Understanding Temporal Relations in Mandarin Chinese: An ERP Investigation
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Comprehension of Before and After
- (1a)
- Before the scientist submitted the article, the journal changed its policy.
- (1b)
- After the scientist submitted the article, the journal changed its policy.
- (2a)
- The journal changed its policy before the scientist submitted the article.
- (2b)
- The journal changed its policy after the scientist submitted the article.
1.2. How World Knowledge Is Activated during Temporal Connectives Processing
- (3a)
- After the global economic crisis, securing a mortgage was harder.
- (3b)
- After the global economic crisis, securing a mortgage was easy.
- (3c)
- Before the global economic crisis, securing a mortgage was easy.
- (3d)
- Before the global economic crisis, securing a mortgage was harder.
1.3. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design and Materials
2.3. Procedures
2.4. EEG Recording and Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral Results
3.2. ERP Results
3.2.1. ERP Responses to the Critical Words (300–800 ms Time Window/P600)
3.2.2. ERP Responses to the Sentence-Final Word (300-800 ms Time Window/Negativity)
3.2.3. Correlation Analyses
4. Discussion
4.1. Effect of Temporal Connectives on Pragmatic Processing at the Critical Words
4.2. Pragmatic Incongruence Effect in Sentence-Final Position
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Region | Source | df | F | Significant | η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Midline | connective | 1, 27 | 0.971 | 0.333 | 0.035 |
congruency | 1, 27 | 1.199 | 0.283 | 0.043 | |
connective × congruency | 1, 27 | 5.073 | 0.033 * | 0.158 | |
connective × congruency × ACP | 2, 54 | 0.444 | 0.644 | 0.016 | |
Lateral | connective type | 1, 27 | 3.141 | 0.088 | 0.104 |
congruency | 1, 27 | 1.055 | 0.313 | 0.038 | |
connective × congruency | 1, 27 | 9.399 | 0.005 * | 0.258 | |
connective × congruency × ACP × H | 2, 54 | 0.810 | 0.418 | 0.029 |
Region | Source | df | F | Significant | η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Midline | connective | 1, 27 | 0.499 | 0.486 | 0.018 |
congruency | 1, 27 | 2.689 | 0.113 | 0.091 | |
connective × congruency | 1, 27 | 0.009 | 0.923 | 0.000 | |
connective × congruency × ACP | 2, 54 | 0.041 | 0.960 | 0.002 | |
Lateral | connective type | 1, 27 | 0.101 | 0.753 | 0.004 |
congruency | 1, 27 | 6.014 | 0.021 * | 0.182 | |
connective × congruency | 1, 27 | 0.520 | 0.477 | 0.019 | |
congruency × H | 1, 27 | 5.004 | 0.034 * | 0.156 | |
connective × congruency × ACP × H | 2, 54 | 2.47 | 0.108 | 0.084 |
References
- Boroditsky, L. Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition 2000, 75, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boroditsky, L.; Fuhrman, O.; McCormick, K. Do English and Mandarin speakers think differently about time? Cognition 2011, 118, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, K.E. Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cogn. Linguist. 2006, 17, 199–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Comrie, B. Tense; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1985; Volume 17. [Google Scholar]
- Wadley, S.; Li, C.N.; Thompson, S.A. Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. J. Am. Orient. Soc. 1987, 107, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slabakova, R. Acquiring Temporal Meanings without Tense Morphology: The Case of L2 Mandarin Chinese. Mod. Lang. J. 2015, 99, 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, C.S.; Erbaugh, M. Temporal information in sentences of Mandarin. In New Views in Chinese Syntactic Research—International Symposium on Chinese Grammar for the New Millennium; Xu, L., Shao, J., Eds.; Zhejiang Education Publishing House: Hangzhou, China, 2001; pp. 514–542. [Google Scholar]
- Qin, H.; Kong, L. On semantic underspecification in English and Mandarin ‘BEFORE/AFTER’+NEC constructions. Lingua 2018, 206, 49–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Ruiter, L.E.; Theakston, A.L.; Brandt, S.; Lieven, E.V. Iconicity affects children’s comprehension of complex sentences: The role of semantics, clause order, input and individual differences. Cognition 2018, 171, 202–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, E.V. On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1971, 10, 266–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mandler, J.M. On the comprehension of temporal order. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1986, 1, 309–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Münte, T.F.; Schiltz, K.; Kutas, M. When temporal terms belie conceptual order. Nature 1998, 395, 71–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nieuwland, M.S. The Truth Before and After: Brain Potentials Reveal Automatic Activation of Event Knowledge during Sentence Comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 27, 2215–2228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Politzer-Ahles, S.; Xiang, M.; Almeida, D.A.D.A.E. “Before” and “after”: Investigating the relationship between temporal connectives and chronological ordering using event-related potentials. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0175199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rinck, M.; Hähnel, A.; Becker, G. Using temporal information to construct, update, and retrieve situation models of narratives. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2001, 27, 67–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ye, Z.; Kutas, M.; George, M.S.; Sereno, M.I.; Ling, F.; Münte, T.F. Rearranging the world: Neural network supporting the processing of temporal connectives. NeuroImage 2011, 59, 3662–3667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Perniss, P.; Thompson, R.L.; Vigliocco, G. Iconicity as a General Property of Language: Evidence from Spoken and Signed Languages. Front. Psychol. 2010, 1, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Amidon, A.; Carey, P. Why five-year-olds cannot understand before and after. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1972, 11, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Natsopoulos, D.; Grouios, G.; Bostantzopoulou, S.; Mentenopoulos, G.; Katsarou, Z.; Logothetis, J. Algorithmic and heuristic strategies in comprehension of complement clauses by patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 1993, 31, 951–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Natsopoulos, D.; Mentenopoulos, G.; Bostantzopoulou, S.; Katsarou, Z.; Grouios, G.; Logothetis, J. Understanding of relational time terms before and after in Parkinsonian patients. Brain Lang. 1991, 40, 444–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leech, G.N. Towards a Semantic Description of English. In Indiana Studies in the History and Theory of Linguistics; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, T.E. (Ed.) Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, A.W.; White, S.H. The Development of Spatial Representations of Large-Scale Environments. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 1975, 10, 9–55. [Google Scholar]
- Blything, L.P.; Cain, K. Children’s processing and comprehension of complex sentences containing temporal connectives: The influence of memory on the time course of accurate responses. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 1517–1529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blything, L.P.; Davies, R.A.I.; Cain, K. Young Children’s Comprehension of Temporal Relations in Complex Sentences: The Influence of Memory on Performance. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 1922–1934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- De Ruiter, L.E.; Theakston, A.L.; Brandt, S.; Lieven, E.V.M. The relationship between parental input and children’s spontaneous use of adverbial clauses con- taining after, before, because, and if. In Proceedings of the 14th International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Lyon, France, 17–21 July 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort, P.; Hald, L.; Bastiaansen, M.; Petersson, K.M. Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language Comprehension. Science 2004, 304, 438–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hald, L.A.; Steenbeek-Planting, E.G.; Hagoort, P. The interaction of discourse context and world knowledge in online sentence comprehension. Evidence from the N400. Brain Res. 2007, 1146, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jiang, X.; Li, Y.; Zhou, X. Even a rich man can afford that expensive house: ERP responses to construction-based pragmatic constraints during sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 1857–1866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Romero-Rivas, C.; Corey, J.D.; Garcia, X.; Thierry, G.; Martin, C.D.; Costa, A. World knowledge and novel information integration during L2 speech comprehension. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2016, 20, 576–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xu, X.; Jiang, X.; Zhou, X. When a causal assumption is not satisfied by reality: Differential brain responses to concessive and causal relations during sentence comprehension. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 30, 704–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, J. Crosslinguistic influence on L2 implicature computation for determiners. J. Second. Lang. Stud. 2022, 5, 86–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiang, M.; Hanink, E.; Vegh, G. Before and after—processing presuppositions in discourse. In Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 27–29 August 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Reichenbach, H. Elements of Symbolic Logic; Macmillan & Co.: New York, NY, USA, 1947. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Y.; Jones, M.; Thierry, G. Timeline blurring in fluent Chinese-English bilinguals. Brain Res. 2018, 1701, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lin, T.H. Tense in a Language without Morphological Tense; National Tsing Hua University: Beijing, China, 2003; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, P. On the triplex structure of the temporal system in modern Chinese. Zhongguo Yuwen 1988, 5, 401–422. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, W.L. “Cloze Procedure”: A New Tool for Measuring Readability. Journal. Q. 1953, 30, 415–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, X.; Zhou, X. Topic shift impairs pronoun resolution during sentence comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 2015, 53, 129–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xu, X.; Chen, Q.; Panther, K.-U.; Wu, Y. Influence of Concessive and Causal Conjunctions on Pragmatic Processing: Online Measures from Eye Movements and Self-Paced Reading. Discourse Process. 2017, 55, 387–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daneman, M.; Carpenter, P.A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980, 19, 450–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kerz, E.; Wiechmann, D. The role of verbal working memory in L2 sentence comprehension. J. Second Lang. Stud. 2020, 3, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Y.; Yang, X.; Yang, Y. Importance conveyed in different ways: Effects of hierarchy and focus. J. Neurolinguist. 2018, 47, 37–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, W.; Wang, L.; Li, X.; Yang, Y. Closure Positive Shifts Evoked by Different Prosodic Boundaries in Chinese Sentences. In Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2008; pp. 505–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulakova, E.; Freunberger, D.; Roehm, D. Marking the counterfactual: ERP evidence for pragmatic processing of German subjunctives. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Greenhouse, S.W.; Geisser, S. On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 1959, 24, 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiang, M.; Kuperberg, G. Reversing expectations during discourse comprehension. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 30, 648–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osterhout, L.; Holcomb, P.J. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. J. Mem. Lang. 1992, 31, 785–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Herten, M.; Kolk, H.H.J.; Chwilla, D.J. An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 22, 241–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baggio, G. Two ERP studies on Dutch temporal semantics. Master’s Thesis, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004. Available online: http://www.illc.uva.nl/Publications/ResearchReports/MoL-2004-04.text.pdf (accessed on 2 April 2022).
- Ferguson, H.J. Eye movements reveal rapid concurrent access to factual and counterfactual interpretations of the world. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2012, 65, 939–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xu, X.; Pan, M.; Dai, H.; Zhang, H.; Lu, Y. How referential uncertainty is modulated by conjunctions: ERP evidence from advanced Chinese–English L2 learners and English L1 speakers. Second Lang. Res. 2018, 35, 195–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quante, L.; Bölte, J.; Zwitserlood, P. Dissociating predictability, plausibility and possibility of sentence continuations in reading: Evidence from late-positivity ERPs. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dai, H.; Chen, L.; Ni, C.; Xu, X. Literature reading modulates pronoun resolution in counterfactual world: Evidence from event-related potentials. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2019, 45, 904–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tanner, D.; Goldshtein, M.; Weissman, B. Individual differences in the real-time neural dynamics of language comprehension. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Current Topics in Language; Federmeier, K.D., Watson, D.G., Eds.; Elsevier Academic Press: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; pp. 299–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, L.; Xu, X.; Chen, Q.; Royle, P. Can pragmatic inference benefit from topic prominence? ERP evidence from Mandarin Chinese. J. Neurolinguist. 2018, 46, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baggio, G.; van Lambalgen, M.; Hagoort, P. Computing and recomputing discourse models: An ERP study. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 36–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paczynski, M.; Jackendoff, R.; Kuperberg, G. When Events Change Their Nature: The Neurocognitive Mechanisms Underlying Aspectual Coercion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 1905–1917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wittenberg, E.; Paczynski, M.; Wiese, H.; Jackendoff, R.; Kuperberg, G. The difference between “giving a rose” and “giving a kiss”: Sustained neural activity to the light verb construction. J. Mem. Lang. 2014, 73, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Semantic Accounts | |||
---|---|---|---|
Veridicality | Iconicity | Polarity | |
p before q | non-veridical | iconic | positive |
q after p | veridical | non-iconic | negative |
Before q, p. | non-veridical | non-iconic | positive |
After p, q. | veridical | iconic | negative |
q zhiqian p | non-veridical | non-iconic | positive |
p zhihou q | veridical | iconic | negative |
Conditions | Examples |
---|---|
after- congruent | (4a) 爷爷/去/城里/之后/去了/乡下,因为/那里/空气/清新/如洗。 Yeye/qu/chengli/zhihou/qule/xiangxia/,/yinwei/nali/kongqi/qingxin/ruxi. Grandpa/go-to/city/after/went/countryside, because/there/air/fresh/as washing. After going to the city, Grandpa went to the countryside because the air there was fresh and pure. |
after- incongruent | (4b) 爷爷/去/乡下/之后/去了/城里,因为/那里/空气/清新/如洗。 Yeye/qu/xiangxia/zhihou/qule/chengli/,/yinwei/nali/kongqi/qingxin/ruxi. Grandpa/go-to/countryside/after/went/city, because/there/air/fresh/as washing. After going to the countryside, Grandpa went to the city because the air there was fresh and pure. |
before- congruent | (4c) 爷爷/去/城里/之前/去了/乡下,因为/那里/空气/清新/如洗。 Yeye/qu/chengli/zhiqian/qule/xiangxia/,/yinwei/nali/kongqi/qingxin/ruxi. Grandpa/go-to/city/before/went/countryside, because/there/air/fresh/as washing. Before going to the city, Grandpa went to the countryside because the air there was fresh and pure. |
before- incongruent | (4d) 爷爷/去/乡下/之前/去了/城里,因为/那里/空气/清新/如洗。 Yeye/qu/xiangxia/zhiqian/qule/chengli/,/yinwei/nali/kongqi/qingxin/ruxi. Grandpa/go-to/countryside/before/went/city, because/there/air/fresh/as washing. Before going to the countryside, Grandpa went to the city because the air there was fresh and pure. |
Cloze Probability | Acceptability | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
after-congruent | 0.487 | 0.26 | 5.35 | 0.71 |
after-incongruent | 0.131 | 0.19 | 2.28 | 0.74 |
before-congruent | 0.497 | 0.27 | 5.13 | 0.83 |
before-incongruent | 0.123 | 0.01 | 2.27 | 0.73 |
Truncated Context | Whole Context | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
after-congruent | 85.7 | 0.18 | 87.0 | 0.09 |
after-incongruent | 83.2 | 0.17 | 36.2 | 0.14 |
before-congruent | 90.5 | 0.12 | 97.4 | 0.10 |
before-incongruent | 81.7 | 0.15 | 34.9 | 0.14 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chen, L.; Lu, Y.; Xu, X. Understanding Temporal Relations in Mandarin Chinese: An ERP Investigation. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 474. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040474
Chen L, Lu Y, Xu X. Understanding Temporal Relations in Mandarin Chinese: An ERP Investigation. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(4):474. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040474
Chicago/Turabian StyleChen, Lijuan, Yiyi Lu, and Xiaodong Xu. 2022. "Understanding Temporal Relations in Mandarin Chinese: An ERP Investigation" Brain Sciences 12, no. 4: 474. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040474
APA StyleChen, L., Lu, Y., & Xu, X. (2022). Understanding Temporal Relations in Mandarin Chinese: An ERP Investigation. Brain Sciences, 12(4), 474. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040474