The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Same–Different
1.2. Similar, Different or Opposite?
1.3. A Mainstream Approach to Opposites as a Semantic Relationship and Some Recent Moves towards a Perceptually-Based Perspective
2. Study 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.1.4. Data Analyses
2.2. Results
3. Study 2
- –
- Simplicity: it is a relatively straightforward method that is easy to understand. The participants are shown two options and asked to make a judgment about which option is better in some way (e.g., which appears to be more “similar” to the standard figure). In this way, it is accessible to a wide range of individuals and minimizes the potential for confusion or bias.
- –
- Elimination of absolute scales: Unlike rating scales or Likert-type scales that require participants to assign a numerical value or rate each option individually, paired comparison tasks focus on relative judgments. Participants only need to compare two options at a time, which simplifies the decision-making process and reduces cognitive load. This approach can help overcome potential biases associated with absolute scales and facilitate more accurate and meaningful comparisons.
- –
- Improved discrimination: Paired comparisons can enhance the sensitivity and discrimination of judgments. By presenting options in pairs, participants are forced to make direct comparisons and identify the relative differences between the options. This can lead to more precise rankings especially when comparing complex or nuanced stimuli.
- –
- Reduced response biases: Traditional rating scales can be subject to various response biases, such as central tendency bias (tendency to select neutral or middle options) or acquiescence bias (tendency to agree with statements). The paired comparison task can minimize these biases by asking the participant to focus on relative judgments.
- –
- Robustness: The paired comparison task is known for its robustness and its adaptability to a wide range of stimuli and contexts.
- –
- Quantifiable results: Paired comparison tasks provide data that can be easily quantified and analyzed. The relative rankings obtained from participants’ judgments can be statistically analyzed to determine the overall preferences or rankings of the options being compared.
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Materials
3.1.3. Procedure
3.1.4. Data Analyses
3.2. Results
4. Final Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | On the debate regarding whether there are qualitative differences between the underlying processes adopted by human and non-human species, see for instance (Carstensen and Frank 2021; Gentner et al. 2021; Katz and Wright 2021; Kroupin and Carey 2021; Smirnova et al. 2021; Pepperberg 2021). |
2 | There is an example of this in the structure-mapping theory of comparison (e.g., Markman and Gentner 1993). According to this model, the processing related to comparisons involves structural alignment and mapping between two representations. This facilitates the grasp of structural commonalities and thus also the application and extension of previously acquired knowledge to new instances (Gentner and Medina 1998). |
3 | An interesting exception was found by Medin et al. (1990) who analyzed the influence of relational qualities (e.g., the same color, or the right side smaller than the left side) and attributes (e.g., a triangular shape, or colored white) on people’s perception of similarity and diversity. They concluded that relational properties were more relevant when people were asked to judge similarities, while attributes were more relevant when they focused on differences (see also Markman 1996). |
4 | We have deliberately decided not to speak of 90° and 180° rotations since these would be geometrical rather than phenomenological descriptions. We wanted to keep the two types of changes in orientation distinct: (1) a change in the axis of orientation from vertical to horizontal and (2) a change in the direction or orientation applied while leaving the configuration within the same axis (i.e., upright-upside down with reference to the vertical axis). |
5 | In identity pairs, the only visible difference between the comparison figure and the standard figure concerned the localization of the two shapes (with the comparison figure to the right of the standard figure). We considered ratings given to these pairs as the baseline values for S, D and O associated with a mere replication of the same shape. The ratings given by participants to these pairs revealed that the difference in localization was irrelevant to the S judgments (i.e., the pair was rated as maximally similar) and the D judgments (i.e., the pair was rated as not different at all), while it was associated with a low O rating (the average rating for identity pairs 2.823). The latter result was due to the reciprocal position of the two shapes (one to the right, the other to the left), as the participants explained when asked specifically to comment on their response at the end of the experimental session. |
References
- Addyman, Caspar, and Denis Mareschal. 2010. The perceptual origins of the abstract same/different concept in human infants. Animal Cognition 13: 817–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, Erin M., Yin-Juei Chang, Susan Hespos, and Dedre Gentner. 2018. Comparison within pairs promotes analogical abstraction in three-month-olds. Cognition 176: 74–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arnheim, Rudolf. 1954. Art and Visual Perception. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, and Ugo Savardi. 2008a. The Perception of Contraries. Roma: Aracne. [Google Scholar]
- Bianchi, Ivana, and Ugo Savardi. 2008b. The relationship perceived between the real body and the mirror image. Perception 5: 666–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, and Ugo Savardi. 2014. Grounding naïve physics and optics in perception. The Baltic International Yearbook for Cognition Logic and Communication 6: 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Carita Paradis, Roberto Burro, Joost van de Weijer, Marcus Nyström, and Ugo Savardi. 2017a. Identification of opposites and intermediates by eye and by hand. Acta Psychologica 180: 175–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Marco Bertamini, and Ugo Savardi. 2015. Differences between predictions of how a reflection behaves based on the behaviour of an object. Acta Psychologica 161: 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Marco Bertamini, Roberto Burro, and Ugo Savardi. 2017b. Opposition and identicalness: Two basic components of adults’ perception and mental representation of Symmetry. Symmetry 9: 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Roberto Burro, Roberta Pezzola, and Ugo Savardi. 2017c. Matching Visual and Acoustic Mirror Forms. Symmetry 9: 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Ugo Savardi, and Michael Kubovy. 2011. Dimensions and their poles: A metric and topological theory of opposites. Language and Cognitive Processes 26: 1232–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Ugo Savardi, Roberto Burro, and Maria Francesca Martelli. 2014. Doing the opposite to what another person is doing. Acta Psychologica 151: 117–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, Ann L., and Mary Jo Kane. 1988. Preschool children can learn to transfer: Learning to learn and learning from example. Cognitive Psychology 20: 493–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, Michael F., and Justin M. Sayde. 2013. Same/different discrimination by bumblebee colonies. Animal Cognition 16: 117–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carstensen, Alexandra, and Michael C. Frank. 2021. Do graded representations support abstract thought. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Zhe, Rebecca P. Sanchez, and Tammy Campbell. 1997. From beyond to within their grasp: The rudiments of analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month olds. Developmental Psychology 33: 790–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cook, Robert G., and Daniel I. Brooks. 2009. Generalized auditory same-different discrimination by pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 35: 108–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cook, Robert G., and Muhammad A. J. Qadri. 2021. Examining the extents of same/different processing in non-human animals. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 98–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Critchlow, Douglas E., and Michael A. Fligner. 1991. Paired comparison, triple comparison, and ranking experiments as generalized linear models, and their implementation in GLIM. Psychometrika 56: 517–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Croft, William, and D. Alan Cruse. 2004. Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cruse, D. Alan. 1986. Lexical Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cruse, D. Alan. 2000. Meaning in Language: An Introduction to Semantics and Pragmatics. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- David, Herbert A. 1988. The Method of Paired Comparisons, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- De Lillo, Carlo, Giovanna Spinozzi, Valentina Truppa, and Donna N. Naylor. 2005. A comparative analysis of global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in young children (Homo sapiens) and monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 155–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deruelle, Christine, and Joel Fagot. 1998. Visual search for global/local stimulus features in humans and baboons. Psychonomic Bulletin Review 5: 476–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diaz, Francisca, Ellen M. O’Donoghue, and Edward A. Wasserman. 2021. Two-item conditional same-different categorization in pigeons: Finding differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Learning and Cognition 47: 455–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Domenici, Paolo, and Graeme D. Ruxton. 2015. Prey behaviors during fleeing: Escape trajectories, signaling, and sensory defenses. In Escaping from Predators. An Integrative View of Escape Decisions. Edited by William E. Cooper Jr. and Daniel T. Blumstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 199–224. [Google Scholar]
- Du, Chenguang, Yasuo Miyazaki, Michael Cook, Joanna Papadopoulos, and Yuan Ho. 2018. Relational language improves preschool children’s performance of analogical reasoning. International Journal of Psychological Studies 10: 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fellbaum, Christiane. 1995. Co–occurrence and antonymy. International Journal of Lexicography 8: 281–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferry, Alissa L., Susan J. Hespos, and Dedre Gentner. 2015. Prelinguistic relational concepts: Investigating analogical processing in infants. Child Develoment 86: 1386–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dedre, and Cecile Toupin. 1986. Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cognitive Science 10: 277–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dedre, and Jose Medina. 1998. Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65: 263–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dedre, Ruxue Shao, Nina Simms, and Susan Hespos. 2021. Learning same and different relations: Cross-species comparisons. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 84–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dendre. 2003. Why we’re so smart. In Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Edited by Dendre Gentner and Susan Goldin-Meadow. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 195–235. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner, Dendre. 2010. Bootstrapping the mind: Analogical processes and symbol systems. Cognitive Science 34: 752–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dendre, and Christian Hoyos. 2017. Analogy and Abstraction. Topics in Cognitive Science 9: 672–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dendre, and Kenneth Kurtz. 2006. Relations, objects, and the composition of analogies. Cognitive Science 30: 609–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dendre, Keith J. Holyoak, and Boicho N. Kokinov, eds. 2001. The Analogical Mind. Perspective from Cognitive Science. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner, Dendre, Mary J. Rattermann, and Kenneth D. Forbus. 1993. The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology 25: 524–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gescheider, George A. 2013. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals, 3rd ed. London: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa, Martin. 2021. Learning of sameness/difference relationships by honey bees: Performance, strategies and ecological context. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldmeier, Erich. 1972. Similarity in visually perceived forms. Psychological Issues 29: 1–136. First published 1936. [Google Scholar]
- Goldwater, Micah B., and Lennart Schalk. 2016. Relational categories as a bridge between cognitive and educational research. Psychological Bulletin 142: 729–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodwin, Geoffrey P., and Philip N. Johnson-Laird. 2005. Reasoning about relations. Psychological Review 112: 468–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goulet, Mark-André, and Denis Cousineau. 2020. Sequential sampling models of same-different data and how they explain the fast-same effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 74: 284–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, David M., and John A. Swets. 1966. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Halford, Graeme S., William H. Wilson, and Steven Phillips. 2010. Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 497–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hanel, Paul H. P., Gregory R. Maio, and Antony S. R. Manstead. 2019. A new way to look at the data: Similarities between groups of people are large and important. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 116: 541–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harding, Bradley, and Denis Cousineau. 2022. Is the Fast-Same Phenomenon That Fast? An Investigation of Identity Priming in the Same-Different Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition 48: 520–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hastie, Trevor, Robert Tibshirani, and Jerome Friedman. 2009. The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction, 2nd ed. Berlin: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Hermann, Douglas J., Roger Chaffin, Margaret P. Daniel, and Robert S. Wool. 1986. The role of elements of relation definition in antonym and synonym comprehension. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 194: 133–53. [Google Scholar]
- Hespos, Susan J., Erin Anderson, and Dedre Gentner. 2020. Structure mapping processes enable infants’ learning across domains, including language. In Language and Concept Acquisition from Infancy Through Childhood: Learning from Multiple Exemplars. Edited by Jane B. Childers. Cham: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Hespos, Susan, Dendre Gentner, Erin Anderson, and Apoorva Shivaram. 2021. The origins of same/different discrimination in human infants. Current Opinion in Behavioural Sciences 37: 69–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochmann, Jean Rémy, Arin S. Tuerk, Sophia Sanborn, Rebecca Zhu, Robert Long, Meg Dempster, and Susan Carey. 2017. Children’s representation of abstract relations in relational/array match-to-sample tasks. Cognitive Psychology 99: 17–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hochmann, Jean Rémy, Shilpa Mody, and Susan Carey. 2016. Infants’ representations of same and different in match-and non-match-to-sample. Cognitive Psychology 86: 87–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hochmann, Jean Rémy, Silvia Benavides-Varela, Ana Flo, Marina Nespor, and Jacques Mehler. 2018. Bias for vocalic over consonantal information in 6-month-olds. Infancy 23: 136–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochmann, Jean Rémy, Silvia Benavides-Varela, Marina Nespor, and Jacques Mehler. 2011. Consonants and vowels: Different roles in early language acquisition. Develomental Science 14: 1445–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochmann, Jean-Remy, Ed Wasserman, and Susan Carey. 2021. Editorial overview: Same-different conceptualization. Current Opinion in Behavioural Sciences 37: iii–v. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hodgetts, Carl J., James O. E. Close, and Ulrike Hahn. 2023. Similarity and structured representation in human and nonhuman apes. Cognition 236: 105419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holyoak, Keith J., and Kyunghee Koh. 1987. Surface and structural similarity in analogical transfer. Memory & Cognition 15: 332–40. [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins, William D., and David A. Washburn. 2002. Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition 5: 27–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, Steven. 2002. Antonymy: A Corpus–Based Perspective. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, Steven, Lynne Murphy, Carita Paradis, and Caroline Willners. 2012. Antonyms in English: Construals Constructions and Canonicity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kagan, Jerome. 1984. The Nature of the Child. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Kassambara, Alboukadel, and Fabian Mundt. 2020. Factoextra: Extract and Visualize the Results of Multivariate Data Analyses. R Package Version 1.0.7. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=factoextra (accessed on 1 June 2023).
- Kastak, David, and Ronald J. Schusterman. 1994. Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Animal Learning and Behavior 22: 427–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katz, Jeffrey S., and Anthony A. Wright. 2021. Issues in the comparative cognition of same/different abstract-concept learning. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaufman, Leonard, and Peter J. Rousseeuw. 1990. Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Kawabata, Yuuki, Hideyuki Akada, Ken-ichiro Shimatani, Gregory Naoki Nishihara, Hibiki Kimura, Nozomi Nishiumi, and Paolo Domenici. 2023. Multiple preferred escape trajectories are explained by a geometric model incorporating prey’s turn and predator attack endpoint. eLife 12: e77699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kingdom, Frederick A., and Nicolaas Prins. 2016. Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction. Cambridge: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kovács, Agnes M., and Ansgar D. Endress. 2014. Hierarchical processing in seven-month-old infants. Infancy 19: 409–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroupin, Ivan, and Susan Carey. 2021. Population differences in performance on Relational Match to Sample (RMTS) sometimes reflect differences in inductive biases alone. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazareva, Olga F., and Edward A. Wasserman. 2017. Categories and Concepts in Animals. In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, 2nd ed. Edited by John H. Byrne. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 111–39. [Google Scholar]
- Lenth, Russell V. 2023. Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, Aka Least-Squares Means. R Package Version 1.8.6. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans (accessed on 1 June 2023).
- Limesurvey GmbH. 2023. LimeSurvey: An Open Source Survey Tool. Hamburg: LimeSurvey GmbH. Available online: http://www.limesurvey.org (accessed on 1 February 2023).
- Lin, Po-Han, and Steven J. Luck. 2009. The Influence of Similarity on Visual Working Memory Representations. Visual Cognition 17: 356–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luce, R. Duncan, and John W. Tukey. 1964. Simultaneous conjoint measurement: A new type of fundamental measurement. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 1: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lüdecke, Daniel, Mattan S. Ben-Shachar, Indrajeet Patil, Philip Waggoner, and Dominique Makowski. 2021. Performance: An R Package for Assessment, Comparison and Testing of Statistical Models. Journal of Open Source Software 6: 3139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macmillan, Neil A., and C. Douglas Creelman. 2005. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide, 2nd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- MacQueen, James B. 1967. Some Methods for classification and Analysis of Multivariate Observations. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability 1: 281–97. [Google Scholar]
- Mareschal, Denis, Paul C. Quinn, and Stephen E.G. Lea, eds. 2010. The Making of Human Concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Markman, Arthur B. 1996. Structural alignment in similarity and difference. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3: 227–30. [Google Scholar]
- Markman, Arthur B., and Dedre Gentner. 1993. Splitting the Difference: A Structural Alignment View of Similarity. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 517–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinho, Antone, III, and Alex Kacelnik. 2016. Ducklings imprint on the relations concept of “same or different”. Science 353: 286–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Medin, Douglas, Robert L. Goldston, and Dedre Gentner. 1990. Similarity involving attributes and relations: Judgments of similarity and difference are not inverses. Psychological Science 1: 64–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mercado, Eduardo, III, Deidre A. Killebrew, Adam A. Pack, Inés V. B. Mácha, and Louis M. Herman. 2000. Generalization of ‘same- different’ classification abilities in bottlenosed dolphins. Behavioural Processes 50: 79–94. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, George A., and Christiane Fellbaum. 1991. Semantic networks of English. Cognition 41: 197–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murphy, Gregory L. 2002. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, Gregory L., and Jane M. Andrew. 1993. The Conceptual Basis of antonymy and synonymy in adjectives. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 301–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, M. Lynne. 2003. Semantic Relations and the Lexicon: Antonyms, Synonyms and Other Semantic Paradigms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Nosofsky, Robert M., and Thomas J. Palmeri. 1997. An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. Psychological Review 104: 266–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmer, Steven E. 1978. Structural aspects of visual similarity. Memory & Cognition 6: 91–97. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer, Steven E. 1983. The psychology of Perceptual Organization: A Transformational Approach. In Human and Machine Vision. Edited by Jacob Beck, Barbara Hope and Azriel Rosenfeld. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 269–339. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer, Steven E. 1999. Vision Science. Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Carita. 2016. Corpus methods for the investigation of antonyms across languages. In The Lexical Typology of Semantic Shifts. Edited by Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm and Päivi Juvonen. Cognitive linguistics research Series, 58. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 131–56. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Carita, Caroline Willners, and Steven Jones. 2009. Good and bad opposites Using textual and experimental techniques to measure antonym canonicity. The Mental Lexicon 4: 380–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Carita, Simone Löhndorf, Joost van de Weijer, and Caroline Willners. 2015. Semantic profiles of antonymic adjectives in discourse. Linguistics 53: 153–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penn, Derek C., Keith J. Holyoak, and Daniel J. Povinelli. 2008. Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioural and Brain Science 31: 109–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pepperberg, Irene M. 1987. Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Learning with respect to categories of color, shape, and material. Animal Learning & Behavior 15: 423–32. [Google Scholar]
- Pepperberg, Irene M. 2021. How do a pink plastic flamingo and a pink plastic elephant differ? Evidence for abstract representations of the relations same-different in a Grey parrot. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 146–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinger, Pia, Isabel Ruhmer-Krell, and Heiner Schumacher. 2016. The compromise effect in action: Lessons from a restaurant’s menu. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 128: 14–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2023. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 June 2023).
- Revelle, William. 2023. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. R Package Version 2.3.3. Evanston: Northwestern University. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 1 June 2023).
- Richland, Lindsey Engle, and Nina Simms. 2015. Analogy, higher order thinking, and education. WIREs Cognitive Science 6: 177–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rock, Irvin. 1973. Orientation and Form. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rosch, Eleanor, and Carolyn B. Mervis. 1975. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 7: 573–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ross, Brian H. 1987. This is like that: The use of earlier problems and the separation of similarity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13: 629–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scagel, Allison, and Eduardo Mercado III. 2023. Same-different conceptualization in dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology 137: 45–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smirnova, Anna A., Tatiana A. Obozova, Zoya A. Zorina, and Edward A. Wasserman. 2021. How do crows and parrots come to spontaneously perceive relations-between-relations? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 109–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Edward E., and Douglas L. Medin. 1981. Categories and Concepts. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, J. David, Joshua S. Redford, Sarah M. Haas, Mariana V. C. Coutinho, and Justin J. Couchman. 2008. The comparative psychology of same–different judgments by humans (Homo sapiens) and monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 34: 361–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Still, Susanne, and William Bialek. 2004. How many clusters? An information-theoretic perspective. Neural Computation 16: 2483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thurstone, Louis L. 1927. A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review 34: 273–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tibshirani, Robert, Guenther Walther, and Trevor Hastie. 2001. Estimating the number of clusters in a data set via the gap statistic. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology) 63: 411–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trench, Máximo, and Ricardo A. Minervino. 2015. The role of surface similarity in analogical retrieval: Bridging the gap between the naturalistic and the experimental traditions. Cognitive Science 39: 1292–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, Amos. 1977. Features of similarity. Psychological Review 84: 327–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyrrell, Donald J., Lori B. Stauffer, and Linda G. Snowman. 1991. Perception of abstract identity/difference relationships by infants. Infant Behavior and Development 14: 125–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van de Weijer, Joost, Ivana Bianchi, and Carita Paradis. 2023. Sensory modality profiles of antonyms. Language and Cognition, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicario, Giovanni Bruno. 1975. Some observations gestalt principles of organization. In Studies in Perception. Festschrift for Fabio Metelli. Edited by Giovanni B. Flores D’Arcais. Milano–Firenze: Martello Giunti, pp. 67–80. [Google Scholar]
- Wasserman, Edward A., and Michael E. Young. 2010. Same–different discrimination: The keel and backbone of thought and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 36: 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wasserman, Edward A., Leyre Castro, and Joël Fagot. 2017. Relational thinking in animals and humans: From percepts to concepts. In APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Perception, Learning, and Cognition. Edited by Josep Call, Gordon M. Burghardt, Irene M. Pepperberg, Charles T. Snowdon and Thomas Zentall. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 359–84. [Google Scholar]
- Wertheimer, Max. 1923. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt, II [Investigations of the principles of Gestalt, II]. Psychologische Forschung 4: 301–50, [condensed and translated by Ellis, Willis D. as Laws of organization in perceptual forms. In Willis D. Ellis (ed.) A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1939, pp. 71–88]. [Google Scholar]
- Wickelmaier, Florian, and Christian Schmid. 2004. A Matlab function to estimate choice model parameters from paired-comparison data. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 36: 29–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickham, Hadley. 2016. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, Anthony A., and Jeffrey S. Katz. 2006. Mechanisms of same/different concept learning in primates and avians. Behavioural Processes 72: 234–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, Anthony A., Jacquelyne J. Rivera, Jeffrey S. Katz, and Jocelyne Bachevalier. 2003. Abstractc oncept learning and list-memory processing by capuchin and rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 29: 184–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, Michael E., and Edward A. Wasserman. 2002. Detecting variety: What’s so special about sameness? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 131: 131–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zentall, Thomas R. 2021. Sameness may be a natural concept that does not require learning. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 37: 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Zhiyong, and Yujiao Mai. 2023. WebPower: Basic and Advanced Statistical Power Analysis. R Package Version 0.9.3. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=WebPower (accessed on 1 June 2023).
Transformations | Matched Ratings | Est | SE | z Ratio | O vs. D | O vs. S | O Different from Both S and D | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Size | O | D | −0.514 | 0.146 | −3.524 | ns | O < S | |
Size | O | S | −5.958 | 0.147 | −40.512 *** | |||
Axis | O | D | 1.439 | 0.148 | 9.702 *** | O > D | O < S | X |
Axis | O | S | −5.211 | 0.147 | −35.34 *** | |||
size + axis | O | D | −0.517 | 0.145 | −3.573 | ns | O < S | |
size + axis | O | S | −3.451 | 0.146 | −23.672 *** | |||
shape + size | O | D | −2.991 | 0.145 | −20.585 *** | O < D | O < S | X |
shape + size | O | S | −0.912 | 0.145 | −6.288 *** | |||
Shape | O | D | −1.066 | 0.145 | −7.361 *** | O < D | O < S | X |
Shape | O | S | −1.592 | 0.145 | −10.994 *** | |||
Direction | O | D | 1.602 | 0.146 | 10.986 *** | O > D | O < S | X |
Direction | O | S | −4.338 | 0.147 | −29.493 *** | |||
shape+size+axis | O | D | −1.184 | 0.145 | −8.183 *** | O < D | O > S | X |
shape+size+axis | O | S | 1.550 | 0.145 | 10.715 *** | |||
shape+axis | O | D | −1.304 | 0.145 | −9.006 *** | O < D | O > S | X |
shape+axis | O | S | 1.135 | 0.145 | 7.850 *** | |||
size+direction | O | D | 2.968 | 0.145 | 20.486 *** | O > D | O < S | X |
size+direction | O | S | −0.776 | 0.145 | −5.347 *** | |||
shape+direction | O | D | 0.143 | 0.145 | 0.986 | ns | O > S | |
shape+direction | O | S | 1.786 | 0.145 | 12.349 *** | |||
shape+size+direction | O | D | −0.579 | 0.145 | −3.982 * | O < D | O > S | X |
shape+size+direction | O | S | 3.194 | 0.145 | 22.085 *** |
EST (Mean Difference) | Cohen’s d (Effect Size) | z-Ratio (p-Value) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Transformations | O | D | S | O | D | S | O | D | S |
Size | −1.019 | 2.354 | −2.145 | −0.426(3) | 0.984 (1) | −0.897(2) | −7.056 *** | 16.251 *** | −14.588 *** |
Axis | 0.060 | 1.477 | −1.811 | 0.025(3) | 0.618(2) | −0.758(1) | 0.417 | 9.986 *** | −12.229 *** |
size+axis | 0.278 | 3.654 | −3.357 | 0.116(3) | 1.528(1) | −1.404(2) | 1.936 | 25.274 *** | −22.899 *** |
shape+size | 0.276 | 6.127 | −5.899 | 0.116(3) | 2.563(1) | −2.467(2) | 1.916 | 42.363 *** | −40.538 *** |
Shape | 0.606 | 4.530 | −4.890 | 0.253(3) | 1.895(2) | −2.045(2) | 4.214 * | 31.364 *** | −33.605 *** |
Direction | 0.929 | 2.184 | −1.818 | 0.389 (3) | 0.914(1) | −0.760(2) | 6.447 *** | 15.051 *** | −12.334 *** |
shape+size+axis | 1.882 | 5.924 | −6.756 | 0.787(3) | 2.478(2) | −2.826(1) | 13.101 *** | 41.015 *** | −46.409 *** |
shape+axis | 2.264 | 6.426 | −5.960 | 0.947(3) | 2.688(1) | −2.493(2) | 15.756 *** | 44.452 *** | −40.954 *** |
size+direction | 2.895 | 2.784 | −3.417 | 1.211(2) | 1.165 (3) | −1.429(1) | 20.148 *** | 19.243 *** | −23.410 *** |
shape+direction | 3.032 | 5.748 | −5.842 | 1.268(3) | 2.404(2) | −2.443(1) | 21.103 *** | 39.793 *** | −40.146 *** |
shape+size+direction | 3.521 | 6.960 | −6.762 | 1.472(3) | 2.911(1) | −2.828(2) | 24.503 *** | 47.969 *** | −46.467 *** |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bianchi, I.; Burro, R. The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 172. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11090172
Bianchi I, Burro R. The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(9):172. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11090172
Chicago/Turabian StyleBianchi, Ivana, and Roberto Burro. 2023. "The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 9: 172. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11090172
APA StyleBianchi, I., & Burro, R. (2023). The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition. Journal of Intelligence, 11(9), 172. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11090172