and RS²). Here, the wider range in the amount of behaviour and the extreme values caused greater differences between the animals, especially in the higher rankings. Therefore, the ranking order was less affected by shorter observation periods with a disproportionate distribution of behaviour shown. This resulted in better accuracy scores of initiating tail-biting behaviour.
When evaluating the results of the accuracy scores it is essential to keep in mind the different foci of the measurements of agreement and the hypothesis of the study to be investigated. The measurements Top 1 and Top 3 are the proportion of correctly chosen animals in the highest or the three highest rankings in a pen based on the behaviour shown. RS² gives an insight into the stability of the ranking order of the whole pen and PA is the proportion of correctly estimated amount of behaviour shown. Thus, it is important to choose a measurement that is appropriate for the hypothesis under investigation. Besides, for the measurement PA, it has to be considered when the animals are observed and whether the behaviour is regularly shown throughout the day, as there are differences in the social interaction of animals during activity or resting periods [23]. In this study, for example, tail-biting behaviour was shown more often during the activity time in the morning or in the afternoon. Thus, focusing the observations on activity time and applying this to the whole day overestimated certain behaviour, since resting periods were neglected. As a result, the accuracy scores of observation periods focusing on the activity time were worse for the measurement PA compared to observation periods which included inactivity time.
Moreover, the comparison between the observation periods of the same kind and length produced mostly low accuracy scores. Therefore, the starting point of the observation can also affect the results considerably, which was demonstrated by Hämäläinen et al. [24] as well. This can complicate comparability between studies if animals are observed at different times or if animals differ in their daily routine because of variations in, for example, feeding schedules. Thus, sampling method, observation length as well as observation time should be carefully considered.
However, the question is whether it is possible to distinguish between bad, moderate or good accuracy. Or whether it is possible to determine a required minimum analysis time per day, bearing in mind that it is unusual for studies on biting behaviour to observe animals for longer than 2 h per day [13,14,15,16,17,18]? There are defined thresholds for the evaluation of correlation coefficients [2]. Applying them to RS², an accuracy score between 0.16–0.49 results in moderate accuracy, between 0.49–0.81 in good accuracy and an accuracy score above 0.81 results in very good accuracy. If these thresholds are applied to the other measurements of accuracy as well, no observation period would exceed the threshold for very good accuracy on average. Moreover, not all observation periods of a total analysis time of 2 h or longer would exceed the threshold for good accuracy on average. For the measurement PA, only mean accuracy scores of the observation periods longer than 4 h would produce a moderate accuracy. Thus, the ranking order could be estimated with an at least moderate validity, whereas the amount of behaviour shown could not be estimated with an at least moderate validity except for the longest tested observation periods.
Nevertheless, these are results of biting behaviour that is difficult to observe because it is irregular and has a short duration [9]. Thus, it should be analysed whether the results presented are transferable to other types of behaviour as well. As different studies have demonstrated, behaviour can be efficiently and accurately estimated using scan sampling for behaviour with a long duration (e.g., resting) [4,6,8,9,10,11] or time sampling for behaviour with a moderate duration (e.g., feeding and locomotion) [3,8]. However, in other studies on pigs, it was not possible to accurately estimate different behaviour with short duration using scan sampling, such as pain associated behaviour [12] or agonistic or sexual behaviour [9]. Hence, the validity of observations of short-term and irregular behaviour should be investigated in greater detail to approve results of existing studies and to determine optimal observation techniques for future studies.

5. Conclusions

This study investigated the validity of different observation periods and sampling methods on the basis of biting behaviour of pigs. It could be shown that observation periods which analysed more time in total produced better accuracy scores. Additionally, time sampling covers longer time periods without decreasing the accuracy compared to continuous sampling. Focusing on the activity time of the animals can lead to overestimation if the behaviour is not shown regularly during the day. This study demonstrated difficulties in choosing the appropriate observation period or sampling method to accurately observe short and irregular behaviour. Therefore, the validity of other behavioural observations should be further investigated in future studies to develop optimal sampling methods.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.W., J.K. and K.B.; methodology, T.W., J.K. and K.B.; software, T.W. and K.B.; validation, J.K. and N.K.; formal analysis, T.W. and K.B.; investigation, T.W., J.K. and K.B.; resources, J.K. and K.B.; data curation, T.W.; writing—original draft preparation, T.W.; writing—review and editing, T.W., J.K., N.K. and K.B.; visualization, T.W.; supervision, J.K., N.K. and K.B.; project administration, K.B.; funding acquisition, J.K. and K.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by H.W. Schaumann Foundation.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study, because the animals were housed under standard commercial conditions and no pain, suffering or injury was inflicted on the animals during the experiments. The authors declare that the experiments were carried out strictly following international animal welfare guidelines. Additionally, the “German Animal Welfare Act” (German designation: TierSchG), the “German Order for the Protection of Animals used for Experimental Purposes and other Scientific Purposes (German designation: TierSchVersV) and the “German Order for the Protection of Production Animals used for Farming Purposes and other Animals kept for the Production of Animal Products” (German designation: TierSchNutztV) were applied.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy concerns.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Altmann, J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Martin, P.; Bateson, P. Measuring Behaviour: An Introduction Guide, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnold-Meeks, C.; McGlone, J.J. Validating techniques to sample behavior of confined, young pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1986, 16, 149–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bowden, J.M.; Karriker, L.A.; Stalder, K.J.; Johnson, A.K. Scan Sampling Techniques for Behavioral Validation in Nursery Pigs. Iowa State Univ. Anim. Ind. Rep. 2008, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Choi, C.-Y.; Nam, H.-Y.; Lee, W.-S. Measuring the Behaviors of Wintering Black-faced Spoonbills (Platalea minor): Comparison of Behavioral Sampling Techniques. Waterbirds 2007, 30, 310–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Mitlöhner, F.M.; Morrow-Tesch, J.L.; Wilson, S.C.; Dailey, J.W.; McGlone, J.J. Behavioral sampling techniques for feedlot cattle. J. Anim. Sci. 2001, 79, 1189–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Jauhiainen, L.; Korhonen, H.T. Optimal behaviour sampling and autocorrelation curve: Modelling data of farmed foxes. Acta Ethol 2005, 8, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Daigle, C.L.; Siegford, J.M. When continuous observations just won’t do: Developing accurate and efficient sampling strategies for the laying hen. Behav. Processes 2014, 103, 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Oliveira, G.F.D.; Caldara, F.R.; Garcia, R.G.; Seno, L.D.O.; Marcon, Á.V.; Foppa, L.; Martins, R.A.; Crone, C. Different intervals of behavioral observation in the scanning method and the real behavior of pigs. R. Bras. Zootec. 2018, 47, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Chen, J.M.; Schütz, K.E.; Tucker, C.B. Technical note: Comparison of instantaneous sampling and continuous observation of dairy cattle behavior in freestall housing. J. Dairy Sci. 2016, 99, 8341–8346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pullin, A.N.; Pairis-Garcia, M.D.; Campbell, B.J.; Campler, M.R.; Proudfoot, K.L. Technical note: Instantaneous sampling intervals validated from continuous video observation for behavioral recording of feedlot lambs. J. Anim. Sci. 2017, 95, 4703–4707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Park, R.M.; Cramer, M.C.; Wagner, B.K.; Turner, P.; Moraes, L.E.; Viscardi, A.V.; Coetzee, J.F.; Pairis-Garcia, M.D. A comparison of behavioural methodologies utilised to quantify deviations in piglet behaviour associated with castration. Anim. Welf. 2020, 29, 285–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Statham, P.; Green, L.; Bichard, M.; Mendl, M. Predicting tail-biting from behaviour of pigs prior to outbreaks. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 121, 157–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Brunberg, E.; Wallenbeck, A.; Keeling, L.J. Tail biting in fattening pigs: Associations between frequency of tail biting and other abnormal behaviours. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 133, 18–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Zonderland, J.J.; Schepers, F.; Bracke, M.B.M.; den Hartog, L.A.; Kemp, B.; Spoolder, H.A.M. Characteristics of biter and victim piglets apparent before a tail-biting outbreak. Animal 2011, 5, 767–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Ursinus, W.W.; van Reenen, C.G.; Kemp, B.; Bolhuis, J.E. Tail biting behaviour and tail damage in pigs and the relationship with general behaviour: Predicting the inevitable? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 156, 22–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Munsterhjelm, C.; Heinonen, M.; Valros, A. Can tail-in-mouth behaviour in weaned piglets be predicted by behaviour and performance? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 184, 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lahrmann, H.P.; Hansen, C.F.; D’Eath, R.; Busch, M.E.; Forkman, B. Tail posture predicts tail biting outbreaks at pen level in weaner pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 200, 29–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wilder, T.; Krieter, J.; Kemper, N.; Honeck, A.; Büttner, K. Tail-directed behaviour in pigs–relation to tail posture and tail lesion. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 233, 105151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Anonymus. Deutscher Schweine-Boniturschlüssel (DSBS). Available online: https://www.fli.de/fileadmin/FLI/ITT/Deutscher_Schweine_Boniturschluessel_2016–06-30_de.pdf (accessed on 30 June 2016).
  21. Borgatti, S.P.; Carley, K.M.; Krackhardt, D. On the robustness of centrality measures under conditions of imperfect data. Soc. Netw. 2006, 28, 124–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Campbell, D.T. Factors relevant to the validity of experiments in social settings. Psychol. Bull. 1957, 54, 297–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Harcourt, A.H. Activity Periods and Patterns of Social Interaction: A Neglected Problem. Behaviour 1978, 66, 121–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hämäläinen, W.; Ruuska, S.; Kokkonen, T.; Orkola, S.; Mononen, J. Measuring behaviour accurately with instantaneous sampling: A new tool for selecting appropriate sampling intervals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 180, 166–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Overview of observation periods used (OP). The grey sections display the earliest examples of each kind of observation period per day. The activity times were 08:30 to 11:30 and 14:30 to 17:30.
Figure 1. Overview of observation periods used (OP). The grey sections display the earliest examples of each kind of observation period per day. The activity times were 08:30 to 11:30 and 14:30 to 17:30.
Applsci 11 09770 g001
Figure 2. Agreement to reference of 12 h observation period. Accuracy scores ± standard deviation of the number of shown (a) initiating tail-biting, (b) receiving tail-biting, (c) initiating ear-biting and (d) receiving ear-biting according to the observation period and measurement of accuracy (Top 1: Overlap Top 1, Top 3: Overlap Top 3, RS²: Squared Spearman correlation coefficient and PA: percentage of agreement).
Figure 2. Agreement to reference of 12 h observation period. Accuracy scores ± standard deviation of the number of shown (a) initiating tail-biting, (b) receiving tail-biting, (c) initiating ear-biting and (d) receiving ear-biting according to the observation period and measurement of accuracy (Top 1: Overlap Top 1, Top 3: Overlap Top 3, RS²: Squared Spearman correlation coefficient and PA: percentage of agreement).
Applsci 11 09770 g002aApplsci 11 09770 g002b
Figure 3. Agreement among observation periods of the same kind (continuous sampling or time sampling) and length. Accuracy scores ± standard deviation of the number of shown (a) initiating tail-biting, (b) receiving tail-biting, (c) initiating ear-biting and (d) receiving ear-biting according to the observation period and measurement of accuracy (Top 1: Overlap Top 1, Top 3: Overlap Top 3, RS²: Squared Spearman correlation coefficient and PA: percentage of agreement).
Figure 3. Agreement among observation periods of the same kind (continuous sampling or time sampling) and length. Accuracy scores ± standard deviation of the number of shown (a) initiating tail-biting, (b) receiving tail-biting, (c) initiating ear-biting and (d) receiving ear-biting according to the observation period and measurement of accuracy (Top 1: Overlap Top 1, Top 3: Overlap Top 3, RS²: Squared Spearman correlation coefficient and PA: percentage of agreement).
Applsci 11 09770 g003aApplsci 11 09770 g003b
Table 1. Overview of the observation periods used (OP). The activity times were 08:30 to 11:30 and 14:30 to 17:30.
Table 1. Overview of the observation periods used (OP). The activity times were 08:30 to 11:30 and 14:30 to 17:30.
Observation Period (OP)Sampling MethodHours CoveredHours AnalysedNumber of Different OP per DayLimited to Activity Time
12 hcontinuous sampling12121no
6 hcontinuous sampling662no
3 hcontinuous sampling332yes
1 hcontinuous sampling116yes
0.5 hcontinuous sampling0.50.512yes
24 × 10 mintime sampling1243no
12 × 10 mintime sampling626no
2 × 6 × 10 mintime sampling623yes
6 × 10 mintime sampling316yes
Table 2. Mean ± standard deviation and the range of the amount of behaviour per hour per pig according to the behaviour and observation period.
Table 2. Mean ± standard deviation and the range of the amount of behaviour per hour per pig according to the behaviour and observation period.
Initiating BehaviourReceiving Behaviour
BehaviourObservation PeriodMean ± Standard DeviationRangeMean ± Standard DeviationRange
Tail-Biting12 h1.45 ± 1.770–17.671.45 ± 1.180–9.33
Tail-Biting6 h1.45 ± 2.100–25.331.45 ± 1.420–12
Tail-Biting3 h1.57 ± 2.420–26.671.57 ± 1.620–13
Tail-Biting1 h1.57 ± 3.120–541.57 ± 2.160–24
Tail-Biting0.5 h1.57 ± 3.690–741.57 ± 2.660–26
Tail-Biting24 × 10 min1.45 ± 1.930–21.251.45 ± 1.310–12.25
Tail-Biting12 × 10 min1.45 ± 2.340–31.51.45 ± 1.630–13.5
Tail-Biting2 × 6 × 10 min1.57 ± 2.220–23.51.57 ± 1.560–13
Tail-Biting6 × 10 min1.57 ± 2.840–401.57 ± 2.010–17
Ear-Biting12 h1.23 ± 1.000–7.331.23 ± 0.830–5.08
Ear-Biting6 h1.23 ± 1.270–131.23 ± 1.060–6.33
Ear-Biting3 h1.36 ± 1.600–15.331.36 ± 1.340–9
Ear-Biting1 h1.36 ± 2.220–331.36 ± 1.880–15
Ear-Biting0.5 h1.36 ± 2.790–541.36 ± 2.400–22
Ear-Biting24 × 10 min1.23 ± 1.160–91.23 ± 0.980–7.25
Ear-Biting12 × 10 min1.23 ± 1.520–16.51.23 ± 1.280–9.5
Ear-Biting2 × 6 × 10 min1.36 ± 1.480–131.36 ± 1.300–10.5
Ear-Biting6 × 10 min1.36 ± 2.000–211.36 ± 1.740–12
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wilder, T.; Krieter, J.; Kemper, N.; Büttner, K. Observing Brief and Irregular Behaviour of Animals—The Validity of Short Observation Periods. Appl. Sci. 2021, 11, 9770. https://doi.org/10.3390/app11209770

AMA Style

Wilder T, Krieter J, Kemper N, Büttner K. Observing Brief and Irregular Behaviour of Animals—The Validity of Short Observation Periods. Applied Sciences. 2021; 11(20):9770. https://doi.org/10.3390/app11209770

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wilder, Thore, Joachim Krieter, Nicole Kemper, and Kathrin Büttner. 2021. "Observing Brief and Irregular Behaviour of Animals—The Validity of Short Observation Periods" Applied Sciences 11, no. 20: 9770. https://doi.org/10.3390/app11209770

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop