On the Nature of the Word-Reduction Phenomenon: The Contribution of Bilingualism
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Predictions for Current Research
- Both repeat and switch second mentions will be reduced in comparison to first mentions.
- Due to a plausible larger amount of facilitation, second mentions in the repeat condition are susceptible to be more reduced than second mentions in the switch condition.
- Word reduction in the switch condition might be benefited by the presence of cognates, compared to non-cognates.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Procedure
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Word Spanish | Word Catalan | Word English | LogFreq Spanish | LogFreq Catalan | #Letters Spanish | #Letters Catalan | Cognate Status |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
aguja | agulla | needle | 0.995 | 1.38 | 5 | 6 | cognate |
alfombra | catifa | carpet | 1.23 | 0.921 | 8 | 6 | non-cognate |
anillo | anell | ring | 1.197 | 1.255 | 6 | 5 | cognate |
arco | arc | arc | 1.375 | 1.555 | 4 | 3 | cognate |
balanza | balança | scale | 1.092 | 1.062 | 7 | 7 | cognate |
ballena | balena | whale | 0.604 | 0.484 | 7 | 6 | cognate |
banco | banc | bench | 1.679 | 1.612 | 5 | 4 | cognate |
bastón | bastó | walking stick | 1.105 | 1.399 | 6 | 5 | cognate |
biberón | biberó | bottle | 0.604 | 0.143 | 7 | 6 | cognate |
bolo | bitlla | skittle | 0.186 | 0.105 | 4 | 6 | non-cognate |
búho | mussol | owl | 0.736 | 0.844 | 4 | 6 | non-cognate |
caja | caixa | box | 1.661 | 1.743 | 4 | 5 | cognate |
cama | llit | bed | 2.136 | 2.188 | 4 | 4 | non-cognate |
cangrejo | cranc | crab | 0.384 | 0.719 | 8 | 5 | non-cognate |
casco | casc | helmet | 1.265 | 0.998 | 5 | 4 | cognate |
cepillo | raspall | brush | 0.789 | 0.625 | 7 | 7 | non-cognate |
cerilla | llumí | match | 0.658 | 0.556 | 7 | 5 | non-cognate |
cesta | cistella | basket | 0.889 | 0.712 | 5 | 8 | cognate |
cinturón | cinturó | belt | 1.192 | 0.837 | 8 | 7 | cognate |
cometa | estel | kite | 0.927 | 1.298 | 6 | 5 | non-cognate |
cruz | creu | cross | 1.401 | 2.248 | 4 | 4 | cognate |
cubo | galleda | bucket | 0.848 | 0.738 | 4 | 7 | non-cognate |
cuchillo | ganivet | knife | 1.212 | 1.353 | 8 | 7 | non-cognate |
cuna | bressol | cot | 1.122 | 1.098 | 4 | 6 | non-cognate |
espejo | mirall | mirror | 1.822 | 1.587 | 6 | 6 | non-cognate |
gafas | ulleres | glasses | 1.53 | 1.266 | 5 | 7 | non-cognate |
hacha | destral | ax | 0.858 | 1.038 | 5 | 7 | non-cognate |
hucha | guardiola | piggy bank | 0.233 | 0.388 | 5 | 9 | non-cognate |
imán | imant | magnet | 0.776 | 0.363 | 4 | 5 | cognate |
jarrón | gerro | vase | 0.691 | 0.79 | 6 | 5 | non-cognate |
jirafa | girafa | giraffe | 0.315 | 0.137 | 6 | 6 | cognate |
martillo | martell | hammer | 0.801 | 0.987 | 8 | 7 | cognate |
mono | mico | monkey | 1.281 | 0.544 | 4 | 4 | non-cognate |
oreja | orella | ear | 1.359 | 1.724 | 5 | 6 | cognate |
pájaro | ocell | bird | 1.335 | 1.564 | 6 | 5 | non-cognate |
pecera | peixera | fishbowl | 0.233 | 0.359 | 6 | 7 | cognate |
perro | gos | dog | 1.787 | 1.786 | 5 | 3 | non-cognate |
pie | peu | foot | 2.124 | 2.246 | 3 | 3 | cognate |
pinza | pinça | pin | 0.47 | 0.283 | 5 | 5 | cognate |
piña | pinya | pineapple | 0.542 | 0.633 | 4 | 5 | cognate |
plato | plat | dish | 1.502 | 1.639 | 5 | 4 | cognate |
ratón | ratolí | mouse | 0.927 | 0.755 | 5 | 6 | cognate |
silbato | xiulet | whistle | 0.415 | 0.949 | 7 | 6 | non-cognate |
silla | cadira | chair | 1.69 | 1.726 | 5 | 6 | non-cognate |
sombrero | barret | hat | 1.497 | 1.538 | 8 | 6 | non-cognate |
tiburón | tauró | shark | 0.564 | 0.296 | 7 | 5 | non-cognate |
tronco | tronc | log | 1.161 | 1.436 | 6 | 5 | cognate |
ventana | finestra | window | 1.975 | 1.89 | 7 | 8 | non-cognate |
Condition 1a | Condition 1b | Condition 2a | Condition 2b | ||||
1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object |
whistle (cat) | bird (cat) | bird (cat) | whistle (cat) | whistle (sp) | bird (sp) | bird (sp) | whistle (sp) |
knife (sp) | kite (sp) | kite (sp) | knife (sp) | knife (cat) | kite (cat) | kite (cat) | knife (cat) |
skittle (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | skittle (sp) | skittle (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | skittle (cat) |
skittle (cat) | pin (cat) | pin (cat) | skittle (cat) | skittle (sp) | pin (sp) | pin (sp) | skittle (sp) |
knife (sp) | pin (sp) | pin (sp) | knife (sp) | knife (cat) | pin (cat) | pin (cat) | knife (cat) |
monkey (cat) | bird (cat) | bird (cat) | monkey (cat) | monkey(sp) | bird (sp) | bird (sp) | monkey(sp) |
monkey(sp) | kite (sp) | kite (sp) | monkey (sp) | monkey (cat) | kite (cat) | kite (cat) | monkey(cat) |
whistle (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | whistle (cat) | whistle (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | whistle (sp) |
Condition 3a | Condition 3b | Condition 4a | Condition 4b | ||||
1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object |
knife (sp) | kite (sp) | kite (sp) | knife (sp) | knife (cat) | kite (cat) | kite (cat) | knife (cat) |
skittle (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | skittle (cat) | skittle (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | skittle (sp) |
skittle (cat) | bird (cat) | bird (cat) | skittle (cat) | skittle (sp) | bird (sp) | bird (sp) | skittle (sp) |
knife (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | piggy bank (cat) | knife (cat) | knife (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | piggy bank (sp) | knife (sp) |
monkey (sp) | peg (sp) | peg (sp) | monkey (sp) | monkey (cat) | peg(cat) | peg(cat) | monkey (cat) |
monkey (sp) | bird (sp) | bird (sp) | monkey (sp) | monkey (cat) | bird (cat) | bird (cat) | monkey (cat) |
whistle (cat) | kite (cat) | kite (cat) | whistle (cat) | whistle (sp) | kite (sp) | kite (sp) | whistle (sp) |
whistle (sp) | peg (sp) | Peg (sp) | whistle (sp) | whistle (cat) | peg (cat) | peg (cat) | whistle (cat) |
Condition 1a | Condition 1b | Condition 2a | Condition 2b | ||||
1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object |
xiulet | ocell | ocell | xiulet | silbato | pájaro | pájaro | silbato |
cuchillo | cometa | cometa | cuchillo | ganivet | estel | estel | ganivet |
bolo | hucha | hucha | bolo | bitlla | guardiola | guardiola | bitlla |
bitlla | pinça | pinça | bitlla | bolo | pinza | pinza | bolo |
cuchillo | pinza | pinza | cuchillo | ganivet | pinça | pinça | cuchillo |
mico | ocell | ocell | mico | mono | pájaro | pájaro | mono |
mono | cometa | cometa | mono | mico | estel | estel | mico |
xiulet | guardiola | guardiola | xiulet | silbato | hucha | hucha | silbato |
Condition 3a | Condition 3b | Condition 4a | Condition 4b | ||||
1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object | 1st object | 2nd object |
cuchillo | cometa | cometa | cuchillo | ganivet | estel | estel | ganivet |
bitlla | guardiola | guardiola | bitlla | bolo | hucha | hucha | bolo |
bitlla | ocell | ocell | bitlla | bolo | pájaro | pájaro | bolo |
ganivet | guardiola | guardiola | ganivet | cuchillo | hucha | hucha | cuchillo |
hucha | pinza | pinza | hucha | mico | pinça | pinça | mico |
mono | pájaro | pájaro | mono | mico | ocell | ocell | mico |
xiulet | estel | estel | xiulet | silbato | cometa | cometa | silbato |
silbato | pinza | pinza | silbato | xiulet | pinça | pinça | xiulet |
Word Spanish | Word Catalan | Word English | LogFreq Spanish | LogFreq Catalan | #Letters Spanish | #Letters Catalan | Cognate Status |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
árbol | arbre | tree | 1.55 | 1.99 | 5 | 5 | cognate |
bombilla | bombeta | lightbulb | 0.8 | 0.94 | 8 | 7 | cognate |
cohete | cohet | rocket | 0.87 | 0.1 | 6 | 5 | cognate |
globo | globus | balloon | 1.05 | 1.03 | 5 | 6 | cognate |
manzana | poma | apple | 1.08 | 1.13 | 7 | 4 | non-cognate |
seta | bolet | mushroom | 0.13 | 0.95 | 4 | 5 | non-cognate |
tenedor | forquilla | fork | 0.67 | 0.73 | 7 | 9 | non-cognate |
vaso | got | glass | 1.58 | 1.12 | 4 | 3 | non-cognate |
References
- Aylett, M.; Turk, A. The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Lang. Speech 2004, 47, 31–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, R.; Bradlow, A. variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody. Lang. Speech 2009, 52, 391–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bell, A.; Brenier, J.; Gregory, M.; Girand, C.; Jurafsky, D. Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. J. Mem. Lang. 2009, 60, 92–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jurafsky, D.; Bell, A.; Gregory, M.; Raymond, W.D. Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production. In Typological Studies in Language; Noonan, M., Ed.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2001; Volume 45, pp. 229–254. [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez-Cuadrado, S.; Baus, C.; Costa, A. Foreigner talk through word reduction in native/non-native spoken interactions. Biling. Lang. Cognit. 2008, 21, 419–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bell, A.; Gregory, M.L.; Brenier, J.M.; Jurafsky, D.; Ikeno, A.; Girand, C. Which predictability measures affect content word durations? In Proceedings of the ISCA Tutorial and Research Workshop (ITRW) on Pronunciation Modeling and Lexicon Adaptation for Spoken Language Technology, Aspen Lodge, Estes Park, CO, USA, 14–15 September 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Bell, A.; Jurafsky, D.; Fosler-Lussier, E.; Girand, C.; Gregory, M.; Gildea, D. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 113, 1001–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clark, H.H.; Haviland, S.E. Comprehension and the given-new contract. In Discourse Production and Comprehension. Discourse Processes: Advances in Research and Theory; Freedle, R.O., Ed.; Ablex Publishing: Norwood, NJ, USA; New York, NY, USA, 1977; Volume 1, pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Fowler, C.; Housum, J. Talkers’ signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. J. Mem. Lang. 1987, 26, 489–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gregory, M.L.; Raymond, W.D.; Bell, A.; Fosler-Lussier, E.; Jurafsky, D. The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production. In Chicago Linguistic Society; Chicago, IL, USA, 1999; pp. 151–166. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman, P. Some effects of context on the intelligibility of hearing and deaf children’s speech. Lang. Speech 1963, 24, 255–264. [Google Scholar]
- Samuel, S.G.; Troicki, M. Articulation quality is inversely related to redundancy when children or adults have verbal control. J. Mem. Lang. 1998, 39, 175–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, D.G.; Arnold, J.E.; Tanenhaus, M.K. Tic Tac TOE: Effects of predictability and importance on acoustic prominence in language production. Cognition 2008, 106, 1548–1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lam, T.Q.; Marian, V. Repetition reduction during word and concept overlap in bilinguals. J. Mem. Lang. 2015, 84, 88–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bard, E.; Anderson, A.; Sotillo, C.; Aylett, M.; Doherty-Sneddon, G.; Newlands, A. Controlling the intelligibility of referring expressions in dialogue. J. Mem. Lang. 2000, 42, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bard, E.G.; Aylett, M.P. Referential form, word duration, and modelling the listener in spoken dialogue. In Approaches to Studying World Situated Language Use: Bridging the Language-as-Product and Language-as-Action Traditions; Trueswell, J.C., Tanenhaus, M.K., Eds.; MIT: Cambridge, MA USA, 2004; pp. 173–191. [Google Scholar]
- Fowler, C. Differential Shortening of Repeated Content Words Produced in Various Communicative Contexts. Lang. Speech 1988, 31, 307–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fowler, C.; Levy, E.; Brown, J. Reductions of spoken words in certain discourse contexts. J. Mem. Lang. 1997, 37, 24–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahn, J.; Arnold, J.A. Processing-Centered look at the contribution of givenness to durational reduction. J. Mem. Lang. 2012, 67, 311–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trón, V. On the durational reduction of repeated mentions: Recency and speaker effects. In Proceedings of the LREC European Language Resources 2008 Association, Marrakech, Morocco, 26 May–1 June 2008; pp. 2777–2780. [Google Scholar]
- Lam, T.; Watson, D. Repetition is easy: Why repeated referents have reduced prominence. Mem. Cognit. 2010, 38, 1137–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Greenberg, S.; Hollenback, J.; Ellis, D. Insights into spoken language gleaned from phonetic transcription of the Switchboard corpus. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 3–6 October 1996; pp. S32–S35. [Google Scholar]
- McKoon, G.; Ratcliff, R. The comprehension processes and memory structures involved in anaphoric reference. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980, 19, 668–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, A.; Santesteban, M.; Caño, A. On the facilitatory effects of cognate words in bilingual speech production. Brain Lang. 2005, 94, 94–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christoffels, I.K.; Firk, C.; Schiller, N.O. Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study. Brain Res. 2007, 1147, 192–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Costa, A.; Caramazza, A.; Sebastian-Galles, N. The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2000, 26, 1283–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dijkstra, T.; Miwa, K.; Brummelhuis, B.; Sappelli, M.; Baayen, H. How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. J. Mem. Lang. 2010, 62, 284–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gollan, T.H.; Acenas, L.A.R. What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and tagalog-English bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2004, 30, 246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kroll, J.F.; Dijkstra, A.; Janssen, N.; Schriefers, H. Selecting the language in which to speak: Experiments on lexical access in bilingual production. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA, USA, 16–19 November 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Van Assche, E.; Duyck, W.; Hartsuiker, R.J.; Diependaele, K. Does bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in a sentence context. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 923–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Hell, J.G.; De Groot, A.M. Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Biling. Lang. Cognit. 1998, 1, 193–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hoshino, N.; Kroll, J.F. Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition 2008, 106, 501–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, R.; Baese-Berk, M.; Bonnasse-Gahot, L.; Kim, M.; Van Engen, K.; Bradlow, A. Word durations in non-native English. J. Phon. 2011, 39, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Forster, K.I.; Forster, J.C. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behav. Res. Methods 2003, 35, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Snodgrass, J.G.; Vanderwart, M. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1980, 6, 174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szekely, A.; Jacobsen, T.; D’Amico, S.; Devescovi, A.; Andonova, E.; Herron, D.; Lu, C.; Pechmann, T.; Pléh, C.; Wicha, N.; et al. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. J. Mem. Lang. 2004, 51, 247–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guasch, M.; Boada, R.; Ferré, P.; Sánchez-Casas, R. NIM: A web-based swiss army knife to select stimuli for psycholinguistic studies. Behav. Res. Methods 2012, 45, 765–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. (Eds.) Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer, Version 5.0. 30. 2008.
- Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Dai, B. (Eds.) lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using S4 Classes, Version 0.999375-28. 2008.
- Baayen, R.H.; Davidson, D.J.; Bates, D.M. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 390–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Galati, A.; Brennan, S. Attenuating information in spoken communication: For the speaker, or for the addressee? J. Mem. Lang. 2010, 62, 35–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lam, T. The Prominence of Referring Expressions: Message and Lexical Level Effects. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Meuter, R.; Allport, A. Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection. J. Mem. Lang. 1999, 40, 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Costa, A.; Santesteban, M. Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. J. Mem. Lang. 2004, 50, 491–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraundorf, S.; Watson, D.; Benjamin, A. Reduction in prosodic prominence predicts speakers’ recall: Implications for theories of prosody. Lang. Cognit. Neurosci. 2014, 30, 606–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bard, E.G.; Aylett, M.P. The dissociation of deaccenting, givenness, and syntactic role in spontaneous speech. In Proceedings of the 1999 International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, San Francisco, CA, USA, 1–7 August 1999; pp. 1753–1756. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, P.; Dell, G. Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments. Cognit. Psychol. 1987, 19, 441–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lockridge, C.B.; Brennan, S.E. Addressees’ needs influence speakers’ early syntactic choices. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002, 9, 550–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
DURATION | INTENSITY | PITCH | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | EST | SE | t-value | EST | SE | t-value | EST | SE | t-value |
(Intercept) | 468.5 | 12.9 | 36 | 68.4 | 0.5 | 120 | 178 | 7.8 | 22 |
Mention1 (M1) vs. Mention2 (M2) | −22.9 | 3.7 | −6.1 | −0.7 | 0.1 | −5 | −4.8 | 1.7 | −2 |
Mention NS (MNS) vs. Mention SW (MSW) | 11.9 | 5.3 | 2.2 | 0.06 | 0.2 | 0.3 | 1.2 | 2.4 | 0.5 |
Cognate (Cogn) | 13.1 | 14.4 | 0.9 | 0.3 | 0.2 | 1.3 | 2.1 | 1.8 | 1.1 |
M1 vs. M2: Cogn | 1.3 | 7.5 | 0.1 | −0.02 | 0.2 | −0.08 | −0.6 | 2.6 | −0.2 |
MNS vs. MSW: Cogn | −17.4 | 10.7 | −1.6 | 0.1 | 0.4 | 0.3 | 3.2 | 3.8 | 0.8 |
Random effects | VAR | SD | VAR | SD | |||||
Items | 5099 | 71.4 | 0.68 | 0.8 | 29.8 | 5.4 | |||
Subjects | 3421 | 58.4 | 9.6 | 3.1 | 1881 | 43.3 | |||
Residual | 7421 | 86.15 | 11.6 | 3.4 | 954 | 30.8 |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Rodriguez-Cuadrado, S.; Baus, C.; Costa, A. On the Nature of the Word-Reduction Phenomenon: The Contribution of Bilingualism. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 294. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9110294
Rodriguez-Cuadrado S, Baus C, Costa A. On the Nature of the Word-Reduction Phenomenon: The Contribution of Bilingualism. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(11):294. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9110294
Chicago/Turabian StyleRodriguez-Cuadrado, Sara, Cristina Baus, and Albert Costa. 2019. "On the Nature of the Word-Reduction Phenomenon: The Contribution of Bilingualism" Brain Sciences 9, no. 11: 294. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9110294
APA StyleRodriguez-Cuadrado, S., Baus, C., & Costa, A. (2019). On the Nature of the Word-Reduction Phenomenon: The Contribution of Bilingualism. Brain Sciences, 9(11), 294. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9110294