Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”
Abstract
:1. Current Hypotheses on Animal Cognition
2. Performing Competently–Performing Poorly: Cognitive Skills Are Not Necessarily Linked Together
2.1. Chimpanzee Cognition
2.2. Dog Cognition
2.3. New Caledonian Crow Cognition
3. When Animals Outperform Humans
4. Ecology, Perception, and Crucial Limitations
5. New Challenges in Animal Cognition
- Studies should be clear about which cognitive skill(s) they are testing and should not interpret evidence for one skill as automatically proving another, untested skill. Research should not assume that cognitive skills cluster the way they do in humans, but rather should start from the expectation of multiple cognitions until proven otherwise.
- Studies should be based on detailed knowledge of the natural behavior and the ecological environment of the test species, so that it is possible to generate precise hypotheses about the species’ performance on a specific cognitive task (Bates and Byrne 2007).
- Experimental settings should take into account social structures, developmental constraints, and preferred modality of the species under study (Bates and Byrne 2007; Roth et al. 2019).
- Studies with nonhuman animals should no longer target only typically human cognitive skills such as tool-use, self-control, or social cooperation, but should also test skills in which humans might be outperformed by other animals, such as visual and odor perception, working memory, and reaction time (i.e., De Waal 2016; Bräuer and Belger 2018; Inoue and Matsuzawa 2007).
- A holistic approach should be implemented to better integrate laboratory and fieldwork of behavioral ecologists, including the conducting of more rigorous observations and field experiments (Janmaat 2019; Boesch 2010; Bueno-Guerra and Amici 2018).
- An even wider variety of animal taxa should be tested—starting with species that are as yet untested and under-represented in experiments—to gain a whole picture of cognition in the Animal kingdom (Vonk 2016; Roth et al. 2019).
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Agrillo, Christian, Laura Piffer, Angelo Bisazza, and Brian Butterworth. 2012. Evidence for two numerical systems that are similar in humans and guppies. PLoS ONE 7: e31923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Allen, Colin. 1998. Assessing animal cognition: Ethological and philosophical perspectives. Journal of Animal Science 76: 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, Colin. 2017. On (not) defining cognition. Synthese 194: 4233–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, Colin, and Michael Trestman. 2017. Animal Consciousness. In The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc., pp. 63–76. [Google Scholar]
- Arvidsson, Josefin, Mats Amundin, and Matthias Laska. 2012. Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination test by Asian elephants, Elephas maximus. Physiology Behaviour 105: 809–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Auersperg, Alice M. I., Auguste M. I. von Bayern, S. Weber, A. Szabadvari, Thomas Bugnyar, and Alex Kacelnik. 2014. Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281: 20140972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Balda, Russell P., and Alan C. Kamil. 1992. Long-term spatial memory in clark’s nutcracker, Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Behaviour 44: 761–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balda, Russell P., Alan C. Kamil, and Peter A. Bednekoff. 1996. Predicting cognitive capacity from Natural History. In Current Ornithology. Edited by Val Nolan and Ellen D. Ketterson. Boston: Springer US, pp. 33–66. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett, Louise. 2016. Why brains are not computers, why behaviorism is not satanism, and why dolphins are not aquatic apes. The Behavior Analyst 39: 9–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bates, Lucy Anne, and Richard W. Byrne. 2007. Creative or created: Using anecdotes to investigate animal cognition. Methods 42: 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beach, Frank A. 1995. The snark was a boojum. The American Psychologist 5: 115–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benson-Amram, Sarah, Virginia K. Heinen, Sean L. Dryer, and Kay E. Holekamp. 2011. Numerical assessment and individual call discrimination by wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour 82: 743–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beran, Michael J., Audrey E. Parrish, Bonnie M. Perdue, and David A. Washburn. 2014. Comparative cognition: Past, present, and future. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 27: 3–30. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Boesch, Christophe. 2007. What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology 121: 227–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Boesch, Christophe. 2010. Listening to the appeal from the wild. Animal Behavior and Cognition. Animal Behavior and Cognition 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boesch, Christophe, and Hedwige Boesch-Achermann. 2000. The Chimpanzees of the Tai Forest. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boesch, Christophe, Gottfried Hohmann, and Linda F. Marchant. 2002. Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Edited by Christophe Boesch, Gottfried Hohmann and Linda F Marchant. Cambridge: University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boesch, Christophe, Dasa Bombjaková, Adam Boyette, and Amelia Meier. 2017. Technical intelligence and culture: Nut cracking in humans and chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 163: 339–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borrego, Natalia. 2017. Big cats as a model system for the study of the evolution of intelligence. Behavioral Processes 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botigué, Laura R., Shiya Song, Amelie Scheu, Shyamalika Gopalan, Amanda L. Pendleton, Matthew Oetjens, Angela M. Taravella, Timo Seregély, Andrea Zeeb-Lanz, Rose-Marie Arbogast, and et al. 2017. Ancient European dog genomes reveal continuity since the Early Neolithic. Nature Communications 8: 16082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bräuer, Juliane. 2015. I do not understand but I care: The prosocial dog. Interaction Studies 16: 341–60. [Google Scholar]
- Bräuer, Juliane, and Julia Belger. 2018. A ball is not a Kong: Odor representation and search behavior in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different education. Journal of Comparative Psychology 132: 189–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bräuer, Juliane, Juliane Kaminski, Julia Riedel, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2006. Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog, causal ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology 120: 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bräuer, Juliane, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2007. Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition 10: 439–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bräuer, Juliane, Katja Schönefeld, and Josep Call. 2013a. When do dogs help humans? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 148: 138–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bräuer, Juliane, Magdalena Keckeisen, Andrea Pitsch, Juliane Kaminski, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2013b. Domestic dogs conceal auditory but not visual information from others. Animal Cognition 16: 351–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bräuer, Juliane, Milena Bös, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2013c. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Animal Cognition 16: 273–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bshary, Redouan, and Culum Brown. 2014. Fish cognition. Current Biology 24: R947–R50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bshary, Redouan, Felice Lascio, Ana Pinto, and Erica van de Waal. 2011. How Intelligent Is Machiavellian Behavior? Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bueno-Guerra, Nereida, and Federica Amici, eds. 2018. Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition: A Comparative Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bugnyar, Thomas, Stephan A. Reber, and Cameron Buckner. 2016. Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors. Nature Communications 7: 10506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burkart, Judith M., and Carel P Van Schaik. 2010. Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Animal Cognition 13: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Burkart, Judith M., Sarah B. Hrdy, and Carel P. Van Schaik. 2009. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 18: 175–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burkart, Judith M., Michele N. Schubiger, and Carel P. van Schaik. 2016. The evolution of general intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40: e195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Burrows, Anne M., Bridget M. Waller, and Katja Liebal. 2013. Primate Communication: A Multimodal Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne, Richard W. 1996. Machiavellian Intelligence. Evolutionary Anthropology 5: 172–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byrne, Richard W. 1997. The Technical Intelligence hypothesis: An additional evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In Machiavellian Intelligence II: Extensions and Evaluations. Edited by Richard W. Byrne and Andrew Whiten. Cambridge: University Press, pp. 289–311. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne, Richard W., and Andrew Whiten. 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Call, Josep, and Michael Tomasello. 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a Theory of Mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 187–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Call, Josep, Juliane Bräuer, Juliane Kaminski, and Michael Tomasello. 2003. Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Are Sensitive to the Attentional State of Humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology 117: 257–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Call, Josep, Gordon M. Burghardt, Irene M. Pepperberg, Charles T. Snowdon, and Thomas Zentall. 2017. APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology. Edited by J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon and T. Zentall. Volume 1: Basic Concepts, Methods, Neural Substrate, and Behavior, Volume 2: Perception, Learning, and Cognition. 2 vols. Part of the APA Handbooks in Psychology Series; Washington: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, Neil A., Jane B. Reece, Lisa A. Urry, Michael L. Cain, Steven A. Wasserman, Peter V. Minorsky, and Robert B. Jackson. 2008. Biology. Berkeley: Pearson Benjamin Cummings. [Google Scholar]
- Cantlon, Jessica F., and Benjamin Y. Hayden. 2017. Editorial overview: Comparative cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 16: iv–vi. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clayton, Nicola S., and Nathan J. Emery. 2002. Corvid cognition. Current Biology 15: R80–R81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clayton, Nicola S., Nathan J. Emery, and Anthony Dickinson. 2006. The prospective cognition of food caching and recovery. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews 1: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Craig, Catherine L. 1994. Limits to learning: Effects of predator pattern and colour on perception and avoidance-learning by prey. Animal Behaviour 47: 1087–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Crockford, Catherine, Roman Wittig, Roger Mundry, and Klaus Zuberbühler. 2012. Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology 22: 142–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Crockford, Catherine, Roman M. Wittig, and Klaus Zuberbühler. 2017. Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Science Advances 3: e1701742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dale, Rachel, Mylene Quervel-Chaumette, Ludwig Huber, Friederike Range, and Sarah Marshall-Pescini. 2016. Task differences and prosociality: Investigating pet dogs’ prosocial preferences in a token choice paradigm. PLoS ONE 11: e0167750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dally, Joanna M., Nathan J. Emery, and Nicola S. Clayton. 2006. Food-Caching Western Scrub-Jays Keep Track of Who Was Watching When. Science 310: 1662–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Darwin, Charles. 1859. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection. London: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- De Waal, Frans B. M. 2016. Are We Smart Enough to Know How Smart ANIMALS are? New York: W.W. Norton et Company. [Google Scholar]
- De Waal, Frans B. M., and Peter L. Tyack, eds. 2009. Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ducatez, Simon, Joanne Clavel, and Louis Lefebvre. 2015. Ecological generalism and behavioural innovation in birds: Technical intelligence or the simple incorporation of new foods? Journal of Animal Ecology 84: 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunbar, Robin I. M., and Susanne Shultz. 2007. Evolution in the social brain. Science 317: 1344–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Eaton, Taryn, Robert Hutton, Jessica Leete, Jennifer Lieb, Audrey Robeson, and Jennifer Vonk. 2018. Bottoms-up! Rejecting top-down human-centered approaches in comparative psychology. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 31: 1–19. [Google Scholar]
- Emery, Nathan J. 2006. Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 361: 23–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Erdohegyi, Ágnes, József Topál, Zsófia Virányi, and Ádám Miklósi. 2007. Dog-Logic: Inferential Reasoning in a Two-Way Choice Task and Its Restricted Use. Animal Behaviour 74: 725–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fehr, Ernst, and Simon Gächter. 2002. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415: 137–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feinerman, Ofer, and Amos Korman. 2017. Individual versus collective cognition in social insects. The Journal of Experimental Biology 220: 73–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fitch, W. Tecumseh, Ludwig Huber, and Thomas Bugnyar. 2010. Social cognition and the evolution of language: Constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron 65: 795–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fröhlich, Marlen, Paul Kuchenbuch, Gudrun Müller, Barbara Fruth, Takeshi Furuichi, Roman M. Wittig, and Simone Pika. 2016. Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences. Scientific Reports 6: 25887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furuichi, Takeshi, Genichi Idani, Hiroshi Ihobe, Suehisa Kuroda, Koji Kitamura, Akio Mori, Tomoo Enomoto, Naobi Okayasu, Chie Hashimoto, and Takayoshi Kano. 1998. “Population Dynamics of Wild Bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba”. International Journal of Primatology 19: 1029–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaissmaier, Wolfgang, and Lael J. Schooler. 2008. The smart potential behind probability matching. Cognition 109: 416–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gazit, Irit, and Joseph Terkel. 2003. Domination of Olfaction over Vision in Explosives Detection by Dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82: 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillespie-Lynch, Kristen, Patricia M. Greenfield, Heidi Lyn, and Sue Savage-Rumbaugh. 2014. Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: Support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- González-Forero, Mauricio, and Andy Gardner. 2018. Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature 557: 554–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Goodall, Jane. 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge: Belknap Press. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths, Paul E., and Karola Stotz. 2000. How the mind grows: A developmental perspective on the biology of cognition. Synthese 122: 29–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosenick, Logan, Tricia S. Clement, and Russell D. Fernald. 2007. Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature 445: 429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gruber, Romana, Martina Schiestl, Markus Boeckle, Anna Frohnwieser, Rachael Miller, Russell Gray, Nicola Clayton, and Alex Taylor. 2019. New Caledonian Crows Use Mental Representations to Solve Metatool Problems. Current Biology 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Güntürkün, Onur, and Thomas Bugnyar. 2016. Cognition without cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 291–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halina, Marta, Katja Liebal, and Michael Tomasello. 2018. The goal of ape pointing. PLoS ONE 13: e0195182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hanus, Daniel. 2016. Causal reasoning versus associative learning: A useful dichotomy or a strawman battle in comparative psychology? Journal of Comparative Psychology 130: 241–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hanus, Daniel, Natacha Mendes, Claudio Tennie, and Josep Call. 2011. Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the Floating Peanut Task. PLoS ONE 6: e19555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hare, Brian. 2001. Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Animal Cognition 4: 269–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, Brian, and Michael Tomasello. 2004. Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour 68: 571–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hare, Brian, and Michael Tomasello. 2005. Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 439–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, Brian, Josep Call, Bryan Agnetta, and Michael Tomasello. 2000. Chimpanzees Know What Conspecifics Do and Do Not See. Animal Behaviour 59: 771–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, Brian, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2001. Do Chimpanzees Know What Conspecifics Know? Animal Behaviour 61: 139–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hare, Brian, Victoria Wobber, and Richard Wrangham. 2012. The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behavior 83: 573–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hart, Benjamin L., Lynette A. Hart, and Noa Pinter-Wollman. 2008. Large brains and cognition: Where do elephants fit in? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 32: 86–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Ibarra, N. Hempel, Misha Vorobyev, and Randolf Menzel. 2014. Mechanisms, functions and ecology of colour vision in the honeybee. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 200: 411–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Herbranson, Walter T. 2012. Pigeons, Humans, and the Monty Hall Dilemma. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Herrmann, Esther, and Michael Tomasello. 2006. Apes’ and children’s understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation. Developmental Science 9: 518–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herrmann, Esther, Josep Call, María Victoria Hernández-Lloreda, Brian Hare, and Michael Tomasello. 2007. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis. Science 317: 1360–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Heyes, Cecilia, and Ludwig Huber. 2000. The Evolution of Cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hodos, William, and Colin Boyd G. Campbell. 1969. Scala naturae: Why there is no theory in comparative psychology. Psychological Review 76: 337–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holekamp, Kay E. 2007. Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 65–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hollard, Valerie D., and Juan D. Delius. 1982. Rotational invariance in visual pattern recognition by pigeons and humans. Science 218: 804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holzhaider, Jennifer C., Gavin R. Hunt, and Russell D. Gray. 2010a. Social learning in New Caledonian crows. Learning and Behavior 38: 206–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Holzhaider, Jennifer C., Gavin R. Hunt, and Russell D. Gray. 2010b. The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour 147: 553–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Holzhaider, J. C., M. D. Sibley, A. H. Taylor, P. H. Singh, R. D. Gray, and G. R. Hunt. 2011. The social structure of New Caledonian crows. Animal Behaviour 81: 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horowitz, Alexandra, Julie Hecht, and Alexandra Dedrick. 2013. Smelling more or less: Investigating the olfactory experience of the domestic dog. Learning and Motivation 44: 207–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huber, Ludwig. 2016. How dogs perceive and understand us. Current Directions in Psychological Science 25: 339–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphrey, Nicholas K. 1976. The social function of intellect. In Growing Points in Ethology. Edited by Patrick P. G. Bateson and Robert A. Hinde. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 303–17. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, Gavin R., and Russell D. Gray. 2004. The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 271: S88–S90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, Gavin R., and Natalie Uomini. 2016. A complex adaptive system may be essential for cumulative modifications in tool design. Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Inoue, Sana, and Tetsuro Matsuzawa. 2007. Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Current Biology 17: R1004–R1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jacobs, Ivo F., Auguste von Bayern, Gema Martin-Ordas, Lauriane Rat-Fischer, and Mathias Osvath. 2015. Corvids create novel causal interventions after all. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282: 20142504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Janmaat, Karline R. L. 2019. What animals do not do or fail to find: A novel observational approach for studying cognition in the wild. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 28: 303–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Janmaat, Karline R. L., Leo Polansky, Simone Dagui Ban, and Christophe Boesch. 2014. Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111: 16343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jelbert, Sarah A., Puja J. Singh, Russell D. Gray, and Alex H. Taylor. 2015. New Caledonian Crows rapidly solve a collaborative problem without cooperative cognition. PLoS ONE 10: e0133253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jensen, Keith, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2007. Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an Ultimatum Game. Science 318: 107–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jiang, Xinjian, Tenghai Long, Weicong Cao, Junru Li, Stanislas Dehaene, and Liping Wang. 2018. Production of Supra-regular Spatial Sequences by Macaque Monkeys. Current Biology 28: 1851–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jolly, Alison. 1966. Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 153: 501–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kabadayi, Can, and Mathias Osvath. 2017. Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science 357: 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kamil, Alan C. 1987. A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence. Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences 35: 257–308. [Google Scholar]
- Kaminski, Juliane, and Sarah Marshall-Pescini. 2014. The Social Dog: Behaviour and Cognition. San Diego, London, Waltham: Elsevier Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Josep Call, and Julia Fischer. 2004a. Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “Fast Mapping”. Science 304: 1682–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2004b. Body Orientation and Face Orientation: Two Factors Controlling Apes’ Begging Behavior from Humans. Animal Cognition 7: 216–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2008. Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition 109: 224–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Juliane Bräuer, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2009a. Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human’s perspective. Behaviour 146: 979–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Sebastian Tempelmann, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2009b. Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs. Developmental Science 12: 831–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Linda Schulz, and Michael Tomasello. 2012. How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Developmental Science 15: 222–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaminski, Juliane, Jennifer Hynds, Paul Morris, and Bridget M. Waller. 2017. Human attention affects facial expressions in domestic dogs. Scientific Reports 7: 12914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kellogg, Winthrop N., and Luella Agger Kellogg. 1933. The Ape and the Child: A Study of Early Environmental Influence Upon Early Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Kenward, Ben, Christian Rutz, Alex A. S. Weir, and Alex Kacelnik. 2006. Development of tool use in new caledonian crows: Inherited action patterns and social influences. Animal Behaviour 72: 1329–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krachun, Carla, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2009. A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science 12: 521–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krause, Mark, Monique Udell, David Leavens, and Lyra Skopos. 2018. Animal pointing: Changing trends and findings from 30 years of research (2018). Journal of Comparative Psychology 132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krupenye, Christopher, Fumihiro Kano, Satoshi Hirata, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2016. Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science 354: 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Krupenye, Christopher, Fumihiro Kano, Satoshi Hirata, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2017. A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative and Integrative Biology 10: e1343771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kundey, Shannon M. A., Andres De Los Reyes, Chelsea Taglang, Rebecca Allen, Sabrina Molina, Erica Royer, and Rebecca German. 2010. Domesticated Dogs (Canis familiaris) React to What Others Can and Cannot Hear. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 126: 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laland, Kevin N., John Odling-Smee, and Marcus W. Feldman. 2000. Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral Brain Sciences 23: 131–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lampe, Michelle, Juliane Bräuer, Juliane Kaminski, and Zsófia Virányi. 2017. The effects of domestication and ontogeny on cognition in dogs and wolves. Scientific Reports, 11690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Laumer, Isabelle B., Thomas Bugnyar, Stephan A. Reber, and Alice M. I. Auersperg. 2017. Can hook-bending be let off the hook? Bending/unbending of pliant tools by cockatoos. Proceedings Biology Sciences 284: 1862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leavens, David A., William D. Hopkins, and Kim A. Bard. 1996. Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 110: 346–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leavens, David A., Kim A. Bard, and William D. Hopkins. 2017. The mismeasure of ape social cognition. Animal Cognition. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Levenson, Richard M., Elizabeth A. Krupinski, Victor M. Navarro, and Edward A. Wasserman. 2015. Pigeons (Columba livia) as trainable observers of pathology and radiology breast cancer images. PLoS ONE 10: e0141357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Logan, Corina, Sarah Jelbert, Alexis Breen, Russell Gray, and Alex Taylor. 2014. Modifications to the Aesop’s Fable Paradigm Change New Caledonian Crow Performances. PLoS ONE 9: e103049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Logan, Corina J., Alexis J. Breen, Alex H. Taylor, Russell D. Gray, and William J. E. Hoppitt. 2016. How New Caledonian crows solve novel foraging problems and what it means for cumulative culture. Learning and Behavior 44: 18–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- MacLean, Evan L. 2016. Unraveling the evolution of uniquely human cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113: 6348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- MacLean, Evan L., Luke J. Matthews, Brian A. Hare, Charles L. Nunn, Rindy C. Anderson, Filippo Aureli, Elizabeth M. Brannon, Josep Call, Christine M. Drea, Nathan J. Emery, and et al. 2012. How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Animal Cognition 15: 223–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macphail, Euan M. 1982. Brain and Intelligence in Vertebrates. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Maestripieri, Dario. 2003. Primate Psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall-Pescini, Sarah, Rachel Dale, Mylene Quervel-Chaumette, and Friederike Range. 2016. Critical issues in experimental studies of prosociality in non-human species. Animal Cognition 19: 679–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Marshall-Pescini, Sarah, Jonas F. L. Schwarz, Inga Kostelnik, Zsofia Viranyi, and Friederike Range. 2017. Importance of a species’ socioecology: Wolves outperform dogs in a conspecific cooperation task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: 11793–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Matsubara, Satoko, D. Charles Deeming, and Anna Wilkinson. 2017. Cold-blooded cognition: New directions in reptile cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 16: 126–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McComb, Karen, Craig Packer, and Anne Pusey. 1994. Roaring and Numerical Assessment in Contests Between Groups of Female Lions, Panthera leo. Animal Behaviour 47: 379–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McMillan, Neil, Allison H. Hahn, Marcia L. Spetch, and Christopher B. Sturdy. 2015. Avian cognition: Examples of sophisticated capabilities in space and song. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 6: 285–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mendes, Natacha, Nikolaus Steinbeis, Nereida Bueno-Guerra, Josep Call, and Tania Singer. 2018. Preschool children and chimpanzees incur costs to watch punishment of antisocial others. Nature Human Behaviour 2: 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miklosi, Adam. 2007. Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition, 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Miletto Petrazzini, Maria E., and Clive D. Wynne. 2016. What counts for dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in a quantity discrimination task? Behavioural Processes 122: 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mitani, John C., and David P. Watts. 2001. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour 61: 915–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morgan, Thomas, Natalie T. Uomini, Luke E. Rendell, Laura Chouinard-Thuly, Sally E. Street, Helen M. Lewis, Catherine P. Cross, Cara Evans, R. Kearney, Ignacio de la Torre, and et al. 2015. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications 6: 6029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Osiurak, François, Mathieu Lesourd, Jordan Navarro, and Emanuelle Reynaud. 2020. Technition: When tools come out of the closet. Perspectives on Psychological Science. Online ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osthaus, Britta, Stephen E. G. Lea, and Alan M. Slater. 2005. Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition 8: 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papini, Mauricio R. 2002. Pattern and process in the evolution of learning. Psychol Rev 109: 186–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Patton, B. Wren, and Victoria A. Braithwaite. 2015. Changing tides: Ecological and historical perspectives on fish cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 6: 159–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pika, S. 2014. Chimpanzee grooming gestures and sounds: What might they tell us about how language evolved? In The Social Origins of Language: Early Society, Communication and Polymodality. Edited by Daniel Dor, Cris Knight and Jerome Lewis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 129–40. [Google Scholar]
- Pika, Simone, and Thomas Bugnyar. 2011. The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild. Nature Communications 2: 560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pika, Simone, and John Mitani. 2006. Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Current Biology 16: R191–R192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pika, Simone, Ray Wilkinson, Kobin H. Kendrick, and Sonja C. Vernes. 2018. Taking turns: Bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 285: 20180598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pike, Thomas W., and Kevin N. Laland. 2010. Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks’ foraging decisions. Biology Letters 6: 466–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pinker, Steven. 2010. The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107: 8993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Piotti, Patrizia, and Juliane Kaminski. 2016. Do Dogs Provide Information Helpfully? PLoS ONE 11: e0159797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Polansky, Leo, Werner Kilian, and George Wittemyer. 2015. Elucidating the significance of spatial memory on movement decisions by African savannah elephants using state-space models. Proceedings. Biological sciences 282: 20143042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Povinelli, Daniel J., and Timothy J. Eddy. 1996. What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 61: 1–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Premack, David. 2007. Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 13861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Premack, David, and Guy Woodruff. 1978. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 515–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Prétôt, Laurent, Redouan Bshary, and Sarah Brosnan. 2016. Factors influencing the different performance of fish and primates on a dichotomous choice task. Animal Behaviour 119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pusey, Anne E., and Kara Schroepfer-Walker. 2013. Female competition in chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 368: 20130077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Quervel-Chaumette, Mylene, Gaëlle Mainix, Friederike Range, and Sarah Marshall-Pescini. 2016. Dogs do not show pro-social preferences towards humans. Humans Frontier in Psycholy 7: 1416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Raby, C. R., D. M. Alexis, Anthony Dickinson, and Nicola S. Clayton. 2007. Planning for the Future by Western Scrub-Jays. Nature 445: 919–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Range, Friederike, Sarah Marshall-Pescini, Corinna Kratz, and Zsófia Virányi. 2019. Wolves lead and dogs follow, but they both cooperate with humans. Scientific Reports 9: 3796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Redshaw, Jonathan, Alex H. Taylor, and Thomas Suddendorf. 2017. Flexible planning in ravens? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 821–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Reznikova, Z. Zhanna. 2007. Animal Intelligence: From Individual to Social Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, William A. 2002. Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin 128: 473–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rooijakkers, Eveline, Juliane Kaminski, and Josep Call. 2009. Comparing dogs and great apes in their ability to visually track object transpositions. Animal Cognition 12: 789–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rosati, Alexandra G. 2017. Foraging cognition: Reviving the Ecological Intelligence Hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 691–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosati, Alexandra G., and Brian Hare. 2011. Chimpanzees and bonobos distinguish between risk and ambiguity. Biology Letters 7: 15–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosati, Alexandra G., Jeffrey R. Stevens, Brian Hare, and Marc D. Hauser. 2007. The evolutionary origins of human patience: Temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology 17: 1663–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Roth, Timothy C., Aaaron R. Krochmal, and Lara D. LaDage. 2019. Reptilian cognition: A more complex picture via integration of neurological mechanisms, behavioral constraints, and evolutionary context. BioEssays 41: 1900033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowell, Thelma. 1999. The myth of peculiar primates. In Symposia of the Zoological Society of London; Mammalian Social Learning: Comparative and Ecological Perspectives. Edited by Hilary O. Box and Kathleen R Gibson. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rutz, Christian, Lucas A. Bluff, Nicola Reed, Jolyon Troscianko, Jason Newton, Richard Inger, Alex Kacelnik, and Stuart Bearhop. 2010. The ecological significance of tool use in New Caledonian Crows. Science 329: 1523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salwiczek, Lucie H., Laurent Prétôt, Lanila Demarta, Darby Proctor, Jennifer Essler, Ana I. Pinto, Sharon Wismer, Tara Stoinski, Sarah F. Brosnan, and Redouan Bshary. 2012. Adult Cleaner Wrasse Outperform Capuchin Monkeys, Chimpanzees and Orang-utans in a Complex Foraging Task Derived from Cleaner—Client Reef Fish Cooperation. PLoS ONE 7: e49068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seed, Amanda M., Sabine Tebbich, Nathan J. Emery, and Nicola S. Clayton. 2006. Investigating physical cognition in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Current Biology 16: 697–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seed, Amanda, Nathan Emery, and Nicola Clayton. 2009. Intelligence in corvids and apes: A case of convergent evolution? Ethology 115: 401–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sewall, Kendra B. 2015. Social complexity as a driver of communication and cognition. Integrative Comparative Biology 55: 384–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Seyfarth, Robert M., and Dorothy L. Cheney. 2003. The structure of social knowledge in monkeys. In Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies. Edited by Frans B. M. de Waal and Peter L. Tyack. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, pp. 207–29. [Google Scholar]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 1983. Memory in food-hoarding birds. Scientific American 248: 102–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 1998. Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 2000. Modularity and the evolution of cognition. In The Evolution of Cognition. Edited by Cecilia Heyes and Ludwig Huber. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 43–60. [Google Scholar]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 2009. The evolution of comparative cognition: Is the snark still a boojum? Behavioural Processes 80: 210–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 2010. Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 477–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shettleworth, Sarah J. 2012. Modularity, comparative cognition and human uniqueness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 2794–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sol, Daniel. 2009. Revisiting the cognitive buffer hypothesis for the evolution of large brains. Biology Letters 5: 130–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stanton, Lauren, Emely Davis, Shylo Johnson, Amy Gilbert, and Sarah Benson-Amram. 2017. Adaptation of the Aesop’s Fable paradigm for use with raccoons (Procyon lotor): Considerations for future application in non-avian and non-primate species. Animal Cognition 20: 1147–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sterck, Elisabeth H. M., David P. Watts, and Carel P. van Schaik. 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 41: 291–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sterelny, Kim. 2003. Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Blackwell: Malden. [Google Scholar]
- Suddendorf, Thomas, and Janie Busby. 2003. Mental Time Travel in Animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 391–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Surbeck, Martin, and Gottfried Hohmann. 2008. Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park. Current Biology 18: R906–R907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., and Russell D. Gray. 2014. Is there a link between the crafting of tools and the evolution of cognition? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 5: 693–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, Alex H., Gavin R. Hunt, Jennifer C. Holzhaider, and Russell D. Gray. 2007. Spontaneous metatool use by new caledonian crows. Current Biology 17: 1504–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., Gavin R. Hunt, Felipe S. Medina, and Russell D. Gray. 2009. Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276: 247–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., Felipe S. Medina, Jennifer C. Holzhaider, Lindsay J. Hearne, Gavin R. Hunt, and Russell D. Gray. 2010. An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in New Caledonian Crows. PLoS ONE 5: e9345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., Brenna Knaebe, and Russell D. Gray. 2012a. An end to insight? New Caledonian crows can spontaneously solve problems without planning their actions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279: 4977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., Gavin R. Hunt, and Russell D. Gray. 2012b. Context-dependent tool use in New Caledonian crows. Biology Letters 8: 205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Taylor, Alex H., Lucy G. Cheke, Anna Waismeyer, Andrew N. Meltzoff, Rachael Miller, Alison Gopnik, Nicola S. Clayton, and Russell D. Gray. 2014. Of babies and birds: Complex tool behaviours are not sufficient for the evolution of the ability to create a novel causal intervention. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281: 20140837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tebbich, Sabine, Amanda M. Seed, Nathan J. Emery, and Nicola S. Clayton. 2007. Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Animal Cognition 10: 225–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tennie, Claudio, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello. 2009. Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences 364: 2405–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Teschke, Irmgard, E. A. Cartmill, S. Stankewitz, and Sabine Tebbich. 2011. Sometimes tool use is not the key: No evidence for cognitive adaptive specializations in tool-using woodpecker finches. Animal Behaviour 82: 945–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teschke, Irmgard, Claudia A. F. Wascher, Madeleine Scriba, Auguste M. P. von Bayern, Jana Vanessa Huml, B. Siemers, and Sabine Tebbich. 2013. Did tool-use evolve with enhanced physical cognitive abilities? Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 368: 20120418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Thalmann, Olaf, B. Shapiro, P. Cui, V. J. Schuenemann, S. K. Sawyer, D. L. Greenfield, M. B. Germonpré, M. V. Sablin, S. López-Giráldez, X. Domingo-Roura, and et al. 2013. Complete mitochondrial genomes of ancient canids suggest a european origin of domestic dogs. Science 342: 871–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tobin, Henry, A. W. Logue, John J. Chelonis, Kimberly T. Ackerman, and Jack G. May. 1996. Self-control in the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Animal Learning and Behavior 24: 168–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2019. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge: Belknap Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, Michael, and Josep Call. 1997. Primate Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, Michael, and Josep Call. 2008. Assessing the Validity of Ape-Human Comparisons: A Reply to Boesch (2007). Journal of Comparative Psychology 122: 449–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael, and Esther Herrmann. 2010. Ape and human cognition: What’s the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomonaga, Masaki, Kiyonori Kumazaki, Florine Camus, Sophie Nicod, Carlos Pereira, and Tetsuro Matsuzawa. 2015. A horse’s eye view: Size and shape discrimination compared with other mammals. Biology Letters 11: 20150701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Uomini, Natalie. 2008. Cognition and culture: The potential for archaeology. Commentary on Haidle. Erwägen Wissen Ethik (Deliberation Knowledge Ethics) 19: 50–53. [Google Scholar]
- Uomini, Natalie. 2009. Prehistoric left-handers and prehistoric language. In The Emergence of Cognitive Abilities: The Contribution of Neuropsychology to Archaeology. Edited by S. A. de Beaune and F. L. Coolidge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 37–55. [Google Scholar]
- Uomini, Natalie. 2014. Paleoneurology and behaviour. In Human Paleoneurology. Edited by Emiliano Bruner. Springer Series in Bio-/Neuroinformatics; Cham: Springer, vol. 3, pp. 121–43. [Google Scholar]
- Uomini, Natalie. 2017. Neuroarchaeology: Language and tools in the brain. Nature Human Behaviour. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uomini, Natalie, and Gavin Hunt. 2017. A new tool-using bird to crow about. Learning and Behaviour. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Uomini, Natalie, and Georg Meyer. 2013. Shared cerebral blood flow lateralization patterns in language and stone tool production. PLoS ONE 8: e72693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uomini, Natalie, and Lana Ruck. 2018. Manual laterality and cognition through evolution: An archaeological perspective. Progress in Brain Research 238: 295–323. [Google Scholar]
- Uomini, Natalie, Joanna Fairlie, Russell D. Gray, and Michael Griesser. 2020. Extended parenting and the evolution of cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Urhan, A. Utku, Ellen Emilsson, and Anders Brodin. 2017. Evidence against observational spatial memory for cache locations of conspecifics in marsh tits Poecile palustris. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 71: 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vail, Alexander, Andrea Manica, and Redouan Bshary. 2013. Referential gestures in fish collaborative hunting. Nature Communications 4: 1765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vasilieva, Olga. 2019. Beyond “uniqueness”: Habitual traits in the context of cognitive-communicative continuity. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum 16: 129–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veit, Ariane, Marianne Wondrak, and Ludwig Huber. 2017. Object movement re-enactment in free-ranging Kune Kune piglets. Animal Behaviour 132: 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vermaercke, Ben, Elsy Cop, Sam Willems, Rudi D’Hooge, and Hans P. Op de Beeck. 2014. More complex brains are not always better: Rats outperform humans in implicit category-based generalization by implementing a similarity-based strategy. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21: 1080–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vlamings, Petra, Brian Hare, and Josep Call. 2010. Reaching around barriers: The performance of the great apes and 3–5-year-old children. Animal Cognition 13: 273–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vonk, Jennifer. 2016. Advances in animal cognition. Behavioral Sciences 6: 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vonk, Jennifer, and Daniel J. Povinelli. 2012. Similarity and difference in the conceptual systems of primates: The unobservability hypothesis. In The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Cognition. Edited by Thomas R. Zentall and Edward A. Wasserman. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 552–75. [Google Scholar]
- Wasserman, Edward A., and Thomas R. Zentall, eds. 2006. Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe, Shigeru. 2001. Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: Picture discrimination in pigeons and humans. Animal Cognition 4: 147–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watanabe, Shigeru, Junko Sakamoto, and Masumi Wakita. 1995. Pigeons’ discrimination of paintings by Monet and Picasso. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 63: 165–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Webb, Barbara A. 2012. Cognition in insects. Journal of the Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 367: 2715–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weir, Alex A., Jackie Chappell, and Alex Kacelnik. 2002. Shaping of hooks in new caledonian crows. Science 297: 981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Werdenich, Dagmar, and Ludwig Huber. 2006. A case of quick problem solving in birds: String pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour 71: 855–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wismer, Sharon, Alexandra Grutter, and Redouan Bshary. 2016. Generalized rule application in bluestreak cleaner wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus): Using predator species as social tools to reduce punishment. Animal Cognition 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittig, Roman, and Christophe Boesch. 2003. Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Tai National Park. International Journal of Primatology 24: 847–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wondrak, Marianne, Elin Conzelmann, Ariane Veit, and Ludwig Huber. 2018. Pigs (Sus scrofa domesticus) categorize pictures of human heads. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 205: 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wrangham, Richard W., Nancy L. Conklin-Brittain, and Kevin D. Hunt. 1998. Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance. International Journal of Primatology 19: 949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zentall, Thomas R., and Jacob P. Case. 2018. The Ephemeral-Reward Task: Optimal Performance Depends on Reducing Impulsive Choice. Current Directions in Psychological Science 27: 103–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zentall, Thomas R., and Edward A. Wasserman, eds. 2012. The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Topic Search Term | Number of Publications | Total Citations | Total Excluding Self-Citations | Citations Per Year in 2000 | Citations Per Year in 2019 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
animal cognition | 1202 | 18,550 | 17,760 | ||
animal psychology | 435 | 3329 | 3295 | ||
cognitive ethology | 216 | 3242 | 3051 | ||
comparative cognition | 642 | 8379 | 7822 | ||
comparative psychology | 1376 | 13,496 | 12,587 | ||
Any of the above | 3657 | 43,590 | 39,619 | 396 | 4394 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bräuer, J.; Hanus, D.; Pika, S.; Gray, R.; Uomini, N. Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030028
Bräuer J, Hanus D, Pika S, Gray R, Uomini N. Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”. Journal of Intelligence. 2020; 8(3):28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030028
Chicago/Turabian StyleBräuer, Juliane, Daniel Hanus, Simone Pika, Russell Gray, and Natalie Uomini. 2020. "Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”" Journal of Intelligence 8, no. 3: 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030028
APA StyleBräuer, J., Hanus, D., Pika, S., Gray, R., & Uomini, N. (2020). Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”. Journal of Intelligence, 8(3), 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030028