Concurrent Performance of Executive Function during Acute Bouts of Exercise in Adults: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Data Sources and Search Strategy
2.2. Study Selection
2.3. Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria
2.4. Methodological Quality of Included Studies
2.5. Data Extraction of Included Studies
3. Results
3.1. Study Selection and Study Characteristics
3.2. Study Quality
3.3. Study Findings
4. Discussion
4.1. Main Findings
4.2. Role of Exercise Intensity
4.3. Role of Participants’ Physical Fitness and Exercise Mode
4.4. Time Point of the EF Task Administration
4.5. Neural Physiological Responses
4.6. Strengths and Limitations
4.7. Perspectives and Future Direction
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Stillman, C.M.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Brown, B.; Bender, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. Effects of Exercise on Brain and Cognition Across Age Groups and Health States. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Pühse, U.; Looser, V.N.; Kamijo, K. Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 603–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hillman, C.H.; Erickson, K.I.; Kramer, A. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kramer, A.; Erickson, K.I. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: Influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chaddock, L.; Pontifex, M.; Hillman, C.; Kramer, A.F. A Review of the Relation of Aerobic Fitness and Physical Activity to Brain Structure and Function in Children. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 975–985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Magalhães, S.; Carneiro, L.; Limpo, T.; Filipe, M. Executive functions predict literacy and mathematics achievements: The unique contribution of cognitive flexibility in grades 2, 4, and 6. Child Neuropsychol. 2020, 26, 934–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lin, J.; Wang, K.; Chen, Z.; Fan, X.; Shen, L.; Wang, Y.; Yang, Y.; Huang, T. Associations Between Objectively Measured Physical Activity and Executive Functioning in Young Adults. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2017, 125, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cox, E.P.; O’Dwyer, N.; Cook, R.; Vetter, M.; Cheng, H.L.; Rooney, K.; O’Connor, H. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: A systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 616–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, T.; Tarp, J.; Domazet, S.L.; Thorsen, A.K.; Froberg, K.; Andersen, L.B.; Bugge, A. Associations of Adiposity and Aerobic Fitness with Executive Function and Math Performance in Danish Adolescents. J. Pediatr. 2015, 167, 810–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lautenschlager, N.T.; Cox, K.L.; Flicker, L.; Foster, J.K.; Van Bockxmeer, F.M.; Xiao, J.; Greenop, K.R.; Almeida, O.P. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease—A randomized trial. JAMA 2008, 300, 1027–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xue, Y.; Yang, Y.; Huang, T. Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2019, 53, 1397–1404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moreau, D.; Chou, E. The Acute Effect of High-Intensity Exercise on Executive Function: A Meta-Analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 14, 734–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Brand, S.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; Pühse, U. Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 1611–1626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Joyce, J.; Graydon, J.; McMorris, T.; Davranche, K. The time course effect of moderate intensity exercise on response execution and response inhibition. Brain Cogn. 2009, 71, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Agbangla, N.F.; Maillot, P.; Vitiello, D. Mini-Review of Studies Testing the Cardiorespiratory Hypothesis With Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS): Overview and Perspectives. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 699948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dustman, R.E.; Ruhling, R.O.; Russell, E.M.; Shearer, D.E.; Bonekat, H.; Shigeoka, J.W.; Wood, J.S.; Bradford, D.C. Aerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individuals. Neurobiol. Aging 1984, 5, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davranche, K.; Audiffren, M. Facilitating effects of exercise on information processing. J. Sports Sci. 2004, 22, 419–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stone, B.L.; Beneda-Bender, M.; Mccollum, D.L.; Sun, J.; Shelley, J.H.; Ashley, J.D.; Fuenzalida, E.; Kellawan, J.M. Understanding cognitive performance during exercise in Reserve Officers’ Training Corps: Establishing the executive function-exercise intensity relationship. J. Appl. Physiol. 2020, 129, 846–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davranche, K.; McMorris, T. Specific effects of acute moderate exercise on cognitive control. Brain Cogn. 2009, 69, 565–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dietrich, A.; Sparling, P.B. Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain Cogn. 2004, 55, 516–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Audiffren, M.; Tomporowski, P.D.; Zagrodnik, J. Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: Modulation of executive control in a Random Number Generation task. Acta Psychol. 2009, 132, 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Komiyama, T.; Tanoue, Y.; Sudo, M.; Costello, J.T.; Uehara, Y.; Higaki, Y.; Ando, S. Cognitive Impairment during High-Intensity Exercise: Influence of Cerebral Blood Flow. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2019, 52, 561–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martins, A.Q.; Kavussanu, M.; Willoughby, A.; Ring, C. Moderate intensity exercise facilitates working memory. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2013, 14, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lambourne, K.; Audiffren, M.; Tomporowski, P.D. Effects of Acute Exercise on Sensory and Executive Processing Tasks. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2010, 42, 1396–1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Joyce, J.; Smyth, P.J.; Donnelly, A.E.; Davranche, K. The Simon Task and Aging: Does Acute Moderate Exercise Influence Cognitive Control? Med. Sci. Sport Exerc. 2014, 46, 630–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomporowski, P.D. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. 2002, 112, 297–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lambourne, K.; Tomporowski, P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010, 1341, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chang, Y.; Labban, J.; Gapin, J.; Etnier, J. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schmit, C.; Brisswalter, J. Executive functioning during prolonged exercise: A fatigue-based neurocognitive perspective. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2018, 13, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Grp, P. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Me-ta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 89, 873–880. [Google Scholar]
- Elkins, M.R.; Moseley, A.M.; Sherrington, C.; Herbert, R.D.; Maher, C.G. Growth in the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) and use of the PEDro scale. Br. J. Sports Med. 2012, 47, 188–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- PEDro Scale. Available online: https://pedro.org.au/english/resources/pedro-scale/ (accessed on 1 March 2021).
- Norton, K.; Norton, L.; Sadgrove, D. Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2010, 13, 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davranche, K.; Hall, B.; McMorris, T. Effect of Acute Exercise on Cognitive Control Required during an Eriksen Flanker Task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2009, 31, 628–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Del Giorno, J.M.; Hall, E.E.; O’Leary, K.C.; Bixby, W.R.; Miller, P.C. Cognitive function during acute exercise: A test of the transient hypofrontality theory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2010, 32, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dodwell, G.; Müller, H.J.; Töllner, T. Electroencephalographic evidence for improved visual working memory performance during standing and exercise. Br. J. Psychol. 2018, 110, 400–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Komiyama, T.; Katayama, K.; Sudo, M.; Ishida, K.; Higaki, Y.; Ando, S. Cognitive function during exercise under severe hypoxia. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 10000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Komiyama, T.; Sudo, M.; Higaki, Y.; Kiyonaga, A.; Tanaka, H.; Ando, S. Does moderate hypoxia alter working memory and executive function during prolonged exercise? Physiol. Behav. 2015, 139, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucas, S.J.; Ainslie, P.N.; Murrell, C.J.; Thomas, K.N.; Franz, E.A.; Cotter, J.D. Effect of age on exercise-induced alterations in cognitive executive function: Relationship to cerebral perfusion. Exp. Gerontol. 2012, 47, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMorris, T.; Davranche, K.; Jones, G.; Hall, B.; Corbett, J.; Minter, C. Acute incremental exercise, performance of a central executive task, and sympathoadrenal system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009, 73, 334–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ogoh, S.; Tsukamoto, H.; Hirasawa, A.; Hasegawa, H.; Hirose, N.; Hashimoto, T. The effect of changes in cerebral blood flow on cognitive function during exercise. Physiol. Rep. 2014, 2, e12163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olson, R.; Chang, Y.-K.; Brush, C.J.; Kwok, A.N.; Gordon, V.X.; Alderman, B.L. Neurophysiological and behavioral correlates of cognitive control during low and moderate intensity exercise. NeuroImage 2016, 131, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pontifex, M.; Hillman, C.H. Neuroelectric and behavioral indices of interference control during acute cycling. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 570–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmit, C.; Davranche, K.; Easthope, C.S.; Colson, S.S.; Brisswalter, J.; Radel, R. Pushing to the limits: The dynamics of cognitive control during exhausting exercise. Neuropsychologia 2015, 68, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, M.; Tallis, J.; Miller, A.; Clarke, N.; Guimarães-Ferreira, L.; Duncan, M. The effect of exercise intensity on cognitive performance during short duration treadmill running. J. Hum. Kinet. 2016, 51, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wang, C.-C.; Chu, C.-H.; Chu, I.-H.; Chan, K.-H.; Chang, Y.-K. Executive function during acute exercise: The role of exercise intensity. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 35, 358–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brisswalter, J.; Collardeau, M.; René, A. Effects of Acute Physical Exercise Characteristics on Cognitive Performance. Sports Med. 2002, 32, 555–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davey, C. Physical exertion and mental performance. Appl. Ergon. 1974, 5, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kamijo, K.; Nishihira, Y.; Hatta, A.; Kaneda, T.; Kida, T.; Higashiura, T.; Kuroiwa, K. Changes in arousal level by differential exercise intensity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004, 115, 2693–2698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dietrich, A.; Audiffren, M. The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 1305–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Study (Authors, Methodological Quality) | Participants Description | Study Design | Exercise Protocol | Time of EF Test Administration and Duration | EF Task | EF Domain | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Audiffren et al. [22] 5/11 | Female: 21.11 ± 1.05 Male: 21.14 ± 0.69 (N = 18) 9M/18 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 90% VT (moderate intensity); exercise duration (40 min) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 5 times) | RNG | Working memory; inhibitory control | Inhibitory control was impaired while working memory did not differ from the control conditions. The EF modulation can be interpreted as a change in strategy. |
Davranche et al. [35] 7/11 | 30 ± 8 (N = 14) 11M/14 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 50% MAP (moderate intensity); Exercise duration (two periods of 20 min cycling) | Four blocks of task trials were performed during a first 15-min period and another four blocks were performed during a second 15-min period. | Flanker task | Inhibitory control | The task performance was not different from the control condition. |
Davranche and McMorris [20] 7/11 | 32 ± 9 (N = 12) 8M/12 | Within-subject design | Cycling at VT intensity (moderate intensity); exercise duration (20 min) | The Simon task began at the end of the 3-min warm-up period and performed within the remaining 17-min period. | Simon task | Inhibitory control | According to the Simon effect, inhibitory control (RT) was impaired. |
Del Giorno et al. [36] 7/11 | 20.2 ± 1.1 (N = 30) 17M/30 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 75% VT and VT intensity (light and moderate intensity); exercise duration (30 min) | Cognitive tests began at 20 min following the onset of exercise, lasting for approximately 4 min. | CPT; WCST | Inhibitory control; cognitive flexibility | The performance of two tasks (RA) was impaired during exercise at both light and moderate intensities. |
Dietrich and Sparling [21] 7/11 | |||||||
Exp.1 | 23.7 ± 9.4 (N = 24) 24M/24 | Between-subject design | Cycling or running at 70–80% HRmax (vigorous intensity); exercise duration (50 min) | Cognitive tests began after 25 min of exercise, lasting for approximately 10 min. | WCST | Cognitive flexibility | For the WCST, the exercise group made significantly more errors compared to the control group. |
Exp.2 | 25.1 ± 6.3 (N = 8) 8M/8 | Within-subject design | Running at vigorous intensity; exercise duration (65 min) | After 25 min of exercise, lasting for approximately 28 min. | PASAT | Working memory | For the PASAT, the exercise condition resulted in significantly more errors than the control condition. |
Dodwell et al. [37] 5/11 | 24.5 ± 2.6 (N = 18) 10M/18 | Within-subject design | Cycling or running at 65% HRR intensity (vigorous intensity) | The Retro-cue task began following 5–10 min of warm-up period, included 4 blocks of 96 trials. | Retro-cue task | Working memory | RT was facilitated in the exercise condition compared to the control condition. |
Joyce et al. [15] 7/11 | 23 ± 2 (N = 10) 7M/10 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 40% MAP (moderate intensity); exercise duration (30 min) | The Stop-signal task was performed whilst cycling after a 4-min warm-up period and lasted approximately 22 min. | Stop-signal task | Inhibitory control | Inhibitory control was improved during exercise (shorter RT without a change in RA). |
Joyce et al. [26] 7/11 | 23 ± 2 (N = 12) 3M/12 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 65% of HRmax (moderate intensity); exercise duration (30 min) | The Simon task was performed after 5-min warm-up period and lasted approximately 23 min. | Simon Task | Inhibitory control | According to the Simon effect, inhibitory control was unchanged during exercise. |
Komiyama et al. [38] 6/11 | 21.5 ± 3.5 (N = 13) 13M/13 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 50% VO2max (moderate intensity); exercise duration (20 min) | The EF tasks were started after a 5-min warm-up period. | Spatial DR task; Go/No-Go task | Working memory; inhibitory control | The task performance (RT) was improved during exercise without sacrificing RA. |
Komiyama et al. [39] 6/11 | 23.0 ± 2.3 (N = 16) 16M/16 | Within-subject design | Cycling at heart rate of 140 beats/min (moderate intensity); exercise duration (30 min) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 2 times) | Spatial DR task; Go/No-Go task | Working memory; inhibitory control | RA was not changed in the Spatial DR task; RT was shorter without sacrificing RA in the Go/No-Go task. |
Komiyama et al. [23] 6/11 | 22.1 ± 1.7 (N = 17) 17M/17 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 50%VO2 peak (moderate intensity) for 8 min; thereafter, participants cycled at 80% VO2 peak (vigorous intensity) for an additional 8 min. | Participant performed the EF tasks 3 min after commencing each workload. | Spatial DR task; Go/No-Go task | Working memory; inhibitory control | RA of the tasks was impaired during vigorous-intensity exercise, whereas it was not changed during moderate-intensity exercise; RT was not changed during both intensity exercises. |
Lambourne et al. [25] 7/11 | 21.1 ± 1.7 (N = 19) 8M/19 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 90% VT intensity (moderate intensity); exercise duration (40 min) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 5 times) | PASAT | Working memory | RA of the task in the exercise condition did not differ from the control condition. |
Lucas et al. [40] 6/11 | 24 ± 5 (N = 13) 7M/13 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 30% followed by 70% of HRR (light and vigorous intensity); exercise duration (two 8-min bouts of cycling) | The Stroop task involved 2 blocks of 20 trials. | Stroop task | Inhibitory control | RT was facilitated during exercise. Vigorous-intensity exercise led to greater improvement compared to light-intensity exercise. |
Martins et al. [24] 7/11 | |||||||
Exp. 1 | 20.50 ± 0.89 (N = 24) 24M/24 | Between-subject design | Cycling at moderate intensity; (short duration) | Four blocks lasting approximately 8 min. | PASAT | Working memory | RA of the task was improved during moderate-intensity exercise. |
Exp. 2 | 19.57 ± 0.83 (N = 120) 55M/120 | Mixed Multi-factorial experimental design | Cycling at light and moderate intensity; (short duration) | Two blocks lasting approximately 16 min. | Sternberg task | Working memory | Light and moderate intensity exercise lowered the response latency slopes, resulting in improved working memory. |
McMorris et al. [41] 7/11 | 24.32 ± 7.10 (N = 24) 24M/24 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 50% and 80% MAP (moderate and vigorous intensity); exercise duration (15 min or until voluntary exhaustion) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 3 times) | Flanker task | Inhibitory control | Vigorous-intensity exercise impaired RT, but moderate-intensity exercise did not change the task performance. |
Ogoh et al. [42] 6/11 | 20.4 ± 0.6 (N = 7) 7M/7 | Within-subject design | Cycling at heart rate of 140 beats/min (moderate intensity); exercise duration (50 min) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 4 times) | Stroop task | Inhibitory control | RT was facilitated during exercise without any loss of performance accuracy. |
Olson et al. [43] 7/11 | 20.4 ± 2.0 (N = 27) 16M/27 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 40% and 60% VO2peak (light and moderate intensity); exercise duration (31 min) | Intermittent assessment (a total of 3 times) | Flanker task | Inhibitory control | RA was impaired during both light and moderate intensity exercise, but RT was facilitated during moderate-intensity exercise. |
Pontifex and Hillman [44] 7/11 | 20.2 ± 1.6 (N = 41) 15M/41 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 60% of HRmax (moderate intensity); exercise duration (approximately 11 min) | The Simon task was performed after 5 min of exercise, lasing for approximately 6.5 min. | Flanker task | Inhibitory control | Exercise did not affect RT but showed a decrease in RA for incongruent trials, resulting in impaired inhibitory control. |
Schmit et al. [45] 7/11 | 22.1 ± 0.6 (N = 15) 10M/15 | Within-subject design | Cycling at 85% MAP until exhaustion; exercise duration (approximately 7 min) | Participants performed the Flanker task until exhaustion. | Flanker task | Inhibitory control | RT was facilitated during exercise in the initial stage and remained unaltered in the final stage. |
Smith et al. [46] 7/11 | 28 ± 5 (N = 15) 6M/15 | Within-subject design | Running at moderate and high intensity; exercise duration (10 min) | The EF task was performed during the last 2 min of exercise. | Go/No-Go task | Inhibitory control | RT was impaired during high-intensity exercise, whereas it was not changed during moderate-intensity exercise. |
Stone et al. [19] 5/11 | 19.6 ± 2 (N = 13) 8M/13 | Within-subject design | Conducted at an exercise intensity in an incremental manner; the average duration was between 20–24 min. | The OWAT test was administered throughout the entirety of the graded exercise test. | OWAT | Cognitive flexibility | RA was not changed at an intensity from 20% to 80% HRR, where it was impaired from 80% to 100% HRR. |
Wang et al. [47] 7/11 | 20.51 ± 1.99 (N = 80) 49M/80 | Between-subject design | Cycling at 30%, 50%, and 80% HRR (light, moderate, and vigorous intensity); exercise duration (30 min) | The WCST was performed 6 min after exercise onset. | WCST | Cognitive flexibility | Cognitive flexibility (WCST indices) was impaired in the group of vigorous intensity, whereas it was not changed in groups of light and moderate intensity compared to the control group. |
Intensity | Facilitation | No Effect | Impairment |
---|---|---|---|
Light | ●● | ●●● | ●● |
Moderate | ●●●●●●●● | ●●●●●●●●●●● | ●●●●● |
Vigorous-high | ●●● | ●●●●●●●● |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zheng, K.; Zou, L.; Wei, G.; Huang, T. Concurrent Performance of Executive Function during Acute Bouts of Exercise in Adults: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1364. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101364
Zheng K, Zou L, Wei G, Huang T. Concurrent Performance of Executive Function during Acute Bouts of Exercise in Adults: A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(10):1364. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101364
Chicago/Turabian StyleZheng, Kefeng, Liye Zou, Gaoxia Wei, and Tao Huang. 2021. "Concurrent Performance of Executive Function during Acute Bouts of Exercise in Adults: A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 11, no. 10: 1364. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101364
APA StyleZheng, K., Zou, L., Wei, G., & Huang, T. (2021). Concurrent Performance of Executive Function during Acute Bouts of Exercise in Adults: A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences, 11(10), 1364. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101364