The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Can Geary’s Theory Explain Why Certain Cognitive Processes Show a Stronger Age-Related Decline Than Others?
1.2. Do Intervention Studies in Healthy Younger Adults Support the Claim That Variation in Mitochondrial Functioning Underlies Variation in Human Intelligence?
1.3. Are Predictions Arising from the Matrilineal Heredity of Mitochondrial DNA Supported by Behavioral Genetics Research?
2. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Avgerinos, Konstantinos I., Nikolaos Spyrou, Konstantinos I. Bougioukas, and Dimitrios Kapogiannis. 2018. Effects of creatine supplementation on cognitive function of healthy individuals: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Experimental Gerontology 108: 166–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barbey, Aron K. 2018. Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 8–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Basten, Ulrike, Kirsten Hilger, and Christian J. Fiebach. 2015. Where smart brains are different: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence 51: 10–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batty, G. David, Ian J. Deary, and Linda S. Gottfredson. 2007. Premorbid (Early Life) IQ and later mortality risk: Systematic review. Annals of Epidemiology 17: 278–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Booth, Tom, Mark E. Bastin, Lars Penke, Susana Muñoz Maniega, Catherine Murray, Natalie A. Royle, Alan J. Gow, Janie Corley, Ross D. Henderson, Maria del C. Valdés Hernández, and et al. 2013. Brain white matter tract integrity and cognitive abilities in community-dwelling older people: The Lothian birth cohort, 1936. Neuropsychology 27: 595–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chien, Ling, Min-Zong Liang, Chu-Yuan Chang, Chen Wang, and Linyi Chen. 2018. Mitochondrial therapy promotes regeneration of injured hippocampal neurons. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA)—Molecular Basis of Disease 1864, Pt B: 3001–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cohen, Joshua T., David C. Bellinger, William E. Connor, and Bennett A. Shaywitz. 2005. A quantitative analysis of prenatal intake of N-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognitive development. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 29: 366.e12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2017. The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1158–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Deary, Ian. 2008. Why do intelligent people live longer? Nature 456: 175–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delanghe, Joris R., Jean-Paul De Slypere, Marc L. De Buyzere, Johan Robbrecht, Roger Wieme, and Alex Vermeulen. 1989. Normal reference values for creatine, creatinine, and carnitine are lower in vegetarians. Clinical Chemistry 35: 1802–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Der, Geoff, G. David Batty, and Ian J. Deary. 2009. The association between IQ in adolescence and a range of health outcomes at 40 in the 1979 US national longitudinal study of youth. Intelligence, Intelligence, Health and Death: The Emerging Field of Cognitive Epidemiology 37: 573–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Engle, Randall W. 2018. Working memory and executive attention: A revisit. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 13: 190–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foti Cuzzola, V., Rosella Ciurleo, Sabrina Giacoppo, Silvia Marino, and Placido Bramanti. 2011. Role of resveratrol and its analogues in the treatment of neurodegenerative diseases: Focus on recent discoveries. CNS & Neurological Disorders Drug Targets 10: 849–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, David C. 2018. Efficiency of mitochondrial functioning as the fundamental biological mechanism of general intelligence (g). Psychological Review 125: 1028–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, David C. 2019. Mitochondria as the linchpin of general intelligence and the link between g, health, and aging. Journal of Intelligence 7: 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hedges, Larry V., and Amy Nowell. 1995. Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science 269: 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Henderson, Samuel T., Janet L. Vogel, Linda J. Barr, Fiona Garvin, Julie J. Jones, and Lauren C. Costantini. 2009. Study of the Ketogenic agent AC-1202 in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s Disease: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, multicenter trial. Nutrition & Metabolism 6: 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hilger, Kirsten, Matthias Ekman, Christian J. Fiebach, and Ulrike Basten. 2017. Efficient hubs in the intelligent brain: Nodal efficiency of hub regions in the salience network is associated with general intelligence. Intelligence 60: 10–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, John L., and Raymond B. Cattell. 1967. Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica 26: 107–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Wendy, Andrew Carothers, and Ian J. Deary. 2008. Sex differences in variability in general intelligence: A new look at the old question. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3: 518–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jung, Rex E., and Richard J. Haier. 2007. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 135–54; discussion 154–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karpinski, Ruth I., Audrey M. Kinase Kolb, Nicole A. Tetreault, and Thomas B. Borowski. 2018. High intelligence: A risk factor for psychological and physiological Overexcitabilities. Intelligence 66: 8–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kennedy, David O., Emma L. Wightman, Jonathon L. Reay, Georg Lietz, Edward J. Okello, Anthea Wilde, and Crystal F. Haskell. 2010. Effects of resveratrol on cerebral blood flow variables and cognitive performance in humans: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 91: 1590–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keylor, MitchellH, Bryan S. Matsuura, and Corey R. J. Stephenson. 2015. Chemistry and biology of resveratrol-derived natural products. Chemical Reviews 115: 8976–9027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kievit, Rogier A., Simon W. Davis, John Griffiths, Marta M. Correia, Cam-CAN, and Richard N. Henson. 2016. A watershed model of individual differences in fluid intelligence. Neuropsychologia 91: 186–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process overlap theory: A unified account of the general factor of intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lassek, William D., and Steven J. C. Gaulin. 2008. Waist-Hip ratio and cognitive ability: Is gluteofemoral fat a privileged store of neurodevelopmental resources? Evolution and Human Behavior 29: 26–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Zhenguang, Pengfei Wang, Zhancai Yu, Yannan Cong, Hairong Sun, Jiangshan Zhang, Jinbiao Zhang, Chao Sun, Yong Zhang, and Xiaohua Ju. 2015. The effect of creatine and coenzyme q10 combination therapy on mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s Disease. European Neurology 73: 205–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ling, Jonathan, Minos Kritikos, and Brian Tiplady. 2009. Cognitive effects of Creatine Ethyl Ester supplementation. Behavioural Pharmacology 20: 673–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Malhotra, Ashim, Sundeep Bath, and Fawzy Elbarbry. 2015. An Organ System Approach to Explore the Antioxidative, Anti-Inflammatory, and Cytoprotective Actions of Resveratrol. Review Article. Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity. London: Hindawi. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nee, Derek Evan, and John Jonides. 2011. Dissociable contributions of prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus to short-term memory: Evidence for a 3-State model of memory. NeuroImage 54: 1540–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nee, Derek Evan, and John Jonides. 2013a. Trisecting representational states in short-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nee, Derek Evan, and John Jonides. 2013b. Neural Evidence for a 3-State model of visual short-term memory. NeuroImage 74: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Oberauer, Klaus, Stephan Lewandowsky, Edward Awh, Gordon D. A. Brown, Andrew Conway, Nelson Cowan, Christopher Donkin, Simon Farrell, Graham J. Hitch, Mark J. Hurlstone, and et al. 2018. Benchmarks for models of short-term and working memory. Psychological Bulletin 144: 885–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Öztekin, Ilke, Brian McElree, Bernhard P. Staresina, and Lila Davachi. 2008. Working memory retrieval: Contributions of the left prefrontal cortex, the left posterior parietal cortex, and the hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21: 581–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Persky, Adam M., and Gayle A. Brazeau. 2001. Clinical pharmacology of the dietary supplement Creatine monohydrate. Pharmacological Reviews 53: 161–76. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Pineda-Pardo, José Angel, Kenia Martínez, Francisco J. Román, and Roberto Colom. 2016. Structural efficiency within a parieto-frontal network and cognitive differences. Intelligence 54: 105–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plomin, Robert, and Sophie von Stumm. 2018. The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics 19: 148–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rae, Caroline, Alison L. Digney, Sally R. McEwan, and Timothy C. Bates. 2003. Oral Creatine monohydrate supplementation improves Brain performance: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial. Proceedings. Biological Sciences 270: 2147–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rawson, Eric S., Harris R. Lieberman, Talia M. Walsh, Sylwia M. Zuber, Jaclyn M. Harhart, and Tracy C. Matthews. 2008. Creatine supplementation does not improve cognitive function in young adults. Physiology & Behavior 95: 130–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhemtulla, Mijke, and Elliot M. Tucker-Drob. 2011. Correlated longitudinal changes across linguistic, achievement, and psychomotor domains in early childhood: Evidence for a global dimension of development. Developmental Science 14: 1245–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salehi, Bahare, Abhay Prakash Mishra, Manisha Nigam, Bilge Sener, Mehtap Kilic, Mehdi Sharifi-Rad, Patrick Valere Tsouh Fokou, Natália Martins, and Javad Sharifi-Rad. 2018. Resveratrol: A double-edged sword in health benefits. Biomedicines 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salthouse, Timothy A. 2004. Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence 32: 541–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salthouse, Timothy A. 2009. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging 30: 507–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Salthouse, Timothy A., and Emilio Ferrer-Caja. 2003. What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychology and Aging 18: 91–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schubert, Anna-Lena, and Gidon T. Frischkorn. 2020. Neurocognitive psychometrics of intelligence: How measurement advancements unveiled the role of mental speed in intelligence differences. Current Directions in Psychological Science 29: 140–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schubert, Anna-Lena, Dirk Hagemann, Christoph Löffler, Jan Rummel, and Stefan Arnau. 2020. A chronometric model of the relationship between frontal midline theta functional connectivity and human intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silbert, Lauren J., Christopher J. Honey, Erez Simony, David Poeppel, and Uri Hasson. 2014. Coupled neural systems underlie the production and comprehension of naturalistic narrative speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111: E4687–E4696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spearman, C. 1904. ‘General Intelligence,’ objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology 15: 201–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Albert Y., Qun Wang, Agnes Simonyi, and Grace Y. Sun. 2010. Resveratrol as a therapeutic agent for neurodegenerative diseases. Molecular Neurobiology 41: 375–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tamnes, Christian K., Ylva Østby, Kristine B. Walhovd, Lars T. Westlye, Paulina Due-Tønnessen, and Anders M. Fjell. 2010. Intellectual abilities and white matter microstructure in development: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Human Brain Mapping 31: 1609–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tucker-Drob, Elliot M. 2011. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Developmental Psychology 47: 331–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tucker-Drob, Elliot M., Daniel A. Briley, John M. Starr, and Ian J. Deary. 2014. Structure and correlates of cognitive aging in a narrow age cohort. Psychology and Aging 29: 236–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Der Maas, Han L. J., Conor V. Dolan, Raoul P. P. P. Grasman, Jelte M. Wicherts, Hilde M. Huizenga, and Maartje E. J. Raijmakers. 2006. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review 113: 842–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wai, Jonathan, Megan Cacchio, Martha Putallaz, and Matthew C. Makel. 2010. Sex differences in the right tail of cognitive abilities: A 30 year examination. Intelligence 38: 412–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wendelken, Carter, Emilio Ferrer, Simona Ghetti, Stephen K. Bailey, Laurie Cutting, and Silvia A. Bunge. 2017. Frontoparietal structural connectivity in childhood predicts development of functional connectivity and reasoning ability: A large-scale longitudinal investigation. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 37: 8549–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whitley, Elise, Catharine R. Gale, Ian J. Deary, Mika Kivimaki, and G. David Batty. 2011. Association of maternal and paternal IQ with offspring conduct, emotional, and attention problem scores. Transgenerational evidence from the 1958 British birth cohort study. Archives of General Psychiatry 68: 1032–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wightman, Emma L., Jonathon L. Reay, Crystal F. Haskell, Gary Williamson, Tristan P. Dew, and David O. Kennedy. 2014. Effects of resveratrol alone or in combination with piperine on cerebral blood flow parameters and cognitive performance in human subjects: A Randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over investigation. The British Journal of Nutrition 112: 203–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wightman, Emma L., Crystal F. Haskell-Ramsay, Jonathon L. Reay, Gary Williamson, Tristan Dew, Wei Zhang, and David O. Kennedy. 2015. The effects of chronic trans-resveratrol supplementation on aspects of cognitive function, mood, sleep, health and cerebral blood flow in healthy, young humans. British Journal of Nutrition 114: 1427–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Witte, A. Veronica, Lucia Kerti, Daniel S. Margulies, and Agnes Flöel. 2014. Effects of resveratrol on memory performance, hippocampal functional connectivity, and glucose metabolism in healthy older adults. Journal of Neuroscience 34: 7862–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wong, Rachel H. X., Narelle M. Berry, Alison M. Coates, Jonathan D. Buckley, Janet Bryan, Iris Kunz, and Peter R. C. Howe. 2013. Chronic resveratrol consumption improves brachial flow-mediated dilatation in healthy obese adults. Journal of Hypertension 31: 1819–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out that recent evidence suggests that this association may reverse in highly intelligent individuals (i.e., those within the upper 2% of the general population), who may be at a higher risk than the general population to develop certain psychological disorders as well as physiological conditions (Karpinski et al. 2018). |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Schubert, A.-L.; Hagemann, D. The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030029
Schubert A-L, Hagemann D. The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing. Journal of Intelligence. 2020; 8(3):29. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030029
Chicago/Turabian StyleSchubert, Anna-Lena, and Dirk Hagemann. 2020. "The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing" Journal of Intelligence 8, no. 3: 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030029
APA StyleSchubert, A. -L., & Hagemann, D. (2020). The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing. Journal of Intelligence, 8(3), 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030029