Sharing Perceptual Experiences through Language
Abstract
:1. Introduction
[A fire is] a terrestrial event with flames and fuel. It is a source of four kinds of stimulation, since it gives off sound, odor, heat and light […] One can hear it, smell it, feel it, and see it, or get any combination of these detections, and thereby perceive a fire. […] For this event, the four kinds of stimulus information and the four perceptual systems are equivalent. If the perception of fire were a compound of separate sensations of sound, smell, warmth and color, they would have had to be associated in past experience in order to explain how any one of them could evoke memories of all the others.
How does the room you’re sitting in make you feel? What is it about the soaring roof of a railway station, the damp odour of a cellar, the feel of worn stone steps beneath your feet, the muffled echo of a cloister or the cosy familiarity of your lounge that elicits glee, misery, fear or contentment? We’ve tasked seven architects with reawakening our visitors’ sensibilities to the spaces around them—bringing to the fore the experiential qualities of architecture.1
2. The Advent of Embodied Cognition
A lake thought re-activates areas of visual cortex that respond to visual information corresponding to lakes; areas of auditory cortex that respond to auditory information corresponding to lakes; areas of motor cortex that correspond to actions typically associated with lakes (although this activation is suppressed so that it does not lead to actual motion), and so on. The result is a lake concept that reflects the kinds of sensory and motor activities that are unique to human bodies and sensory systems. Lake means something like “thing that looks like this, sounds like this, smells like this, allows me to swim within it like this.”
3. Explaining Built Space
3.1. Data
3.2. Word Meanings within and across Sense Modalities
- Juxtapositions of sleek finishes [inside the building] such as citrus-colored partitions and tiny halogen spotlights feel cacophonous against the rough timber walls and columns.3
- Tucked behind two refurbished cottages that now serve as flexible office and guest quarters, [the house] is a cacophony of pitched roofs, steel awnings and crisp eyelid-like window hoods.
- [Chief architect] recalls, “They said, ‘Maybe this building could be a little quieter.’” [And they] set to work defining just what “quiet” could mean, esthetically speaking. “We talked a lot about trying […] to make it a quiet place.” That discussion soon led to ideas of garden, the metaphor that began to inform their design studies. […] nonliteral notions of garden did more to germinate this inventive building’s abstract qualities as a salve for the sensory whipping delivered by its suburban context. […] The building’s main event is inside […] the architects created an alternating rhythm of angled surfaces to bounce light and disperse sound. […] the sparsely landscaped lawn was […] a way to buffer the noise, both aural and visual, that is certain to kick in when the adjacent corner lot becomes a gas station or convenience store.
- 4.
- Since concert halls are large open spaces, they present opportunities for sound abrasions and acoustic glare.
- 5.
- The zigzag channels are made from aluminum that is perforated to achieve acoustic transparency […] the most unusual feature [is] as a set of velour curtains that hang between the acoustically transparent skin of the auditorium and the concrete outer wall.
- 6.
- Acoustic shadow created by Podium Building; as a result of the angle created between the source and podium edge, a portion of the facade is acoustically shaded.
- 7.
- Texture has various meanings […] Optical texture could be given by the organization of architectural elements, such as windows, doors, solids or voids. The repetition of elements creates a pattern that is observed as an optical texture. Tactile texture on the other hand could be given by building materials, such as concrete, brick, stone, glass, steel etc. [You can achieve both, as] In Baker House, in addition to a visual rhythm, Alvar Alto has used rough clinker brick to be able to give the building a tactile texture. Moreover, he had the bricks laid in a random pattern to add visual texture. [www.archplea121.cankaya.edu.tr] (accessed on 1 January 2023)
- 8.
- S1: Yes and also with this [taps image provided a visual stimulus] … with this thing is the idea of … of a soft and hard places.S2: yes it’s like soft this is more of soft [points to sketch they’re drawing]S1: yeah there you have more hard and here have maybe this …S3: and why would we split it up in soft and hard…it could be scattered around?S1: yeah yeah … like the main [inaudible] so this one would be hard anyway otherwise…
- 9.
- [student takes visual stimulus] four was interesting ‘cause it seem like, er, this contrast between very dense, this compact cluster [draws a cluster plus some scattered dots], but then it’s like a very contrast with this very open area, so this show contrast between density and then open [inaudible] which is more expansive
- 10.
- Texture can make or break a structure or building when it comes to design. It can be a crucial part or architecture, creating pattern or rhythm and allowing the viewer to believe the piece moves through space. Textures create a different experience; they allow more than one sense to be used at once by just “seeing” it. Textures allow viewers see the building as well as imagine how it would feel.[https://blogs.lt.vt.edu/kristen3/2013/02/08/texture-in-architecture] (accessed on 1 January 2023)
3.3. Fictive Motion and Built Spaces
- 11.
- The […] volume becomes the dominant form reaching out to the natural landscape. […] to the west are the information/ticket desk, the museum shop and a cafe which opens out onto a public terrace and the reflecting pool. A steel framed glazed canopy extends out from the vertical fissure […] A ramp climbs up the curved east wall of the void, arriving at the critical crossing point […]. Suites of enfilade double-height galleries step up the building in four split levels […] The underlying order of the building cannot be understood from a single vantage point, but unfolds cinematically as you move through a landscape of interior space. This is an architecture of promenade, yet without a prescribed or privileged route. Multiple lifts, stairs and ramps combine with the split-level galleries to create many possible itineraries. Passage between rooms occurs in a zigzag trajectory […]. Circulation always returns to the central orienting void. […] the wall also rotates from a 9.5-degree outward tilt at its southern end […]. The north end of the building twists towards the west […] Much of the daylight in the building […] is diffused by translucent glass which both intensifies the weak Nordic light and imparts a sense of quiet abstraction and detachment from the life of the city. So movement through the building becomes an introverted journey.
- 12.
- The massive warehouse runs along the north side of site.
- 13.
- The roof cantilevers towards the street through two traffic lights.
- 14.
- The green prism crouching.
- 15.
- The garden, which rambles into the house through a number of small courtyards, is an apt reminder of how a home should be occupied.
- 16.
- The interior open library wraps around staff spaces, a large community room, small meeting spaces and building services.
- 17.
- This sumptuously landscaped park in Santa Monica includes […] ramps that clamber over a drought-defying fountain, and […].
- 18.
- Customers descend to the store from the parking levels by elevators or by stairs that scissor down through the three-story space.
- 19.
- At north and south ends are stands for hardier (and poorer) fans, unshaded and raking up at a steep angle.
- 20.
- Here, the walls curve gently backwards until they get to the seventh floor, where they crank quite severely back to obey planning profile rules.
- 21.
- The driveway slips in under the west courtyard wall and then ramps up steeply to the entrance level.
- 22.
- I think you should combine these two into one design because it’s also the same shape like it starts very small and then curves around …
- 23.
- so [starts drawing boxy things] what I thought is sort of a box was coming out of the plinths, it was sticking through, the entry is maybe is here, and in the box there is the canteen...
- 24.
- so I kept this as the entrance to the building and sloping out [students start a discussion on entrances to the building].
The temple complex […] seems to have been built around the act of climbing. There, thousands of feet above the valley floor, a flat plaza was made from which each temple was entered, up a flight of steps, then down, then up again higher to the special place. To arrive at the largest temple, one went up, then down, then up, then down, then farther up again. […] getting there is all the fun. (Italics in the original)
I confront the city with my body; my legs measure the length of the arcade and the width of the square; my gaze unconsciously projects my body onto the facade of the cathedral, where it roams over the mouldings and contours, sensing the size of recesses and projections; my body weight meets the mass of the cathedral door, and my hand grasps the door pull as I enter the dark void behind. I experience myself in the city, and the city exists through my embodied experience. The city and my body supplement and define each other. I dwell in the city and the city dwells in me.
- 25.
- Chinese architect Li Xiaodong is not seen or experienced as an object in space. Instead, it builds upon the sequential experience of visiting the Academy—via courtyard, town-palace, grand staircase and Beaux-Arts gallery […] Experienced as a choreographed one-way route, the timber frame is […] An acrylic raised floor is illuminated by LEDs, and plywood-lined niches provide accents along the route, culminating in [what the architect likens to] a Zen garden […] presented as the final scene on a route that the architect likens to ‘a walk through a forest in the snow at night’. […] In this place, visitors are invited on a journey of discovery and to sense that alternative worlds run alongside their path and intersect with it. They can experience the different spaces, from the narrow passageways and intimate niches, to the expansive Zen garden. […] this is an unfolding story that is best moved through slowly and appreciated over time.
- Describe what you experienced walking through this installation.
- Did you feel differently in the passageways and niches?
- How did you react when you emerged in the Zen Garden?
- 26.
- one of my first ideas and my favourite one was […] different, er, paths, so, actually, to make this as the pathways and then have hills in between them, so to create like a hills landscape that would define different spaces that would become much freer and more playful] and give opportunities for use.
- 27.
- ST 3 starts drawing on ST2’s sketch and says: what I got from this was something different actually, er, what I got was like this centre point also this sort of axis … that comes up and from here I got this central axis, secondary axis here … so it’s more like directionality with a frame circulation […].
- 28.
- ‘cause this is more a, a, a route, also from this side and, I don’t know, what I see here like city facilities but more, is more about the route and the, the orientation of the paths which are going into, er, which are letting you in the building”
- 29.
- ST1: “I really thought literally of the rollercoaster and saw this element going up and down and around.
- 30.
- S1: You said two things about navigation … I think that is forced navigationS3: if you have activities then that is the circulation that you want to reach” [3 clarifies the other idea and 1 agrees].S2: if you make a connection to the park and it’s good to put these trees also in this…pathways […] you have other ways of entering this public space […] you can also like dictate one major access that all the flows go through.
- 31.
- Depositing their coats at the counter to the left, [people] turn to start on one of the three batteries of stairs rising to higher levels, going to left or right depending on destination. As they rise through the building, each new stair invites them to the next stage of the promenade until they find the appropriate level for their seat. Unprecedented is the delightful sloping route along the south edge on top of the lorry ramp, which is treated as a series of very long steps [...]. It winds irresistibly round, gathering more stair connections as it goes, and culminates in a double-level bar and restaurant [...]. Further stairs within this volume lead to an upper bar level and to a whole additional foyer leading back the other way to another bar above the entrance. The sequence of spaces–every bit a contrived promenade architecturale–is enriched by careful framing of views with various scales of window. Like the Philharmonic in Berlin [...] it provides a kind of internal landscape of interacting layers where the people of Rotterdam can parade in their finery to see and be seen, creating a theatre of the interval almost as important as that of the stage.
4. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | https://www.royalacademy.org.uk/exhibition/sensing-spaces (accessed on 1 January 2023). |
2 | Clearly, there are also meanings in language that are less sensory than the meanings we deal with here, for instance, more abstract shell concepts such us “idea”, “politics” “news” and the like. |
3 | Unless otherwise indicated, all the examples discussed in this chapter belong to the 150-text corpus described at the beginning of this section. |
4 | In De Anima, Aristotle offered a view of motion as a complex, holistic sense referred to as common sensible and encompassing physical properties indirectly perceived by more than one sense. This classical notion is revamped by contemporary anthropologists (Howes 2009, 2010) and literary scholars (Heller-Roazen 2007) claiming the existence of a holistic sense called common sense, which unifies and coordinates the other—acknowledged—five senses and their physical instantiations or expressions. |
References
- Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2020. Challenges and opportunities for grounding cognition. Journal of Cognition 3: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beveridge, Martin, and Madeleine Pickering. 2013. Perspective taking in language: Integrating the spatial and action domains. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Burro Roberto, Torquati Stefania, and Ugo Savardi. 2013. The middle of the road: Perceiving intermediates. Acta Psychologica 144: 121–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Paradis Carita, Roberto Burro, Joost van de Weijer, Nyström Marcus, and Ugo Savardi. 2017. Identification of poles and intermediates by eye and by hand. Acta Psychologica 180: 175–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Ugo Savardi, and Michael Kubovy. 2011. Dimensions and their poles: A metric and topological theory of opposites. Language and Cognitive Processes 26: 1232–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blomberg, Johan, and Jordan Zlatev. 2014. Actual and nonactual motion: Why experientialist semantics needs phenomenology (and vice versa). Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13: 395–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomer, Kent, and Charles Moore, eds. 1977. Body, Memory, and Architecture. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Borghi, Anna M., and Felice Cimatti. 2010. Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body. Neuropsychologia 48: 763–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caballero, Rosario. 2006. Re-Viewing Space. Figurative Language in Architects’ Assessment of Built Space. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero, Rosario. 2009. Dynamic predicates in English architectural discourse. In Metonymy and Metaphor in Grammar. Edited by K.-U. Panther, Thornburg Linda and Barcelona Antonio. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 277–90. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero, Rosario. 2017. Buildings that move: Motion metaphors in architectural reviews. Ibérica 34: 89–110. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero, Rosario. 2020. Sensory landscapes. Crossmodal metaphors in architecture. In Performing Metaphoric Creativity across Modes and Contexts. Edited by Hidalgo-Downing Laura and Kraljevic Mujic Blanca. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 197–219. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero, Rosario, and Carita Paradis. 2013. Perceptual landscapes from the perspective of cultures and genres. In Sensuous Cognition. Explorations into Human Sentience: Imagination, (E)motion and Perception. Edited by Caballero Rosario and Diaz-Vera Javier. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 77–107. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero, Rosario, and Carita Paradis. 2015. Making sense of sensory perceptions across languages and cultures. Functions of Language 22: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Caballero, Rosario, and Carita Paradis. 2018. Verbs in speech framing expressions: Comparing English and Spanish. Journal of Linguistics 54: 45–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Caballero, Rosario, and Carita Paradis. 2020. Soundscapes in English and Spanish: A corpus investigation of verb constructions. Language and Cognition 12: 705–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caballero, Rosario, Suarez-Toste Ernesto, and Carita Paradis. 2019. Representing Wine. Sensory Perceptions, Communication and Cultures. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Casakin, Hernan. 2019. Metaphors as discourse interaction devices in architectural design. Buildings 9: 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Connell, Louise, and Dermot Lynott. 2010. Look but don’t touch: Tactile disadvantage in processing modality–specific words. Cognition 115: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Damasio, Antonio. 1989. Concepts in the brain. Mind and Language 4: 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fodor, Jerry A. 1975. The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, Peter. 2000. Conceptual Spaces. The Geometry of Thought. Cambridge, MA and London: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, Peter. 2018. From sensations to concepts: A proposal for two learning processes. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 10: 441–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geeraerts, Dirk. 2010. Theories of Lexical Semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Geeraerts, Dirk. 2018. Ten Lectures on Cognitive Sociolinguistics. Leiden: Brill. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs, Raymond. 2006. Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation. Mind and Language 21: 434–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibbs, Raymond. 2011. Metaphoric imagery as embodied cognition. Journal of Mental Imagery 35: 42–48. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, James J. 1966. The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston: Houghton Mufflin. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, Robert F., Charles A. Perfetti, and Walter Schneider. 2006. Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 6: 214–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hartman, Jenny, and Carita Paradis. 2021. Figurative meaning in multimodal work by an autistic artist: A cognitive semantic approach. Language and Cognition 13: 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heller-Roazen, Daniel. 2007. The Inner Touch. Archaeology of a Sensation. New York: Zone Books. [Google Scholar]
- Hörberg, Thomas, Larsson Maria, Ekström Ingrid, Sandöy Camilla Lundén Peter, and Jonas Olofsson. 2020. Olfactory influences on visual categorization: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Cerebral Cortex 30: 4220–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Howes, David, ed. 2009. The Sixth Sense Reader. Oxford and New York: Berg. [Google Scholar]
- Howes, David. 2010. Sensual Relations: Engaging the Senses in Culture and Social Theory. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. [Google Scholar]
- James, William. 1890. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt. [Google Scholar]
- Jędrzejowski, Lukasz, and Przemyslaw Staniewski, eds. 2021. The Linguistics of Olfaction. Typological and Diachronic Approaches to Synchronic Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Kemmerer, David. 2015. Are the motor features of verb meanings represented in the precentral motor cortices? Yes, but within the context of a flexible, multilevel architecture for conceptual knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 22: 1068–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kennedy, Christopher, and Louise McNally. 2005. Scale structure, degree modification, and the semantics of gradable predicates. Language 81: 345–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria, and Dmitry Nikolaev. 2021. Talking About Temperature and Social Thermoregulation in the Languages of the World. International Review of Social Psychology 34: 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria. 2022. Talking temperature with close relatives. Semantic systems across Slavic languages. In The Typology of Physical Qualities. Edited by Rakhilina Ekaterin, Reznikova Tatiana and Ryzhova Daria. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 215–68. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1980. Metaphors We Live by. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic books. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, Ronald. 1986. Abstract motion. Proceedings of the Twelfth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 12: 455–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Langacker, Ronald. 1987. Foundation of Cognitive Grammar. Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, Ronald. 2000. Virtual reality. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 29: 77–103. [Google Scholar]
- Louwerse, Max. 2018. Knowing the meaning of a word by the linguistic and perceptual company it keeps. Topics in Cognitive Science 10: 573–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Louwerse, Max, and Louise Connell. 2011. A Taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science 35: 381–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynott, Dermot, and Louise Connell. 2009. Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties. Behavior Research Methods 41: 558–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lynott, Dermot, and Louise Connell. 2013. Modality exclusivity norms for 400 nouns: The relationship between perceptual experience and surface word form. Behavior Research Methods 45: 516–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynott, Dermot, Connell Louise, and Marc Brysbaert. 2020. The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: Multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words. Behavior Research 52: 1271–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Matlock, Teenie. 2004. The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In Motivation in Grammar. Edited by Radden Günter and Dirven René. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 221–48. [Google Scholar]
- Matlock, Teenie. 2010. Abstract motion is no longer abstract. Language and Cognition 2: 243–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matlock, Teenie. 2017. Metaphor, Simulation, and Fictive Motion. In The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Edited by Dancygier Barbara. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 477–90. [Google Scholar]
- Matlock, Teenie, and Till Bergmann. 2015. Fictive motion. In Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Edited by Dabrowska Ewa and Dagmar Divjak. Berlin: Mouton, pp. 546–62. [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto, Yo. 1996. Subjective motion in English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics 7: 183–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olofsson, Jonas K., Ekström Ingrid, Joanna Lindström, Elmeri Syrjänen, Anna Stigsdotter-Neely, and Lars Nyberg. 2020. Smell-based memory training: Evidence of olfactory learning and transfer to the visual domain. Chemical Senses 45: 593–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pallasmaa, Juhani. 2005. The Eyes of the Skin. Architecture and the Senses. Chichester: Wiley-Academy. First published 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Carita. 2015a. Meanings of words: Theory and application. In Handbuch Wort und Wortschatz (Handbücher Sprachwissen-HSW Band 3). Edited by Hass Ulrike and Storjohann Petra. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 274–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Carita. 2015b. Conceptual spaces atyes work in sensuous cognition: Domains, dimensions and distances. In Applications of Conceptual Spaces: The Case of Geometric Knowledge Representation. Edited by Zenker Frank and Peter Gärdenfors. Dordrecht: Springer Verlag, pp. 33–55. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Carita, and Caroline Willners. 2011. Antonymy: From convention to meaning-making. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 9: 367–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Carita, Caroline Willners, and Steven Jones. 2009. Good and bad opposites: Using textual and experimental techniques to measure antonym canonicity. The Mental Lexicon 4: 380–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Carita, Hudson Jean, and Ulf Magnusson, eds. 2013. The Construal of Spatial Meaning: Windows into Conceptual Space. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pecher, Diane, Zeelenberg René, and Lawrence Barsalou. 2003. Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science 14: 119–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pecher, Diane, and Rolf Zwaan. 2005. Introduction to grounding cognition. In Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Edited by Pecher Diane and Zwaan Rolf. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Pecher, Diane, Zeelenberg René, and Lawrence Barsalou. 2004. Sensorimotor simulations underlie conceptual representations: Modality-specific effects of prior activation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 11: 164–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pulvermüller, Friedemann. 2005. Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 576–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, Steen Eller. 1959. Experiencing Architecture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sathian, Krishnankutty, and Vilawanur Ramachandran, eds. 2020. Multisensory Perception: From Laboratory to Clinic. London: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Seamon, David. 2007. A lived hermetic of people and place: Phenomenology and space syntax. Paper presented at 6th International Space Syntax Symposium, İstanbul, Turkey, June 12–15; pp. 12–15. [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro, Lawrence, and Shannon Spaulding. 2021. Embodied Cognition. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Winter 2021 edition. Edited by Edward N. Zalta. Available online: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2021/entries/embodied-cognition/ (accessed on 1 January 2023).
- Speed, Laura J., and Asifa Majid. 2020. Grounding language in the neglected senses of touch, taste, and smell. Cognitive Neuropsychology 37: 363–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, Charles. 2020. Designing for the Multisensory Mind. Architectural Design 90: 42–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Talmy, Leonard. 1983. How Language Structures Space. Berkeley: Cognitive Science Program, Institute of Cognitive Studies, University of California at Berkeley, pp. 225–82. [Google Scholar]
- Talmy, Leonard. 1988. Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Talmy, Leonard. 1996. Fictive motion in language and ‘ception. In Language and Space. Edited by Bloom Paul, Garrett Merrill, Nadel Lynn and Mary A. Peterson. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 211–76. [Google Scholar]
- Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Conceptual Structuring Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
- Talmy, Leonard. 2003. Concept structuring systems in language. In The New Psychology of Language. Edited by Michael Tomasello. Hove: Psychology Press, pp. 29–60. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2008. The Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, Rosario, Garagnani Max, Wennekers Thomas, and Friedamann Pulvermüller. 2018. A neurobiologically constrained cortex model of semantic grounding with spiking neurons and brain-like connectivity. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 12: 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turatto, Massimo, Galfano Giovanni, Bridgeman Bruce, and Carlo Umiltà. 2004. Space-independent modality-driven attentional capture in auditory, tactile and visual systems. Experimental Brain Research 155: 301–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van de Weijer, Joost, Ivana Bianchi, and Carita Paradis. 2023. Sensory modality profiles of antonyms. Language and Cognition, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van de Weijer, Joost, Paradis Carita, Willners Caroline, and Magnus Lindgren. 2014. Antonym canonicity: Temporal and contextual manipulations. Brain and Language 128: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varela, Francisco J., Thompson Evan, and Eleanor Rosch. 2017. The Embodied Mind, Revised ed. Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Winter, Bodo. 2019. Sensory Linguistics. Language, Perception and Metaphor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Yudell, Robert. 1977. Body movement. In Body, Memory, and Architecture. Edited by Bloomer Kent and Moore Charles. New Haven: Yale University Press, pp. 57–76. [Google Scholar]
- Zlatev, Jordan, Blomberg Johan, and David Caroline. 2010. Translocation, language and the categorization of motion. In Language, Cognition, and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions. Edited by Evans Vyvyan and Chilton Paul. London: Equinox, pp. 389–418. [Google Scholar]
- Zumthor, Peter. 2006. Thinking Architecture. Basel: Birkhäuser. First published 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Zwaan, Rolf A. 2014. Embodiment and language comprehension: Reframing the discussion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 229–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
source sense → target sense |
---|
sound → sight quiet, mute(d), grandiloquent, hushed, jarring, cacophony, cacophonous, discordant, noise, tone down, resonate sound → sight + touch rhythm, rhythmic, beat, melody, orchestrate, choreograph |
sight → sound transparency, glare, shadow/shade |
touch → sound abrasions touch → sight crisp touch → touch + sight coarse, coarseness, grain, warm, warmth, cold, cool, fragile, hard, soft(en), light(en), tactile, coarse, weight, heavy, sharp |
taste → sight bland, insipid |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Caballero, R.; Paradis, C. Sharing Perceptual Experiences through Language. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 129. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070129
Caballero R, Paradis C. Sharing Perceptual Experiences through Language. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(7):129. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070129
Chicago/Turabian StyleCaballero, Rosario, and Carita Paradis. 2023. "Sharing Perceptual Experiences through Language" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 7: 129. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070129
APA StyleCaballero, R., & Paradis, C. (2023). Sharing Perceptual Experiences through Language. Journal of Intelligence, 11(7), 129. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070129