Next Article in Journal
Neural Substrates for Early Data Reduction in Fast Vision: A Psychophysical Investigation
Previous Article in Journal
Understanding the Effect of Listening to Music, Playing Music, and Singing on Brain Function: A Scoping Review of fNIRS Studies
Previous Article in Special Issue
Multiple Types of Developmental Dyslexias in a Shallow Orthography: Principles for Diagnostic Screening in Italian
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Neural Dynamics of Processing Inflectional Morphology: An fMRI Study on Korean Inflected Verbs

1
Department of Psychology, Korea University, Seoul 02841, Republic of Korea
2
Department of Psychology, Chonnam National University, Gwangju 61186, Republic of Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(8), 752; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080752
Submission received: 26 June 2024 / Revised: 19 July 2024 / Accepted: 23 July 2024 / Published: 26 July 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Neuropsychology of Reading)

Abstract

:
The present study aimed to elucidate the neural mechanisms underpinning the visual recognition of morphologically complex verbs in Korean, a morphologically rich, agglutinative language with inherent polymorphemic characteristics. In an fMRI experiment with a lexical decision paradigm, we investigated whether verb inflection types (base, regular, and irregular) are processed through separate mechanisms or a single system. Furthermore, we explored the semantic influence in processing inflectional morphology by manipulating the semantic ambiguity (homonymous vs. unambiguous) of inflected verbs. The results showed equivalent activation levels in the left inferior frontal gyrus for both regular and irregular verbs, challenging the dichotomy between the two. Graded effects of verb regularity were observed in the occipitotemporal regions, with regular inflections eliciting increased activation in the fusiform and lingual gyri. In the middle occipital gyrus, homonyms showed decreased activation relative to that of unambiguous words, specifically for base and irregular forms. Furthermore, the angular gyrus exhibited significant modulation with all verb types, indicating a semantic influence during morphological processing. These findings support single-system theories and the connectionist framework, challenging the assumptions of purely orthographic morphological decomposition and dual-mechanism accounts. Furthermore, they provide evidence for a semantic influence during morphological processing, with differential reliance on semantic activation for regular and irregular inflections.

1. Introduction

The issue of how various types of inflection, such as regular (e.g., work—worked) and irregular (e.g., catch—caught) forms, are represented and processed in the brain remains a central topic in psycholinguistics. Classical full-listing models argue that all morphologically complex words are stored in their entirety, regardless of regularity [1]. On the other hand, full-parsing models propose that these forms undergo an obligatory decomposition into their morphemic units, with only morphemes being stored [2,3]. Bridging these two opposing views, dual-mechanism accounts argue that there are categorical differences between regular and irregular forms, suggesting they are processed by distinct mechanisms [4,5,6]. Regular forms, which follow predictable patterns of inflection (e.g., adding ‘–ed’ to form the past tense), are processed through a rule-based system, while irregular forms are stored and processed in the mental lexicon through lexical-semantic memory. In contrast, single-system accounts challenge this dichotomy, suggesting continuous rather than categorical distinctions between regular and irregular inflections [7,8,9]. These theories, based on earlier connectionist models [10,11], advocate for a single associative system that maps orthographic or phonological forms to semantic meanings, leading to graded effects in inflectional morphology.
There has been a heavy focus on the debate surrounding the past tense in English in the previous literature, despite the cross-linguistic differences between writing systems, such as English and Korean. Furthermore, inconsistent findings may derive from these differences, especially in the morphological architecture of languages [12]. Thus, this suggests that the investigation of the processing of morphologically rich languages is crucial for a comprehensive understanding of linguistic structures and cognitive processing across different language systems. As an agglutinative language, Korean is characterized by its highly inflective and polymorphemic nature in which a verb stem is conjugated with a variety of endings to denote variances in tense, mood, and levels of politeness. For example, the stem of the verb 먹다 (to eat) can be conjugated with various endings such as –습니다 and –었다 to denote the formal polite present tense 먹습니다 (formed by adding –습니다 to –) or the casual past tense먹었다 (formed by adding –었다 to –). Furthermore, irregular verbs in English, while even more frequently used than regular verbs [13,14], follow distinct patterns of vowel and consonant changes or complete suppletion, such as “go—went—gone” and “be—was/were—been” [15]. However, Korean features a highly systematic inflectional morphology in which the majority of verbs are inflected without alternations to the verb stem (e.g., –) and thus are categorized as regular verbs. Only a subset of ‘irregular’ verbs have accompanying changes in their stems based on consistent and predictable phonological rules [16], such as dropping certain consonants or adding specific vowels. For example, in irregular verbs, contained in the verb stem is transformed into a consonant or when conjugated with endings starting with the vowels or , such as in 도와 (formed by adding – to–; to help and).
These unique morphological characteristics make the Korean writing system suitable for testing the existing models of morphological processing (Figure 1). At first glance, the full-parsing models may seem plausible for explaining the processing of Korean inflectional morphology, considering its extensive verb conjugation and systematic inflection patterns. However, a potential semantic influence might emerge during the visual recognition of morphologically complex words, which would contradict a purely orthography-based morphological decomposition in a ‘blind-to-semantics’ manner, as has been assumed in these models. Meanwhile, dual-mechanism accounts of inflectional morphology, distinguishing between rule-based regular forms and memory-based irregular forms, may need to be adapted for the Korean writing system, in which even irregular forms follow predictable phonological rules. On the other hand, single-system accounts, which suggest a continuous mapping of forms to meanings, may find support in the consistent and systematic Korean inflectional morphology. In this regard, the present study employed inherently polymorphemic Korean verbs, aiming to investigate if an inflected form elicits a processing cost and whether inflected verbs undergo memory-based retrieval from the mental lexicon and/or are processed via the application of automatic grammar rules.
In the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) domain, recent studies have investigated brain regions involved in processing morphologically complex words. Initial fMRI research focused on identifying brain regions specifically tuned to morphological processing [18,19]. Devlin et al. [19] observed reduced activation in temporal and parietal regions, such as the bilateral angular gyrus (AG), left occipitotemporal cortex, and left middle temporal gyrus (MTG) for morphologically related word pairs compared to unrelated pairs, suggesting that morphology results from the convergence of form and meaning. Similarly, in an fMRI study using a masked priming paradigm, Gold and Rastle [20] reported the involvement of occipital regions, including the fusiform gyrus (FG) and middle occipital gyrus (MOG), in both morphological and orthographic relationships, with semantic conditions reducing activation in the MTG, indicating the structural nature of early morphological decomposition. The debate continues about whether morphologically complex words are processed as whole units or decomposed into morphemes. Some neuroimaging evidence supports whole-word processing [19,21], while other evidence favors morpheme decomposition [22]. Whole-word processing highlights lexical-semantic effects in widespread bilateral frontotemporal regions, including the MTG, superior temporal gyrus (STG), and inferior frontal gyrus pars orbitalis (IFG, BA 47) [21,23]. Conversely, decomposition emphasizes the role of the posterior left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG, BA 44/45) [22].
The LIFG has been consistently implicated in the literature as a core region for processing inflectional morphology, particularly the past tense in English, demonstrating distinct activation patterns for regular and irregular inflections. Both types show increased activity in temporal and hippocampal regions, including the FG, MTG, and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). Regular verbs, however, exhibit greater activation in the LIFG, along with additional regions, such as the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), basal ganglia, and cerebellum [24,25,26,27]. This binary distinction aligns with the dual-route theory, positing two memory systems: the procedural memory system for regular inflections and the declarative memory system for irregular ones [6,17,28]. Neuroimaging studies corroborate this theory, showing selective activation for regular inflections in the procedural memory network, characterized by the left-lateralized frontotemporal network, including the LIFG, basal ganglia, and cerebellum [29,30,31].
However, some researchers provide evidence for a single-system theory, which suggests continuous differences in inflection processing [7,32]. Joanisse and Seidenberg [7] found greater activation for regular compared to irregular verbs in the bilateral IFG during a past-tense generation task. They observed that phonologically similar irregular verbs elicited similar activation to regular verbs, while irregular verbs with no phonological similarity showed increased activity. Desai et al. [32] also reported no additional activation for regular inflected verbs compared to irregular verbs when the levels of phonological complexity were matched. These findings suggest that inflectional morphology is influenced by phonological, semantic, and probabilistic factors rather than a binary rule-based system.
Despite the heavy focus on the past tense in English, some neuroimaging studies have also investigated the neural correlates of inflectional morphology employing morphologically rich languages like Finnish, Japanese, and Korean. Finnish studies showed that inflected nouns elicited an increased activation in the LIFG (BA 47), STG (BA 22), MTG (BA 21), and AG (BA 39), indicating that morphological processing involves suffix stripping at the semantic/syntactic level rather than the visual word form level [33,34]. Japanese studies indicated greater activation in the left MTG (BA 21) for verbs compared to nouns, with selective activation for inflected verbs in the LIFG (BA 44/45) [35]. The only fMRI study on Korean inflected verbs to the best of our knowledge was that by Yim et al. [36], which found similar activation patterns for regular and irregular verbs in temporal regions, including the left MTG, MFG, STG, and PHG, suggesting that Korean relies on memory and meaning irrespective of verb regularity, supporting the single-system theory.
In sum, the previous fMRI literature has highlighted the distinct neural correlates involved in processing morphologically complex words, identifying widespread activation across occipital, temporal, and frontal regions [19,20,21,22,30,37]. Notably, the left frontotemporal regions, particularly the LIFG, have been proposed as the core network (procedural memory network) for the rule-based computation of regular verbs. In contrast, temporal-hippocampal regions are implicated as the declarative memory network for processing irregular verbs, which relies on the retrieval of semantic information, similar to whole-word processing [26,27,29]. However, some researchers have reported no significant difference between regular and irregular verbs after controlling for phonological complexity [7,32] or found equivalent activation for both inflection types in the temporal regions [36]. These findings suggest that verb regularity effects may be gradually modulated by the convergence of orthographic, semantic, and phonological information rather than by categorical differences among inflection types.
With respect to semantic processing, the LIFG has also been highlighted for its role of top-down control over activating and selecting meanings of homonyms [38,39,40]. Other relevant regions include the MTG, AG, and supramarginal gyrus (SMG), associated with accessing and encoding lexical-semantic representations [39,41,42,43,44,45]. For instance, Hoffman and Tamm [38] found that the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) play roles in semantic control and representation, respectively. Nevertheless, it remains uncertain whether semantic information is engaged in the processing of inflected verbs, and if so, whether it is processed by a distinct mechanism depending on their regularity.
In the present study, using a rapid event-related fMRI design with a lexical decision task (LDT), we aimed to investigate if different inflection types in Korean verbs show distinct brain activation patterns and how semantic information affects morphological processing. To address the question of whether regular and irregular verbs are processed categorically or continuously, the present study manipulated three inflection types: base, regular, and irregular verbs. This allows us to tackle the issues regarding not only whether inflected forms incur a processing cost compared to uninflected forms [46,47], but also whether the base form (i.e., uninflected form) and the two inflected forms (i.e., regular and irregular inflections) exhibit graded effects or if only the two inflected forms display distinctive differences. Furthermore, the present study examined the interaction between form and meaning by orthogonally manipulating the semantic ambiguity of words, comparing homonymous words with those possessing a single meaning within each type of inflection [46,48].
We hypothesized that the memory-based retrieval of inflected forms would activate widespread bilateral frontotemporal regions, including the MTG, STG, and anterior IFG (BA 47). In contrast, rule-based decomposition was expected to activate the posterior LIFG (BA 44/45). If regular and irregular inflections showed categorical differences, regular inflections would increase left frontotemporal activation, especially in the posterior LIFG. If graded effects were assumed, both inflections would increase activation in the left frontotemporal network, with regular verbs showing the most pronounced activation. Semantic effects were expected to modulate activation in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL), including the AG and SMG.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty-four healthy native Korean speakers participated in the experiment (12 female; 23.8 ± 2.5 years, M ± SD). All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were rated as right-handed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [49]. They signed their written informed consent and were compensated with payment for their participation. Two participants’ data were excluded from the further analyses due to their poor behavioral performance, showing accuracy below 70%. The remaining twenty-two participants (11 female) had a mean age of 23.77 ± 2.62 years.

2.2. Materials

The experimental stimuli set comprised 240 items, including 120 Korean words and 120 pseudowords, selected from Korean Sejong Corpus [50], which has 15 million words. All word stimuli were verbs with 2–3 syllables, as confirmed by the Standard Korean dictionary. Only pure Korean words were utilized to avoid potential confounding effects of Sino-Korean words (i.e., Chinese-derived words; see [51]). The stimuli set used in the experiment is shown in Appendix A, in which Table A1 and Table A2 represent words and pseudowords, respectively.
The words were categorized into three inflection types, including uninflected base form (n = 40), regularly inflected form (n = 40), and irregularly inflected form (n = 40). As suggested in the previous literature on Korean inflectional morphology [52,53], base forms consisted of a verb stem and a basic ending suffix (–), as in 씻다 (to wash) with the stem – and suffix –. Regular inflections were formed by the simple addition of a suffix to the verb stem (e.g., 먹어—“to eat and” with the stem and the suffix ), while irregular inflections involved a transformation of the stem when a suffix was used, as in 도와 (“to help and”) with the stem – transformed to 도와 when the suffix– was used.
Semantic ambiguity was manipulated by comparing verbs with single meanings (i.e., control; n = 60) and those with multiple unrelated meanings (i.e., homonyms; n = 60). The classification was based on objective measures from the given corpus and the Standard Korean dictionary. Subjective measures were also utilized, in which three sets of subjective ratings were conducted on the experimental stimuli: familiarity, the number of meanings (NOM), and the relatedness of meanings (ROM). Twenty volunteers (all native Korean speakers, 13 female; mean age 23.1 years, SD = 3.7). For familiarity rating, participants rated the subjective familiarity and frequency of use on a 7-point Likert scale.
The subjective ratings of the NOM and ROM were carried out following the procedure of Azuma [54]. For the NOM rating task, they indicated whether a presented stimulus had no meaning (0), a single meaning (1), or multiple meanings (2). Stimuli were presented in their base form (e.g., stem suffixed with the basic ending suffix ‘–’), including 60 control stems, 60 homonymous stems, and 60 pseudostems. The base form of pseudostems was created by randomly selecting syllables from real verbs. NOM ratings showed that homonymous stems had significantly more meanings compared to control stems (homonym: M = 1.43, SD = 0.43; control: M = 1.18, SD = 0.17; t(59) = −6.40, p < 0.001). For the ROM rating task, participants judged the degree of relatedness of meanings on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (unrelated meanings) to 7 (same meaning). A base form of the stem and exemplar sentences of its meaning pair were presented. For stems with more than two meanings, the most frequently used meaning was paired with another meaning in multiple trials, and the rating score was averaged over all pairwise comparisons. The ROM rating set included 60 homonymous stems and 10 polysemous stems as a filler condition. ROM ratings confirmed that homonymous stems had relatively unrelated meanings (M = 1.89, SD = 1.50), compared to the highly related meanings of polysemous stems (M = 5.35, SD = 1.62).
Moreover, the meaning dominance of homonyms was also assessed through additional subjective ratings. Here, twenty native Korean speakers (16 female; mean age 27.7 years, SD = 4.59) who did not participate in the main experiment or the three sets of subjective ratings were asked to judge the subjective familiarity of each meaning of the homonyms on a 7-point Likert scale. The results demonstrated no significant difference among conditions in the meaning dominance, showing the mean familiarity ratings for dominant and subordinate meanings: base form homonyms scored 6.12 (SD = 0.53) for dominant meanings and 4.54 (SD = 1.05) for subordinate meanings; regular form homonyms scored 5.95 (SD = 0.52) for dominant meanings and 4.25 (SD = 1.07) for subordinate meanings; and irregular form homonyms scored 5.98 (SD = 0.46) for dominant meanings and 4.11 (SD = 0.94) for subordinate meanings.
All frequency measures were log-transformed, per million occurrences in the given corpus. The mean log-transformed word frequency per million for each condition was as follows: base control, 1.16 (SD = 0.51); base homonym, 0.9 (SD = 0.52); regular control, 1.12 (SD = 0.57); regular homonym, 1.11 (SD = 0.5); irregular control, 1.29 (SD = 0.7); and irregular homonym, 1.14 (SD = 0.52). The mean log-transformed cumulative stem frequency per million for each condition was as follows: base control, 1.95 (SD = 0.45); base homonym, 1.78 (SD = 0.56); regular control, 1.67 (SD = 0.73); regular homonym, 2.33 (SD = 0.61); irregular control, 1.71 (SD = 0.37); and irregular homonym, 1.63 (SD = 0.71). Other sublexical and lexical variables known to affect behavioral and brain responses during visual word recognition, including word frequency [55], stem length [56,57], stem frequency [58], neighborhood density (i.e., type frequency) [59,60], cumulative frequency (i.e., token frequency) of a first syllable [60,61], and familiarity [62,63] were also statistically matched between experimental conditions (all ps > 0.05; see Supplementary Table S1).
Finally, 120 pronounceable pseudowords were created for the filler word condition by randomly selecting and concatenating verb stems and suffixes used in the word stimuli set. The length of stimuli of each condition, including pseudowords, was matched so that half of the stimuli had 2 syllables, and the other half had 3 syllables.

2.3. Procedure

A rapid event-related fMRI design was employed for the lexical decision task (Figure 2). Each trial commenced with a fixation point (+) displayed for 100 ms, followed by the presentation of a target stimulus for 1000 ms. Participants responded using their index and middle fingers to indicate whether the stimulus was a word or nonword, respectively. The responding hand was counterbalanced across participants: half (n = 12) used their right hand, while the other half (n = 12) used their left hand. Following each trial, a blank screen was presented during the inter-trial interval (ITI), which varied randomly in duration from 1000 ms to 7000 ms.
All stimuli were presented in white font (Courier New, size 28) on a black background, arranged in a pseudo-random order. The duration of the jittered intervals and the sequence of target stimuli were optimized using optseq2 software [64], and the presentation was controlled with the E-prime 2.0 Professional program (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA). During the experimental session, stimuli were presented in 4 blocks, each consisting of 60 trials, with a one-minute break between blocks. Prior to the experimental session, the participants completed 30 practice trials. These included 5 baseline masks, 10 words, and 10 pseudowords not part of the experiment session. All participants achieved an accuracy rate above 80% in the practice session, ensuring their understanding of the procedure.

2.4. Image Acquisition

MRI scanning was performed using a Siemens Magnetom Trio 3T MRI scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at the Korea University Brain Imaging Center. Functional images were acquired using T2*-weighted gradient EPI (echo-planar imaging) sequences (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 20 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view = 240 mm; slice thickness = 3 mm; no gap for 42 slices; matrix size = 80 × 80; and voxel size = 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm). T1-weighted structural images were acquired with a 3D MP-RAGE (magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo) sequence (TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.52 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view = 256 ms; matrix size = 256 × 256; and voxel size = 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm) covering the whole head.

2.5. Behavioral Analysis

A linear mixed effects model (LMM) [65] was used for response time (RT) analysis with lme4 package [66] in R software (version 4.3.1 [67]). Only correct responses were included in the RT analysis. Accuracy data were analyzed with a general linear mixed effects model (GLMM) with a binomial distribution. Both the RT and accuracy analyses included the fixed factors of ambiguity (2: control vs. homonym) and morphology (3: base vs. regular vs. irregular) with random intercepts of both participant and item. Here, p-values were calculated using the likelihood ratio test (LRT). For the main effects, a model that included only random factors was compared with a model that incorporated each fixed factor individually. For interactions, p-values were calculated by comparing the full model to a model that excluded the interaction term. When any significant effect was found, a post-hoc paired comparison was carried out using estimated marginal means with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. R software [67] was used for the statistical analyses of behavioral and fMRI data.

2.6. fMRI Analysis

The fMRI data were processed and analyzed using the SPM12 toolbox (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK; “http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/ (accessed on 1 July 2023)”) alongside custom-built MATLAB scripts [68]. The first three images from each session were discarded to mitigate the transition effects of hemodynamic responses. The remaining functional images underwent realignment for motion correction, followed by slice timing correction to adjust for acquisition time differences between slices. The structural images were then co-registered to the mean functional images and segmented. Subsequently, the functional images were spatially normalized to a standard MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template, using the parameters obtained during segmentation. To reduce spatial noise, all images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a full-width half-maximum (FWHM) of 6 mm.

2.6.1. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using a general linear model (GLM) with a two-stage mixed effects approach. At the individual level, contrast images between the experimental conditions and baseline masks were generated, resulting in six contrasts: base control > mask, base homonym > mask, regular control > mask, regular homonym > mask, irregular control > mask, and irregular homonym > mask. BOLD signals were convolved with a standard hemodynamic response function (HRF). Only correct responses in the lexical decision task were included in the analysis. Movement parameters obtained during realignment were entered as regressors in the model specification. Additionally, response time (RT) for each condition and participant was included as a parametric modulator, following the method suggested by Taylor et al. [69]. At the group level, one-sample t-tests were conducted on the contrast images estimated at the individual level. A whole-brain statistical parametric map was constructed for each morphology and semantic ambiguity condition. The voxel-level statistical threshold was set at p < 0.001, and the cluster-level threshold was set at q < 0.05 with false discovery rate (FDR) correction, requiring a minimum cluster extent of 30 contiguous voxels (kE > 30).
To further investigate the activation patterns observed in the GLM results, follow-up analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed with the factors of morphology (3: base vs. regular vs. irregular) and semantic ambiguity (2: control vs. homonym) using beta estimates extracted from significant clusters. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were carried out if significant effects were found.

2.6.2. ROI Analysis

Region of interest (ROI) analyses focused on brain regions have previously been associated with morphological and semantic processing [25,70]. ROIs for morphological processing include the LIFG, specifically the pars triangularis (BA 44/45 [−46, 30, 14]) and pars orbitalis (BA 47 [−37, 31, −12]). Semantic processing regions include the bilateral AG (BA 39; left AG [−45, −61, 36]; right AG [44, −59, 39]) and the MTG (BA 40; left MTG [−56, −34, −2]; right MTG [56, −37, −2]). All ROIs were defined as spheres with a 5 mm radius centered at the corresponding MNI coordinates, selected from the Automatic Anatomical Labeling Atlas 3 (AAL3) [71].
Raw average ROI parameter estimates for each of the six experimental conditions contrasted to baseline masks (e.g., base control > mask) were extracted using the MarsBaR toolbox [72]. Separate 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for each ROI with the factors of morphology (base vs. regular vs. irregular) and ambiguity (control vs. homonym), followed by post-hoc pairwise t-tests. Additionally, one-sample t-tests were conducted to assess (de)activation of each inflection type relative to the non-linguistic baseline mask (e.g., regular > mask).

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Results

The participants’ behavioral performance on the lexical decision task is displayed in Table 1. Trials with reaction times exceeding three SDs (standard deviations) from the mean or with error rates above 30% were excluded from the analysis. This accounted for 9.17% of the responses to the word targets. Only correct responses were included in the RT analysis. The overall accuracy was 90.94 ± 28.72%, and the RT was 580 ± 117 ms on average.
A linear mixed effects model conducted on the RT data revealed the significant effect of morphology (χ2(2) = 19.81, p < 0.001). Specifically, the base form elicited faster responses compared to both the regularly and irregularly inflected forms. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons confirmed that the responses were significantly faster for the base form compared to the regular form (b = –4.19, SE = 8.67 p < 0.001) and the irregular form (b = –3.68, SE = 8.72 p < 0.001). There was no significant difference in RTs between the regular and irregular forms (b = 0.467, SE = 9.09, p > 0.1). No further significant effects were observed in the RT analysis.
An analysis of accuracy also demonstrated the significant effect of morphology (χ2(2) = 25.14, p < 0.001), consistent with the RT analysis. The participants responded more accurately to the base form compared to the regular form (b = 5.48, SE = 0.23, p < 0.001) and the irregular form (b = 3.47, SE = 0.24, p = 0.002). Additionally, the interaction between morphology and ambiguity showed a trend toward significance (χ2(2) = 5.7, p = 0.058). Subsequent post-hoc comparisons revealed a significant ambiguity advantage for the base form, in which the homonyms elicited more accurate responses compared to the control words (b = 2.192, p = 0.03).

3.2. GLM Results

The results of the two-staged mixed effect general linear model (GLM) analysis on the whole-brain activation are detailed in Table 2 and Table 3 and illustrated in Figure 3. A broad array of brain regions associated with language processing showed the activation for each type of morphology and semantic ambiguity condition compared to the non-linguistic baseline mask condition. These regions included the precentral gyrus, thalamus, supplementary motor area, lingual gyrus (LG), MOG, and FG.
Follow-up repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted on significant clusters identified in the whole-brain analysis, focusing on occipitotemporal regions including the left FG, lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The factors were morphology (three levels: base, regular, and irregular) and semantic ambiguity (two levels: control and homonym). When significant effects were detected, post-hoc pairwise t-tests with the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were performed, as illustrated in Figure 4.
The main effect of morphology was significant in the FG (F(2, 42) = 10.74, p < 0.001, η² = 0.09) and MOG (F(2, 42) = 14.85, p < 0.001, η² = 0.09) and marginally significant in the LG (F(2, 42) = 3.11, p = 0.055, η² = 0.03). Post-hoc pairwise t-tests revealed that the regular form elicited increased activation in the FG compared to the irregular form (t(44) = −4.26, p < 0.001). There was also a marginally significant increase in activation for the regular form compared to the base form (t(44) = −2.47, p = 0.052). In the MOG, there was a significant decrease in activation for irregular forms compared to both the base form (t(44) = 3.56, p = 0.003) and the regular form (t(44) = −4.58, p < 0.001). The LG showed a marginally significant increase in activation for the regular form compared to the irregular form (t(44) = −2.34, p = 0.071).
The main effect of semantic ambiguity was significant in all of the regions examined. In the FG (F(1, 21) = 21.17, p < 0.001, η² = 0.07), LG (F(1, 21) = 10.99, p = 0.003, η² = 0.03), and MOG (F(1, 21) = 33.37, p < 0.001, η² = 0.13), the homonyms showed reduced activation levels relative to those of control words. The differences were statistically significant with t(65) = 3.69, p < 0.001 for FG, t(65) = 3.00, p = 0.004 for LG and t(65) = 5.19, p < 0.001 for MOG.
The interaction between morphology and semantic ambiguity was significant in the LG (F(2, 42) = 6.04, p = 0.005, η² = 0.04), and marginally significant in the MOG (F(2, 42) = 2.47, p = 0.096, η² = 0.03). An ambiguity advantage, characterized by the increased activation for control words relative to homonyms, was observed in the MOG for both the base form (t(21) = 3.53, p = 0.002) and the irregular form (t(21) = 5.42, p < 0.001). In the LG, a significant increase in activation for the control words compared to the homonyms was found for the base form (t(21) = 3.81, p = 0.001). However, no significant differences between control and homonyms were found in the regular condition for any region (all ps > 0.1).

3.3. ROI Results

Region of interest (ROI) analyses were conducted on brain regions implicated in the previous fMRI literature [25,70], including the LIFG pars triangularis (LIFG tri) and pars orbitalis (LIFG orb), bilateral AG, and MTG. Figure 5 illustrates the ROIs that showed the significant effect of morphology in the repeated measures ANOVAs, which included the factors of morphology and ambiguity.
Significant effects of morphology were observed in the LIFG tri (F(2, 42) = 5.52, p = 0.007, η² = 0.031) and bilateral AG (left: F(2, 46) = 3.83, p = 0.03, η² = 0.03; right: F(2, 46) = 4.17, p = 0.02, η² = 0.031). The post-hoc paired t-tests indicated a significantly increased activation levels for regular verbs compared to those of base forms in the LIFG tri (t(43) = −2.95, p = 0.016). Conversely, the regular form showed decreased activation relative to that of the base form in the bilateral AG (left: t(43) = 2.91, p = 0.006; right: t(43) = 2.58, p = 0.04).
One-sample t-tests were performed to test for the activation of each inflection type relative to the baseline mask. The results revealed that processing inflected forms (regular and irregular) selectively increased activation in the LIFG tri (regular: t(43) = 3.52, p = 0.001; irregular: t(43) = 2.09, p = 0.04), with regular forms also showing increased activation in the LIFG orb (t(43) = 2.45, p = 0.02). In contrast, processing verbs, regardless of inflection type, was associated with increased deactivation in the left AG (base: t(43) = −2.12, p = 0.04; regular: t(43) = −4.89, p < 0.001; irregular: t(43) = −2.55, p = 0.014) and right AG (base: t(43) = −2.05, p = 0.046; regular: t(43) = −5.12, p < 0.001; irregular: t(43) = −3.56, p = 0.001). A similar pattern of deactivation was also found in the right MTG for all types of verbs (base: t(43) = −2.98, p = 0.004; regular: t(43) = −2.29, p = 0.027; irregular: t(43) = −3.07, p = 0.003).
As illustrated in Figure 6, the repeated measures ANOVAs revealed the significant effect of semantic ambiguity in the right MTG (F(1, 21) = 5.84, p = 0.025, η² = 0.014). Specifically, homonyms showed a marginally significant increase in deactivation relative to the control words (t(65) = 1.91, p = 0.061). No other regions exhibited significant differences in response to semantic ambiguity. A further analysis using one-sample t-tests indicated that homonyms showed a significant deactivation compared to that of the baseline mask in the bilateral AG (left: t(65) = −3.49, p < 0.001; right: t(65) = −4.09, p < 0.001) and the right MTG (t(65) = −4.25, p < 0.001). Similarly, the control words also showed significant deactivation in the same regions (left AG: t(65) = −4.16, p < 0.001; right AG: t(65) = −4.5, p < 0.001; right MTG: t(65) = −2.42, p = 0.018). Importantly, the LIFG displayed the opposite pattern of activation. Both the homonymous and control words elicited increased activation in the LIFG pars triangularis (LIFG tri) (control: t(65) = 3.09, p = 0.002; homonym: t(65) = 2.11, p = 0.039).

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the spatial localization of morphological processing using a rapid event-related fMRI design combined with a lexical decision task. The experiment explored how morphological inflection (base, regular, and irregular) and semantic ambiguity (control vs. homonymous) influence both behavioral performance and brain activation patterns during the visual recognition of morphologically complex Korean verbs. It was hypothesized that verb regularity and inflectional cost would be reflected in differential activation in the left frontotemporal regions, particularly the LIFG, with regularly inflected verbs showing increased activity relative to that of base or irregular forms. An interaction between form and meaning was also anticipated, with regularly inflected homonyms expected to show increased activation relative to that of control words in temporal regions, while base and irregular forms were expected to elicit the reverse pattern.
The behavioral results revealed a significant inflectional cost in terms of reaction times and accuracy, indicating that the participants responded faster and more accurately to base forms compared to both regular and irregular inflected forms. Additionally, a significant interaction was observed between morphology and ambiguity. The base forms exhibited an ambiguity advantage, whereas the regular forms displayed a trend towards an ambiguity disadvantage. These findings align with previous research on inflected words in morphologically rich languages, suggesting that uninflected forms are processed more efficiently due to their direct access to semantic representations, while inflected forms require additional processing effort, leading to an increased processing time and error rates [33,73]. Furthermore, the significant interaction between form and meaning suggests that the processing of homonymous words can be facilitated or impeded depending on their morphological form, potentially due to the interplay between morphological parsing and access to semantic representations [74].
The whole-brain fMRI results revealed activations predominantly in occipitotemporal regions associated with morphology and semantics, including the FG, LG, and MOG. A significant modulation of activation by inflection type was observed, with regular forms showing increased activation in the FG and LG compared to that of the base and irregular forms. In contrast, irregular forms exhibited significantly reduced activation in the MOG relative to that of the base and regular forms. These findings are consistent with previous studies implicating the occipitotemporal cortex in visual word form processing and morphological analysis [20,75].
The occipitotemporal cortex, especially the visual word form area (VWFA) located within the left FG, has been consistently associated with orthographic processing during the visual recognition of words [20,76,77,78] and thus is often suggested as evidence for a morphological decomposition driven by orthography in a blind-to-semantics manner. For instance, in a masked priming fMRI experiment, Gold and Rastle [14] found an overlapping activation in occipitotemporal regions, including the left fusiform and lingual gyri for pseudo-morphological (e.g., corner—CORN) and orthographic (e.g., brothel—BROTH) conditions, but not for the semantic (bucket—PAIL) condition. Related to this and more importantly, they found a distinctive selective activation of the LG (BA 19) for morphological word pairs compared to that of unrelated pairs, suggesting early morphological decomposition is purely driven by orthography. Indeed, the current data demonstrate the contribution of the FG, LG, and MOG to morphological effects, though with regular forms showing increased activation compared to that of base or irregular forms, challenging the notion of obligatory segmentation processes for all morphologically complex words [2,79].
Furthermore, semantic effects were observed in occipitotemporal regions, with homonyms showing decreased activation relative to that of unambiguous words. A form-with-meaning interaction was found, in which decreased MOG activation for homonyms relative to unambiguous words was evident for base and irregular forms, but not for regular forms. This pattern is consistent with behavioral findings showing a reversal of the semantic ambiguity effect for base and regular forms. The FG’s role in lexical-semantic processing, as suggested by the previous literature [75,80], fits well with the current data, which demonstrate both morphological and semantic effects, indicating the occipitotemporal region’s role in interfacing form and meaning in morphologically complex Korean verbs.
Our ROI analyses, based on the previous fMRI literature [25,70], further elucidate the neural substrates underlying the processing of different morphological and semantic properties of verbs. The analyses focused on the LIFG pars triangularis and pars orbitalis, bilateral AG, and MTG. A significant main effect of morphology was observed in the LIFG tri and bilateral AG. Regular forms showed increased activation in the LIFG tri compared to base forms, while the regular form showed decreased activation relative to the base form in the bilateral AG. The one-sample t-tests indicated that processing inflected forms (regular and irregular) selectively increased LIFG activation in the LIFG tri, with regular forms also showing increased activation in the LIFG orb. In contrast, all verb types, regardless of inflection, were associated with increased deactivation in the bilateral AG and right MTG.
In the dual-route model of inflectional morphology [6,17,28], the declarative memory network involving the temporal-hippocampal regions is expected to modulate both types of inflection. In contrast, the rule-based processing of regularly inflected verbs is associated with distinctive activation in the procedural memory network, which includes the LIFG, cerebellum, and basal ganglia. According to this model, regular inflections should uniquely engage the LIFG and related structures, while both regular and irregular inflections should activate the temporal-hippocampal network. We indeed observed a significant reduction in AG activation for all verb types compared to the baseline mask, with the regular inflections showing reduced activity relative to that of base forms. Given the functional association of AG and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) with semantic processing [70,81], these results suggest that the processing of base form verbs benefited more from memory-based retrieval processes relative to regularly inflected verbs and that these processes are involved in processing Korean verbs, regardless of whether they are inflected, and if so, whether they are regularly or irregularly inflected.
Contrary to the expectation of the dual-route model, the posterior division of the LIFG (i.e., LIFG pars triangularis, BA 44) showed selective activation for both inflected forms but not for uninflected base forms. Importantly, regular and irregular verbs did not differ significantly in LIFG activation, with irregular verbs eliciting equivalent levels of activation, challenging the binary distinction of LIFG activation patterns for regular and irregular inflections. Furthermore, the current results contradict the notion that all morphologically complex words are processed as whole words, which would predict similar activation levels for regular and irregular inflections across widespread bilateral regions, including the IFG pars orbitalis, MTG, and occipitotemporal regions. Instead, the present findings support a single-system mechanism by which continuous rather than categorical differences between regular and irregular inflections are expected. These results align with a more general function of the LIFG, which involves processing morphologically complex words, as suggested by the previous fMRI literature on derivation and inflection [22,26,33,34].
Furthermore, semantic effects were reflected in the modulation of the temporal region, particularly the right MTG, along with the occipitotemporal regions observed in the whole-brain results. The functional contribution of temporal regions to semantic processing is well documented [41,42]. Surprisingly, increased activation in these regions, interpreted as engaging greater neural resources to resolve semantic ambiguity in previous research [39], contradicts the behavioral findings of this study, in which facilitative effects for homonyms were observed for base and irregular forms. However, a significant difference in right MTG activation between homonymous and unambiguous words suggests that facilitative effects for homonyms may be due to greater activation of abundant lexical-semantic representations [82,83]. Furthermore, both homonymous and unambiguous words elicited equivalent activation in the LIFG pars triangularis, suggesting that the processing of homonyms relies more on lexical-semantic representations than on the top-down regulation of multiple competitors.
Finally, the current findings in Korean, a morphologically rich and agglutinative language, underscore the importance of exploring morphological processing in languages beyond the widely studied Indo-European family. Similar research typically focuses on languages with inflectional suffixes, yet it remains unclear how our methodology might apply to languages that utilize prefixes or exhibit mixed morphology. Languages with prefixes, such as Swahili, and those with mixed morphological systems, like German, may process morphological structures differently due to their unique combinatory patterns of morphemes. Future research should consider adapting our approach to investigate whether similar neural mechanisms are involved in these languages.
Moreover, the languages considered in our study are strongly agglutinative, characterized by clear and regular morpheme boundaries. This raises the question of whether our findings would hold true in languages with a higher degree of synthetic inflection, which generally exhibit more irregular and fusional morphologies. Languages such as Latin or Russian, which often blend morphemes into more complex forms, could potentially demonstrate different patterns of neural activation due to the increased cognitive demands of parsing these forms. Investigating such languages could reveal whether the graded effects of verb regularity and the semantic influence observed in Korean are universally applicable or specific to agglutinative languages.

5. Conclusions

The present study investigated the spatial dynamics underlying the visual recognition of morphologically complex Korean verbs using a rapid event-related fMRI design. The findings challenge the dual-route model of inflectional morphology [6,17,28], which posits distinct processing pathways for regular and irregular verbs. Contrary to this model, both regular and irregular verbs showed equivalent levels of activation in the LIFG, a core region in the proposed procedural memory network. Instead, these results support the general role of the LIFG in handling morphological processing demands, a role primarily demonstrated in studies of morphologically rich languages [33,73]. Importantly, graded effects for verb regularity were observed in the bilateral occipitotemporal regions, with regular inflections eliciting increased activation in the left FG compared to base and irregular forms. The AG, a region in the IPL consistently associated with semantic processing, was differentiated between the three inflection types, with the base forms showing the lowest levels of deactivation, followed by the regular and then irregular inflections. These results provide evidence for the single-system mechanism [7,32], which argues for a continuous effect of verb regularity, with orthographic, semantic, and phonological properties converging in the processing of words. Furthermore, the facilitative effect of semantic ambiguity was reflected in the selective deactivation of the MTG and MOG for homonyms, while the LIFG showed equivalent activation for both homonymous and unambiguous words. These findings suggest that processing Korean homonyms may benefit from facilitated access to lexical-semantic information rather than being inhibited by the processes of selection and regulation among multiple meanings [84,85].
The pattern of activation observed across widespread bilateral inferior frontal, temporal, and occipitotemporal regions for processing morphologically complex Korean verbs suggests that the integration of morphological and semantic information involves a complex network of brain regions. These results align with the view that posits a distributed function involving extensive neural networks involved in morphological processing [86] and, more generally, in language processing [87,88].

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci14080752/s1, Table S1: Experimental conditions and statistical comparisons of sublexical and lexical variables.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.K. and K.N.; Formal analysis, J.K.; Funding acquisition, J.K.; Investigation, J.K. and S.K.; Methodology, J.K. and K.N.; Resources, J.K. and S.K.; Supervision, K.N.; Validation, S.K. and K.N.; Visualization, J.K. and S.K.; Writing—original draft, J.K.; Writing—review and editing, S.K. and K.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea, grant number NRF-2019H1A2A1076382.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Korea University (protocol code KUIRB-2021-0427-01, 21 December 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author due to privacy and ethical restrictions, as they contain information that could compromise the privacy of research participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1 and Table A2 show the stimuli set used in the experiment, representing words and pseudowords, respectively. The words are categorized under six different conditions: Base Control, Base Homonym, Regular Control, Regular Homonym, Irregular Control, and Irregular Homonym.
Table A1. The word stimuli set used in the experiment.
Table A1. The word stimuli set used in the experiment.
Base
Control
Base
Homonym
Regular
Control
Regular
Homonym
Irregular
Control
Irregular
Homonym
씻다갈다죄어감아눠서갈려
신다깨다찧어걷어썩혀쳤어
끊다들다엮고개서기워쪼여
좇다뛰다볶아불지붐벼텄다
놀다뜨다꽂고붓지모셔맞혀
낫다맞다믿니빌지는다말려
뽑다맡다서라빨면도와배겨
돌다매다맺자새자실어걷혀
참다묻다뚫어켜며굽혔다멘다
씹다받다갚게패지놓쳤다져도
찢다쉬다빚고꾸어어는가졸려
얻다타다넣겠다굽지판다고훔쳐
견디다구르다휘었고거르고저었고감겼고
살피다구하다굴리지배어도뒀다들려서
버티다그리다우기지끼치고뭉쳐서바랬지
벌이다달리다감싸면놀리자박혔다물려서
싸우다따르다다듬고닥치지치민다써가며
부르다부치다넓히며돋는다샀고일렀다
거닐다빠지다찾지만싸놓고꿴다익혀서
다투다울리다닿았고불리지줬고태웠다
Table A2. The pseudoword stimuli set used in the experiment.
Table A2. The pseudoword stimuli set used in the experiment.
Pseudowords
닥든멜쳐폐진혈긴치오든기까래
긴들어빌출다칫자싸치곤잡우는
애건벽다치펴드잘기휠지설기건
바면새룬먹낼펴잰먹얹네꺼담네
버닌망갠돛던휘슨이패든뜯리던
취랜다길둘긴다쉴치린지싸눌은
제깐규며슬신시딘까리니지패게
곰다푹인취든잠선기힐지암섰지
태려채셔키룬잴출씌희네밑절까
네칠석린핏지줄핀우이어높리던
펄다쨉던추랜포실졸켜니치철지
몫다닻다급던묵진끼진가뜨러니
꾀실겅틸둡는으인다기묻냐닫내는
죄릴귤던찰댄주갈라씌지곤먹우네
후나우알치핀치놀까골길까기말지
껀든덤네팝자키르냐바몰면꿰루곤
보난긷서매몬박낸들꼽운다차물다
철다지뺀삽아움나며곯기든가먹은
빗셔달탄볍자살건다설까던팝는다
유길유날벗친보힌대기훔다펴려요

References

  1. Butterworth, B. Lexical Representation. In Language Production Vol. 2.; Butterworth, B., Ed.; Academic Press: London, UK, 1983; pp. 257–294. [Google Scholar]
  2. Taft, M. Morphological Decomposition and the Reverse Base Frequency Effect. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A Hum. Exp. Psychol. 2004, 57, 745–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Taft, M.; Forster, K.I. Lexical Storage and Retrieval of Prefixed Words. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1975, 14, 638–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Clahsen, H. Lexical Entries and Rules of Language: A Multidisciplinary Study of German Inflection. Behav. Brain Sci. 1999, 22, 991–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Pinker, S. Rules of Language. Science 1991, 253, 530–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ullman, M.T. Contributions of Memory Circuits to Language: The Declarative/Procedural Model. Cognition 2004, 92, 231–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Joanisse, M.F.; Seidenberg, M.S. Imaging the Past: Neural Activation in Frontal and Temporal Regions during Regular and Irregular Past-Tense Processing. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 5, 282–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Marslen-Wilson, W.; Tyler, L.K. Rules, Representations, and the English Past Tense. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998, 2, 428–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. McClelland, J.L.; Patterson, K. Rules or Connections in Past-Tense Inflections: What Does the Evidence Rule Out? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 465–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. McClelland, J.L.; Rumelhart, D.E. On Learning the Past Tenses of English Verbs. In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition: Psychological and Biological Models; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 1987; pp. 216–271. ISBN 9780262291262. [Google Scholar]
  11. Seidenberg, M.S.; McClelland, J.L. A Distributed, Developmental Model of Word Recognition and Naming. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 523–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Leminen, A.; Smolka, E.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Pliatsikas, C. Morphological Processing in the Brain: The Good (Inflection), the Bad (Derivation) and the Ugly (Compounding). Cortex 2019, 116, 4–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Berg, T. On the Relationship between Type and Token Frequency. J. Quant. Linguist. 2014, 21, 199–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ullman, M.T. Acceptability Ratings of Regular and Irregular Past-Tense Forms: Evidence for a Dual-System Model of Language from Word Frequency and Phonological Neighbourhood Effects. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1999, 14, 47–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cuskley, C.F.; Pugliese, M.; Castellano, C.; Colaiori, F.; Loreto, V.; Tria, F. Internal and External Dynamics in Language: Evidence from Verb Regularity in a Historical Corpus of English. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e102882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kim-Renaud, Y.-K. “Irregular” Verbs in Korean Revisited. Lang. Res. 1977, 9, 206–225. [Google Scholar]
  17. Pinker, S.; Ullman, M.T. The Past and Future of the Past Tense. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 456–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Davis, M.H.; Meunier, F.; Marslen-Wilson, W.D. Neural Responses to Morphological, Syntactic, and Semantic Properties of Single Words: An FMRI Study. Brain Lang. 2004, 89, 439–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Devlin, J.T.; Jamison, H.L.; Matthews, P.M.; Gonnerman, L.M. Morphology and the Internal Structure of Words. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 14984–14988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Gold, B.T.; Rastle, K. Neural Correlates of Morphological Decomposition during Visual Word Recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 1983–1993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bozic, M.; Tyler, L.K.; Li, S.; Wingfield, C.; Marslen-Wilson, W.D. Neurobiological Systems for Lexical Representation and Analysis in English. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 25, 1678–1691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bozic, M.; Marslen-Wilson, W.D.; Stamatakis, E.A.; Davis, M.H.; Tyler, L.K. Differentiating Morphology, Form, and Meaning: Neural Correlates of Morphological Complexity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 1464–1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Vannest, J.; Newport, E.L.; Newman, A.J.; Bavelier, D. Interplay between Morphology and Frequency in Lexical Access: The Case of the Base Frequency Effect. Brain Res. 2011, 1373, 144–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bozic, M.; Marslen-Wilson, W. Neurocognitive Contexts for Morphological Complexity: Dissociating Inflection and Derivation. Linguist. Lang. Compass 2010, 4, 1063–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bulut, T. Neural Correlates of Morphological Processing: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. Cortex 2022, 151, 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Tyler, L.K.; Bright, P.; Fletcher, P.; Stamatakis, E.A. Neural Processing of Nouns and Verbs: The Role of Inflectional Morphology. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 512–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Marslen-Wilson, W.D.; Tyler, L.K. Morphology, Language and the Brain: The Decompositional Substrate for Language Comprehension. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2007, 362, 823–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Ullman, M.T. A Neurocognitive Perspective on Language: The Declarative/Procedural Model. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 717–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Pliatsikas, C.; Johnstone, T.; Marinis, T. FMRI Evidence for the Involvement of the Procedural Memory System in Morphological Processing of a Second Language. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e97298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Vannest, J.; Polk, T.A.; Lewis, R.L. Dual-Route Processing of Complex Words: New FMRI Evidence from Derivational Suffixation. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 5, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Beretta, A.; Campbell, C.; Carr, T.H.; Huang, J.; Schmitt, L.M.; Christianson, K.; Cao, Y. An ER-FMRI Investigation of Morphological Inflection in German Reveals That the Brain Makes a Distinction between Regular and Irregular Forms. Brain Lang. 2003, 85, 67–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Desai, R.; Conant, L.L.; Waldron, E.; Binder, J.R. FMRI of Past Tense Processing: The Effects of Phonological Complexity and Task Difficulty. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 278–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Lehtonen, M.; Vorobyev, V.A.; Hugdahl, K.; Tuokkola, T.; Laine, M. Neural Correlates of Morphological Decomposition in a Morphologically Rich Language: An FMRI Study. Brain Lang. 2006, 98, 182–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Lehtonen, M.; Vorobyev, V.; Soveri, A.; Hugdahl, K.; Tuokkola, T.; Laine, M. Language-Specific Activations in the Brain: Evidence from Inflectional Processing in Bilinguals. J. Neurolinguistics 2009, 22, 495–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Yokoyama, S.; Miyamoto, T.; Riera, J.; Kim, J.; Akitsuki, Y.; Iwata, K.; Yoshimoto, K.; Horie, K.; Sato, S.; Kawashima, R. Cortical Mechanisms Involved in the Processing of Verbs: An FMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 1304–1313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Yim, H.; Park, C.; Lim, H.; Nam, K. Mental Representation and Processing Involved in Comprehending Korean Regular and Irregular Verb Eojeols: An FMRI and Reaction Time Study. In Proceedings of the Neural Information Processing: 13th International Conference, ICONIP 2006, Hong Kong, China, 3–6 October 2006; Volume 4232, pp. 247–254. [Google Scholar]
  37. Russo, A.G.; Esposito, F.; Laudanna, A.; Mancuso, A.; Di Salle, F.; Elia, A.; De Martino, M. The Neural Substrate of Noun Morphological Inflection: A Rapid Event-Related FMRI Study in Italian. Neuropsychologia 2021, 151, 107699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Grindrod, C.M.; Garnett, E.O.; Malyutina, S.; den Ouden, D.B. Effects of Representational Distance between Meanings on the Neural Correlates of Semantic Ambiguity. Brain Lang. 2014, 139, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Rodd, J.M.; Davis, M.H.; Johnsrude, I.S. The Neural Mechanisms of Speech Comprehension: FMRI Studies of Semantic Ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex 2005, 15, 1261–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Vitello, S.; Rodd, J.M. Resolving Semantic Ambiguities in Sentences: Cognitive Processes and Brain Mechanisms. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2015, 9, 391–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Binder, J.R.; Desai, R.H. The Neurobiology of Semantic Memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 527–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lambon Ralph, M.A.; Jefferies, E.; Patterson, K.; Rogers, T.T. The Neural and Computational Bases of Semantic Cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 42–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hoffman, P.; Tamm, A. Barking up the Right Tree: Univariate and Multivariate FMRI Analyses of Homonym Comprehension. Neuroimage 2020, 219, 117050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Humphreys, G.F.; Lambon Ralph, M.A. Mapping Domain-Selective and Counterpointed Domain-General Higher Cognitive Functions in the Lateral Parietal Cortex: Evidence from FMRI Comparisons of Difficulty-Varying Semantic Versus Visuo-Spatial Tasks, and Functional Connectivity Analyses. Cerebral Cortex 2017, 27, 4199–4212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Zempleni, M.Z.; Renken, R.; Hoeks, J.C.J.; Hoogduin, J.M.; Stowe, L.A. Semantic Ambiguity Processing in Sentence Context: Evidence from Event-Related FMRI. Neuroimage 2007, 34, 1270–1279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Vartiainen, J.; Aggujaro, S.; Lehtonen, M.; Hultén, A.; Laine, M.; Salmelin, R. Neural Dynamics of Reading Morphologically Complex Words. Neuroimage 2009, 47, 2064–2072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lehtonen, M.; Cunillera, T.; Rodríguez-Fornells, A.; Hultén, A.; Tuomainen, J.; Laine, M. Recognition of Morphologically Complex Words in Finnish: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. Brain Res. 2007, 1148, 123–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Soveri, A.; Lehtonen, M.; Laine, M. Word Frequency and Morphological Processing in Finnish Revisited. Ment. Lex. 2007, 2, 359–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Oldfield, R.C. The Assessment and Analysis of Handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kang, B.; Kim, H. Frequency of Korean Language: Based on 15 Million Size Eojeol Sejong Syntactic and Semantic Corpus; Handkook Munhwasa: Seoul, Republic of Korea, 2009; ISBN 978895726636693710. [Google Scholar]
  51. Pae, H.K.; Bae, S.; Yi, K. Lexical Properties Influencing Visual Word Recognition in Hangul. Read. Writ. 2020, 33, 2391–2412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lee, E.; Lee, S.; Kim, Y. The Mechanism for Processing Regular and Irregular Inflections of Korean Verbs—A Priming Study. Korean Lang. Lit. 2018, 183, 67–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Park, H.; Nam, K.; Koo, M. The Lexical Access of Regular and Irregular Korean Verbs in the Mental Lexicon. Korean J. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 23, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Azuma, T. Familiarity and Relatedness of Word Meanings: Ratings for 110 Homographs. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1996, 28, 109–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Rubenstein, H.; Garfield, L.; Millikan, J.A. Homographic Entries in the Internal Lexicon. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1970, 9, 487–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Kim, J.; Kang, J.; Kim, J.; Nam, K. Temporal Dynamics of Form and Meaning in Morphologically Complex Word Processing: An ERP Study on Korean Inflected Verbs. J. Neurolinguistics 2022, 64, 101098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kim, S.; Lee, C.; Nam, K. The Examination of the Visual-Perceptual Locus in Hemispheric Laterality of the Word Length Effect Using Korean Visual Word. Laterality 2022, 27, 485–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Kim, S.; Koo, M.-M.; Kim, J.; Nam, K. The Research for Language Information Processing of Bilateral Hemispheres on Korean Noun Eojeol: Visual Half-Field Study. Korean J. Cogn. Biol. Psychol. 2020, 32, 29–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kim, S.; Paterson, K.B.; Nam, K.; Lee, C. Lateralized Displays Reveal the Perceptual Locus of the Syllable Transposition Effect in Korean. Neuropsychologia 2024, 199, 108907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kim, J.; Lee, S.; Kim, S.; Nam, K. Syllable Frequency Effect in Visual Word Recognition: A Regression Study on Morphologically Simple and Complex Korean Words. Korean J. Cogn. Biol. Psychol. 2024, 35, 303–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lee, S.; Kim, S.; Kim, J.; Kwon, S.; Lee, E.-H.; Nam, K. The Facilitative Effect of the First Syllable Token Frequency in Visual Recognition of Korean Predicate Eojeols. Korean J. Cogn. Biol. Psychol. 2023, 35, 337–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kim, S.; Kim, J.; Nam, K. Familiarity with Words Modulates Interhemispheric Interactions in Visual Word Recognition. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 892858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kim, S.; Kim, J.; Nam, K. Electrophysiological Evidence Reveals the Asymmetric Transfer from the Right to Left Hemisphere as Key to Reading Proficiency. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Dale, A.M. Optimal Experimental Design for Event-Related FMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999, 8, 109–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Baayen, R.H. Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics Using R; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 9780521882590. [Google Scholar]
  66. Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.M.; Walker, S.C. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, (Version 4.2.1); R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/(accessed on 7 June 2022).
  68. The MathWorks Inc. MATLAB, Version: 23.2.0 (R2023b); The MathWorks Inc.: Portola Valley, CA, USA, 2023.
  69. Taylor, J.S.H.; Rastle, K.; Davis, M.H. Interpreting Response Time Effects in Functional Imaging Studies. Neuroimage 2014, 99, 419–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Vigneau, M.; Beaucousin, V.; Hervé, P.Y.; Duffau, H.; Crivello, F.; Houdé, O.; Mazoyer, B.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. Meta-Analyzing Left Hemisphere Language Areas: Phonology, Semantics, and Sentence Processing. Neuroimage 2006, 30, 1414–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Rolls, E.T.; Huang, C.C.; Lin, C.P.; Feng, J.; Joliot, M. Automated Anatomical Labelling Atlas 3. Neuroimage 2020, 206, 116189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Brett, M.; Anton, J.-L.; Valabregue, R.; Poline, J.-B. Others Region of Interest Analysis Using the MarsBar Toolbox for SPM 99. Neuroimage 2002, 16, S497. [Google Scholar]
  73. Leminen, A.; Leminen, M.; Lehtonen, M.; Nevalainen, P.; Ylinen, S.; Kimppa, L.; Sannemann, C.; Mäkelä, J.P.; Kujala, T. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of the Processing of Spoken Inflected and Derived Words: A Combined EEG and MEG Study. Front Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Marslen-Wilson, W.; Tyler, L.K.; Waksler, R.; Older, L. Morphology and Meaning in the English Mental Lexicon. Psychol. Rev. 1994, 101, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Devlin, J.T.; Jamison, H.L.; Gonnerman, L.M.; Matthews, P.M. The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 911–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Cohen, L.; Dehaene, S.; Naccache, L.; Lehéricy, S.; Dehaene-Lambertz, G.; Hénaff, M.-A.; Michel, F. The Visual Word Form Area. Brain 2000, 123, 291–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Cohen, L.; Lehéricy, S.; Chochon, F.; Lemer, C.; Rivaud, S.; Dehaene, S. Language-specific Tuning of Visual Cortex? Functional Properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain 2002, 125, 1054–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dehaene, S.; Naccache, L.; Cohen, L.; Bihan, D.L.; Mangin, J.-F.; Poline, J.-B.; Rivière, D. Cerebral Mechanisms of Word Masking and Unconscious Repetition Priming. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 752–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Rastle, K.; Davis, M.H.; New, B. The Broth in My Brother’s Brothel: Morpho-Orthographic Segmentation in Visual Word Recognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2004, 11, 1090–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Kronbichler, M.; Hutzler, F.; Wimmer, H.; Mair, A.; Staffen, W.; Ladurner, G. The Visual Word Form Area and the Frequency with Which Words Are Encountered: Evidence from a Parametric FMRI Study. Neuroimage 2004, 21, 946–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Kuhnke, P.; Chapman, C.A.; Cheung, V.K.M.; Turker, S.; Graessner, A.; Martin, S.; Williams, K.A.; Hartwigsen, G. The Role of the Angular Gyrus in Semantic Cognition: A Synthesis of Five Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Brain Struct. Funct. 2023, 228, 273–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Bedny, M.; McGill, M.; Thompson-Schill, S.L. Semantic Adaptation and Competition during Word Comprehension. Cerebral Cortex 2008, 18, 2574–2585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Tyler, L.K.; Cheung, T.P.L.; Devereux, B.J.; Clarke, A. Syntactic Computations in the Language Network: Characterizing Dynamic Network Properties Using Representational Similarity Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Kim, J.; Kang, K.G.-Y.; Yoo, D.; Jeon, I.; Kim, H.K.; Nam, H.; Shin, J.; Nam, K. Lexico-Semantic Interactions during the Visual and Spoken Recognition of Homonymous Korean Eojeols. Phon. Speech Sci. 2021, 13, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Kim, J.; Kim, S.; Nam, K. The Time Course of Semantic Ambiguity in Visual Word Recognition: Behavioral and ERP Evidence for the Lexical-Semantic Effect. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1378125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Cavalli, E.; Colé, P.; Badier, J.M.; Zielinski, C.; Chanoine, V.; Ziegler, J.C. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Morphological Processing in Visual Word Recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 28, 1228–1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Friederici, A.D. The Cortical Language Circuit: From Auditory Perception to Sentence Comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 262–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Hagoort, P. On Broca, Brain, and Binding: A New Framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 416–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Schematic representation of two opposing views on verb regularity with an example of Korean regular and irregular inflections. (a) An example of Korean regular and irregular inflections. (b) Schematic representation of the dual-mechanism accounts of inflectional morphology [17]. (c) Schematic representation of the single-system theory of morphology [7].
Figure 1. Schematic representation of two opposing views on verb regularity with an example of Korean regular and irregular inflections. (a) An example of Korean regular and irregular inflections. (b) Schematic representation of the dual-mechanism accounts of inflectional morphology [17]. (c) Schematic representation of the single-system theory of morphology [7].
Brainsci 14 00752 g001
Figure 2. Rapid event-related fMRI paradigm used in the study. The plus sign (‘+’) represents a visual fixation point, and a series of asterisks (‘*****’) denotes a non-linguistic baseline mask.
Figure 2. Rapid event-related fMRI paradigm used in the study. The plus sign (‘+’) represents a visual fixation point, and a series of asterisks (‘*****’) denotes a non-linguistic baseline mask.
Brainsci 14 00752 g002
Figure 3. The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of condition contrasted with the baseline mask condition. (a) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of morphology, in which highlighted regions indicate significant effects for base (green), regular (red), and irregular (blue) conditions and overlapping regions (yellow). (b) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, in which highlighted regions represent significant effects for control (green) and homonym (red) conditions. The yellow color indicates overlapping regions.
Figure 3. The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of condition contrasted with the baseline mask condition. (a) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of morphology, in which highlighted regions indicate significant effects for base (green), regular (red), and irregular (blue) conditions and overlapping regions (yellow). (b) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, in which highlighted regions represent significant effects for control (green) and homonym (red) conditions. The yellow color indicates overlapping regions.
Brainsci 14 00752 g003
Figure 4. (a) The whole-brain activation map of the fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The highlighted regions indicate FG (red), LG (blue), and MOG (yellow). (b) Post-hoc analysis results of the main effect of morphology. (c) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of semantic ambiguity. (d) Post-hoc analysis results on the two-way interaction. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, † p < 0.1.
Figure 4. (a) The whole-brain activation map of the fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The highlighted regions indicate FG (red), LG (blue), and MOG (yellow). (b) Post-hoc analysis results of the main effect of morphology. (c) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of semantic ambiguity. (d) Post-hoc analysis results on the two-way interaction. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, † p < 0.1.
Brainsci 14 00752 g004
Figure 5. ROI analysis results that showed the significant effect of morphology. (a) The regions highlighted in red, blue, green, and yellow represent LIFG pars triangluaris, LIFG pars orbitalis, AG, and MTG, respectively. (b) The beta estimates for each condition and statistical comparisons among inflection types. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Figure 5. ROI analysis results that showed the significant effect of morphology. (a) The regions highlighted in red, blue, green, and yellow represent LIFG pars triangluaris, LIFG pars orbitalis, AG, and MTG, respectively. (b) The beta estimates for each condition and statistical comparisons among inflection types. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Brainsci 14 00752 g005
Figure 6. ROI analysis results illustrating the significant effect of ambiguity. (a) The corresponding regions highlighted in red (SMG), blue (left MOG), and yellow (right MTG). (b) The beta estimates for each ambiguity condition and statistical comparisons between control and homonymous words. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05, † p < 0.1.
Figure 6. ROI analysis results illustrating the significant effect of ambiguity. (a) The corresponding regions highlighted in red (SMG), blue (left MOG), and yellow (right MTG). (b) The beta estimates for each ambiguity condition and statistical comparisons between control and homonymous words. Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05, † p < 0.1.
Brainsci 14 00752 g006
Table 1. Mean reaction time (RT, in ms), percentage of accuracy (ACC, %), and standard deviations (SD, in parenthesis) for six conditions.
Table 1. Mean reaction time (RT, in ms), percentage of accuracy (ACC, %), and standard deviations (SD, in parenthesis) for six conditions.
Inflection TypeSemantic Ambiguity
ControlHomonym
RT (ms)ACC (%)RT (ms)ACC (%)
Base569
(113)
93.38
(24.89)
555
(105)
96.78
(17.67)
Regular583
(114)
87.98
(32.56)
602
(122)
85.19
(35.58)
Irregular588
(122)
91.85
(28.87)
594
(121)
90.12
(29.87)
Note. Reaction times (RTs) for correct responses only.
Table 2. Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of morphological condition, relative to the baseline mask condition.
Table 2. Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of morphological condition, relative to the baseline mask condition.
ContrastCluster
Size
Brain RegionsHemisphereMNI CoordinatesZ
xyz
Base > Mask89Precentral gyrusL−36−28565.75
Reg > Mask71CalcarineL−12−100−45.10
32Middle occipital gyrusR36−7623.40
116CalcarineR9−8284.60
Lingual gyrusL0−7353.97
CalcarineR6−8823.94
55Postcentral gyrusL−48−16564.54
Precentral gyrusL−33−10654.00
−51−4503.81
98ThalamusR21−2224.23
PutamenR30−4−73.87
65Fusiform gyrusL−42−40−194.16
−39−76−133.91
−42−55−193.55
39Inferior parietal gyrusL−39−28383.98
Postcentral gyrusL−42−34503.85
Irreg > Mask55Supplementary motor areaL−38653.64
Middle cingulate cortexL−3−4503.44
Note. All regions were significant to q < 0.05 FDR-corrected, kE ≥ 30 at the cluster level, and p < 0.001 at a voxel level. Reg = regular inflection; Irreg = irregular inflection.
Table 3. Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, relative to the mask condition.
Table 3. Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, relative to the mask condition.
ContrastCluster
Size
Brain RegionsHemisphereMNI CoordinatesZ
xyz
Con > Mask285CalcarineL−12−100−45.18
Fusiform gyrusL−36−79−164.67
Middle occipital gyrusL−27−8824.65
105Postcentral gyrusL−36−34564.68
L−42−16503.53
Precentral gyrusL−33−16504.27
316CalcarineR9−8284.26
Lingual gyrusR15−94−74.23
Hom > Mask119Supplementary motor areaR914474.55
L−65564.42
57Postcentral gyrusL−36−34564.10
Precentral gyrusL−24−19683.46
−33−22653.46
Note. All regions were significant to q < 0.05 at the FDR-corrected level, kE ≥ 30 at the cluster level, and p < 0.001 at a voxel level. Con = Control; Hom = Homonym.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kim, J.; Kim, S.; Nam, K. Neural Dynamics of Processing Inflectional Morphology: An fMRI Study on Korean Inflected Verbs. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 752. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080752

AMA Style

Kim J, Kim S, Nam K. Neural Dynamics of Processing Inflectional Morphology: An fMRI Study on Korean Inflected Verbs. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(8):752. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080752

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kim, Joonwoo, Sangyub Kim, and Kichun Nam. 2024. "Neural Dynamics of Processing Inflectional Morphology: An fMRI Study on Korean Inflected Verbs" Brain Sciences 14, no. 8: 752. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080752

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Article metric data becomes available approximately 24 hours after publication online.
Back to TopTop