Next Article in Journal
The Role of Epicardial Adipose Tissue-Derived MicroRNAs in the Regulation of Cardiovascular Disease: A Narrative Review
Previous Article in Journal
Finding a Husband: Using Explainable AI to Define Male Mosquito Flight Differences
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Pollen as Bee Medicine: Is Prevention Better than Cure?

1
CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, 34090 Montpellier, France
2
Laboratory of Zoology, Research Institute for Biosciences, University of Mons, 7000 Mons, Belgium
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Biology 2023, 12(4), 497; https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12040497
Submission received: 24 January 2023 / Revised: 20 March 2023 / Accepted: 23 March 2023 / Published: 24 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Behavioural Biology)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Bees suffer from diverse pathogens and parasites that play crucial roles in shaping their communities. Alas, human activities have deeply disturbed natural bee–pathogen dynamics, through the spread of emerging pathogens and altered transmission networks. Such human-disturbed pollinator–pathogen dynamics are partly assumed to be responsible for pollinator decline. To deal with parasite infection, bees may rely on specific resources that may act as natural pharmacies. In this study, we explored whether different pollen resources may impede parasite establishment in healthy bumble bees or reduce parasite load in infected ones, either by weakening the parasite or improving the host resistance towards infection. Moreover, we also investigated whether infected bumble bees favour medicating resources over non-medicating ones. We found that consuming specific resources could impede parasite dynamics, especially its establishment, but that the cost–benefit trade-off could be detrimental if bumble bee reproductive success is highly reduced along with these medicinal effects. We did not affirm any self-medicative behaviour in infected bumble bees regarding the gut parasite used in our study, but questioned the importance of parasite virulence for such behaviours to occur. Our results showed that the ecological significance of medicating resource may be overlooked when missing parts of the story.

Abstract

To face environmental stressors such as infection, animals may display behavioural plasticity to improve their physiological status through ingestion of specific food. In bees, the significance of medicating pollen may be limited by their ability to exploit it. Until now, studies have focused on the medicinal effects of pollen and nectar after forced-feeding experiments, overlooking spontaneous intake. Here, we explored the medicinal effects of different pollen on Bombus terrestris workers infected by the gut parasite Crithidia bombi. First, we used a forced-feeding experimental design allowing for the distinction between prophylactic and therapeutic effects of pollen, considering host tolerance and resistance. Then, we assessed whether bumble bees favoured medicating resources when infected to demonstrate potential self-medicative behaviour. We found that infected bumble bees had a lower fitness but higher resistance when forced to consume sunflower or heather pollen, and that infection dynamics was more gradual in therapeutic treatments. When given the choice between resources, infected workers did not target medicating pollen, nor did they consume more medicating pollen than uninfected ones. These results emphasize that the access to medicating resources could impede parasite dynamics, but that the cost–benefit trade-off could be detrimental when fitness is highly reduced.

1. Introduction

To protect themselves from structural and functional damage (e.g., protein oxidation, organ dysfunction) due to external challenges, organisms have evolved complex machineries of endogenous defences and physiological adjustments [1,2]. These physiological responses are involved in many essential pathways such as immune defence, cellular signalling, thermal tolerance and survival. However, mounting an appropriate physiological response to face environmental stressors may be inadequate, as it may take explicit time for activation [3], and it requires organisms to be in good conditions as it incurs a cost to recruit endogenous resources [4]. To alleviate the costs related to the use of endogenous resources, or to compensate for latency or for limited physiological plasticity, organisms have the possibility to modify their behaviour (i.e., Bogert effect, [5]), for instance by ingesting specific exogenous resources. Such a regulation of environmental stress using food items could be one of the key mechanisms determining the capacity of organisms to cope with external challenges commonly encountered in an ever-changing world.
Ingestion of pharmaceutically active substances may immediately correct for suboptimal physiological state as observed in Bicyclus anynana (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) exposed to high temperatures [6]. During the feeding experiment, this tropical butterfly adjusted its behaviour by doubling its polyphenol intake under hot conditions, avoiding oxidative damage without affecting its endogenous antioxidant defences. Such ingestions of food items for physiological improvement could be used not only therapeutically (i.e., after facing the environmental stress) but also prophylactically (i.e., before facing the environmental stress), with postponed advantages [7]. For instance, many bird species consume fruits rich in antioxidants before migration to reduce the oxidative stress induced by long flights [8]. However, the prophylactic use of pharmaceutically active substances implies to be able to store and rapidly mobilize them in response to an external challenge, which is not possible for all dietary components (e.g., hydrophylic antioxidants are not easily stored in the organism [9]). The ingestion of specific food items by organisms to improve their physiological and health status corresponds to the notion of “self-medication”, which occurs in many taxa [7,10,11,12,13,14]. Following an evolutionary framework, the conditions necessary for self-medication against environmental stress through the consumption of specific food items are (i) the frequent exposure to environmental stressors over life and evolutionary times, (ii) the evolution in parallel with suitable medicating resources, (iii) the possibility for organisms to recognise medicating resources, (iv) the alleviation of the cost associated with the use of endogenous defences, (v) the cost of the consumption of medicating resources in the absence of environmental stress and (vi) the alleviation of fitness loss caused by environmental stress [15]. Bees (Hymenoptera: Anthophila) constitute an excellent model to investigate the potential for self-medication to face global changes as they are exposed to multiple environmental stressors, experience population declines [16,17], and share a long evolutionary history with their nutritional resource (i.e., flowering plants [18,19]).
Hitherto, studies addressing potential self-medicative behaviours in bees are scarce and mainly concern eusocial corbiculate species, including honey bees and bumble bees. Simone-Finstrom and Spivak [20] showed that honey bees increased resin collection when challenged with chalkbrood disease, but demonstrating resin-induced costs at the colony level still remains a major hurdle [21]. Gherman et al. [22] and Ferguson et al. [23] showed that Nosema-infected honey bees preferentially foraged on specific resources but failed to find any detrimental effect of the latter on uninfected bees. In bumble bees, Baracchi et al. [24] observed the hampering effect of nicotine towards a gut parasite and demonstrated the bumble bee preference for nicotine-laced solution. However, they could not observe any detrimental consequences due to the infection. Therefore, to date, no study has properly demonstrated any case of self-medication in bees. In the present study, we focused on the prophylactic and therapeutic effects of different pollen diets on parasitized bumble bees using forced-feeding assays (experiment on medicinal effects). We also looked for spontaneous pollen intake when given a choice between food items to assess the ability of parasitized bumble bees to exploit medicating pollen (experiment on self-medication). In the context of a parasitic stress, the definition of medicating resources, and specifically of medicinal effects themselves, may be confusing. Indeed, a medicinal effect can occur either by benefiting the host (i.e., increased host tolerance) or by harming the parasite (i.e., increased host resistance). While some have claimed that a food item must be detrimental to the parasite to be considered as medicinal [25], recent studies have argued that it is not mandatory and proposed that medicinal effects could either increase host resistance or tolerance to infection [26]. We applied this second definition in this study and followed a three-step approach to assess the importance of medicating pollen for the bumble bee Bombus terrestris submitted to a parasitic stress (see Appendix A for description of model species): using different pollen species, we (i) assessed their prophylactic and therapeutic effects on fitness components (i.e., host tolerance), (ii) assessed their prophylactic and therapeutic effects on parasite loads (i.e., host resistance) and (iii) examined whether bumble bees favoured medicating pollen species when they could benefit the most from them (i.e., self-medication).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Prophylactic and Therapeutic Effects of Pollen

2.1.1. Experimental Design

Medicinal effects of pollen (i.e., prophylactic and therapeutic ones) were investigated using B. terrestris microcolonies, following the method adapted from Regali and Rasmont [27]. Microcolonies consist of five two-day-old workers of B. terrestris without a queen placed in a plastic box (10 × 16 × 16 cm), whereby one worker will lay unfertilized male-destined eggs. Herein, they were established using three different founding queenright colonies equally distributed among 11 different treatments to ensure homogeneity (n = 9 microcolonies per treatment): (i) uninfected control, which consists in uninfected microcolonies fed with a multifloral diet (i.e., 25% willow, 25% sunflower, 25% orchard, 25% heather); (ii) prophylactic multifloral, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a multifloral diet before worker inoculation; (iii) therapeutic multifloral, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a multifloral diet after worker inoculation; (iv) prophylactic willow, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a willow diet before worker inoculation; (v) therapeutic willow, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a willow diet after worker inoculation; (vi) prophylactic sunflower, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral sunflower diet before worker inoculation; (vii) therapeutic sunflower, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral sunflower diet after worker inoculation; (viii) prophylactic poppy, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral poppy diet before worker inoculation; (ix) therapeutic poppy, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral poppy diet after worker inoculation; (x) prophylactic heather, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral heather diet before worker inoculation and (xi) therapeutic heather, which consists in infected microcolonies fed with a monofloral heather diet after worker inoculation (Figure 1). The multifloral diet has been used as the control as it better reflected the generalist behaviour of B. terrestris. Microcolonies were fed ad libitum with sugar syrup (water:sugar 35:65 w/w) and maintained in a dark room at 26 ± 1 °C and 60 ± 10% relative humidity. After a four-day initiation phase (i.e., syrup only), pollen was provided as candies (i.e., pollen mixed with water and syrup). Microcolonies were then fed with pollen and syrup for a 15-day period (i.e., pollen feeding phase), and pollen candies were freshly prepared and renewed every two days (1–2 g depending on the age of the microcolony) to avoid nutrient alteration and drying out during the experiment. In prophylactic treatments, workers were inoculated individually with C. bombi during the pollen feeding phase (i.e., one day after the start of pollen feeding phase); while in therapeutic treatments, workers were inoculated individually with C. bombi during the initiation phase (i.e., two days before the start of pollen feeding phase) (Figure. 1) (see Appendix B for description of parasite reservoirs and inoculation). To avoid any handling bias, uninfected workers (i.e., uninfected control) were also isolated and starved before being placed in their respective microcolony. Workers that died during the experiment were weighed and replaced by new workers originating from the same foundress colony. The replacing workers did not undergo initiation phase or inoculation and were marked (coloured dot) to avoid any bias in measured parameters. Syrup and pollen supplies as well as microcolonies monitoring were conducted under red light in order to avoid any disturbance, as bees do not detect this range of the light spectrum. The experiments were conducted at the University of Mons from March to April 2022.

2.1.2. Host Tolerance

Alleviation of costs associated with the parasite challenge (i.e., fitness loss, endogenous defenses) through the consumption of medicinal pollen can be identified by evaluating modifications in microcolony growth (i.e., brood mass), individual immunocompetence (i.e., worker fat body content and mortality), as well as in worker behaviour. For instance, the parasite could alter worker behaviour by increasing larval ejection from the brood. Larval ejection is a common process carried out by workers, and sometimes queens, in bumble bee colonies. This biological mechanism involves pulling live larvae out of cells and depositing them outside of the nest, which can control the number of larvae within the brood. This behavior is not well studied but appears to occur when the colony is under stress such as resource deficiency (i.e., stress response) [28,29,30,31]. Another stress response was evaluated through pollen efficiency (i.e., the mass of hatched offspring divided by the mass of collected pollen per microcolony), which highlights when workers need to collect more pollen to produce offspring, and could then be indicative of a resource allocation constraint by directing energy to immune responses. To assess these parameters, pollen collection was measured every two days during the experiment by weighing pollen candies before their introduction into the microcolony and after their removal. Pollen collection data were corrected for evaporation. Besides, dead workers and ejected larvae were counted and removed every day during the pollen feeding phase. At the end of the experiment, workers were weighed and their abdominal fat body content was measured as an indicator of immunocompetence (i.e., two workers per microcolony; [32]) using Ellers’ procedure [33,34]. The brood was dissected for recording the number and mass of individuals within each developmental stage (i.e., eggs, non-isolated larvae, isolated and pre-defecating larvae, isolated and post-defecating larvae). The brood mass was standardised by the total weight of workers in the microcolonies to avoid potential bias from worker activities.
To test for differences in microcolony growth, individual immunocompetence and worker behaviour, we fitted general linear mixed effect models (GLMMs) with treatments (Pollen*Inoculation, 11 levels) as a fixed effect and colony as a random factor. Brood mass and pollen efficiency per microcolony were analysed using models with a Gaussian error structure (i.e., normally distributed residuals, “lmer” function, R-package “lmerTest” [35]). Larval ejection and worker mortality were analyzed using a binomial model with the number of failures (i.e., ejected larvae or dead workers) and the number of successes (i.e., total number of living offspring produced per microcolony or total number of living workers per microcolony) as a bivariate response (“glmer” function, R-package “lmerTest” [35]). Because complete separation occurs for larval ejection data (i.e., Hauck-Donner effect), we used a Bayesian inference by setting a default prior (i.e., zero-mean Normal) on the fixed effect (“bglmer” function, R-package “blme” [36]). Fat body content was analysed using models with a Gamma distribution error structure and a logit link to deal with proportion data. For each parameter, contrasts were then performed on the models to determine whether treatments differed from the uninfected control, whether prophylactic or therapeutic effects differed among pollen, and whether the medicinal effect of pollen differed according to the inoculation (prophylactic vs therapeutic) (“contrast” function, R-package “emmeans” [37]). All analyses were performed in R version 4.2.0.

2.1.3. Host Resistance

Reduction of infection (i.e., therapeutic effect) or loss of infectivity (i.e., prophylactic effect) through the consumption of pollen was assessed by monitoring the parasite load on one marked worker during the experiment. The first measure was taken three days after inoculation, as soon as infection is known to turn patent [38], and following measures were then taken every three days (Figure 1). The marked worker was placed in a 50 mL Falcon tube, faeces were collected using a 10 µL microcapillary and the volume was measured. The faecal sample was then diluted (i.e., dilution 5× or 10× according to the load) to allow for counting the C. bombi cells by using an improved Neubauer haemocytometer at 400-fold magnification under an inverted phase contrast microscope (Eclipse Ts2R, Nikon; Tokyo, Japan). Only flagellated stages were considered to evaluate the parasite load. Despite the possibility for death of marked workers during the experiment (one marker worker died in the multifloral prophylactic treatment at the end of the experiment) or for absence of faeces (three non-defecating events were registered), nine replicates were mostly available at each time point for all treatments (eight replicates in the four cases above-mentioned).
We used generalized additive mixed-effect models (GAMMs) to compare parasite loads over time for all treatments with pollen and inoculation included as crossed fixed effects, faeces volume included as covariate and microcolony included as random factor. Data were log-transformed and two outliers were discarded to achieve normal distribution. We additionally assessed differences in the parasite establishment (i.e., initial load at day 4) and growth among treatments using GLMMs with pollen and inoculation included as crossed fixed effects, and colony as random factor (Gaussian error structure, log-transformed data). The parasite growth was defined as the change in parasite load (i.e., difference between final and initial measures) divided by the initial load. Contrasts were then performed on the models to determine whether prophylactic or therapeutic effects differed among pollen, and whether medicinal effect of pollen differed according to the bioassay (prophylactic vs therapeutic) (“contrast “function, R-package “emmeans” [37]). All analyses were performed in R version 4.2.0.

2.2. Self-Medicative Behaviour

2.2.1. Experimental Design

Based on results from the first experiment (i.e., therapeutic pollen), potential for self-medicative behaviour in B. terrestris was evaluated using a feeding choice experiment with infected and uninfected microcolonies. Each microcolony consisted of five workers placed in a two-box system (10 × 16 × 16 cm per box) maintained at 26 ± 1 °C and 60 ± 10% relative humidity. Food (i.e., pollen and syrup) was provided in one box kept under 12 h natural light conditions (08:00–20:00) and divided in two compartments for the pollen choice (feeding chamber), while the other box was kept in dark conditions and housed the brood (nest chamber). The two boxes communicated through a flexible transparent tube (10 cm) with a diameter enabling worker passage (1.6 cm) (Figure 2). Uninfected and infected microcolonies were offered one out of three different pollen combinations, namely heather/multifloral, sunflower/multifloral and heather/sunflower, which resulted in six treatments. Nine queenless microcolonies were established for each treatment using two-day-old workers of B. terrestris from three different queenright colonies equally distributed among treatments to ensure homogeneity. After a four-day initiation phase (i.e., syrup in the feeding chamber and a small willow candy in the nest chamber for brood initiation), pollen was provided ad libitum as candies (i.e., pollen mixed with water and syrup) in the feeding chamber, and microcolonies were fed with pollen (i.e., binary choice) and syrup for an eight-day period (i.e., pollen feeding phase). Every three days, we provided fresh pollen candies in small caps in the two compartments of the feeding chamber. In infected treatments, workers were inoculated individually with C. bombi during the initiation phase (i.e., two days before the onset of the pollen feeding phase). Workers that died during the experiment were removed but not replaced as pollen collection data were paired within treatment. The experiments were conducted at the University of Mons from late June to early July 2022.

2.2.2. Pollen Preference and Infection Cure

Caps were weighed daily to measure pollen uptake by each microcolony, and data were corrected for evaporation using controls. The position of caps was randomised daily to prevent any positional bias. To test for self-medicative behaviour, we used GAMMs to compare daily amounts of pollen collected from each pollen between infected and uninfected microcolonies, with microcolony included as random factor (Gaussian error structure). Data were processed separately for each pollen combination (i.e., three GAMMs). Additionally, we compared the total pollen amount that each microcolony collected from each of the two diets offered within each treatment (i.e., infection*pollen combination) using paired t-tests after checking for normality. We then tested for the difference in pollen preference between infected and uninfected microcolonies using GLMMs with infection status as a fixed effect, and colony as a random factor (R-package “lmerTest” [35]). We used a binomial model with the consumption of both pollen choices as a bivariate response after checking for overdispersion. Data were processed separately for each pollen combination (i.e., three GLMMs). Moreover, the effects on infection were assessed by measuring the parasite load on two marked workers per microcolony three days after inoculation, and at the end of the experiment. When both marked workers survived, one was randomly selected to be included in the analyses; while the survivor was automatically considered when one died during the experiment. There has never been the case of the two marked workers dead so that nine replicates were available for all treatments. We assessed differences in the parasite establishment and growth among treatments using GLMMs with pollen choice included as fixed effects, and microcolony nested in colony as random factor (Gaussian error structure, log-transformed data). All analyses were performed in R version 4.2.0.

3. Results

3.1. Prophylactic and Therapeutic Effects of Pollen

3.1.1. Host Tolerance

Regarding the medicinal effects of pollen on the microcolony growth, the brood mass of uninfected microcolonies was higher than for all therapeutic treatments, and significantly differed from microcolonies fed sunflower pollen (t = −4.437, p < 0.001). In prophylactic treatments, infected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen displayed the highest growth, even compared to the uninfected microcolonies, and differed significantly from microcolonies fed poppy pollen (t = 2.928, p = 0.004); heather pollen (t = 2.715, p = 0.008); and sunflower pollen (t = 5.688, p < 0.001). By contrast, brood mass of microcolonies fed sunflower pollen was the lowest for both therapeutic (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = −4.437, p < 0.001; and microcolonies fed willow pollen, t = 3.059, p = 0.003; and multifloral pollen, t = 2.858, p = 0.005) and prophylactic treatments (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = −3.831, p < 0.001; and microcolonies fed other pollen, p < 0.01). The only significant difference detected between therapeutic and prophylactic treatments was in microcolonies fed multifloral pollen (t = 3.437, p < 0.001), those in prophylactic treatments displaying a higher growth (Figure 3, Table S1).
Regarding worker behaviour and stress responses, pollen efficiency in uninfected microcolonies was the highest in therapeutic treatments (significantly different from microcolonies fed sunflower pollen, t = −4.928, p < 0.001) while infected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen displayed the highest growth in prophylactic bioassays (significantly different from microcolonies fed sunflower pollen, t = 5.004, p < 0.001). By contrast, pollen efficiency in microcolonies fed sunflower pollen was the lowest for both therapeutic (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = −4.928, p < 0.001; and microcolonies fed other pollen, p < 0.01) and prophylactic treatments (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = −4.015, p < 0.001; and microcolonies fed other pollen, p < 0.01). The only significant difference detected between therapeutic and prophylactic treatments was in microcolonies fed multifloral pollen (t = 2.454, p = 0.016), those in prophylactic treatments displaying a higher pollen efficiency (Figure 4C, Table S1). Regarding the brood, larval ejection was the highest within microcolonies fed sunflower pollen in therapeutic treatments (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = 4.597, p < 0.001; and from microcolonies fed other pollen, p < 0.001), and in prophylactic treatments (significantly different from the uninfected control, t = 4.292, p < 0.001; and from microcolonies fed other pollen, p < 0.001). Microcolonies fed heather pollen also displayed higher larval ejection compared to the uninfected control (t = 3.045, p = 0.002), and to infected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen (t = −3.016, p = 0.003), poppy pollen (t = −2.687, p = 0.007), and willow pollen (t = −2.800, p = 0.005) but only in prophylactic bioassays. No significant difference was detected between therapeutic and prophylactic treatments, irrespective of the pollen diets (Figure 4A, Table S1).

3.1.2. Host Resistance

The infection dynamic was more gradual in therapeutic treatments compared to prophylactic ones (i.e., lower increase), with a significant difference between treatments for willow pollen (t = 3.827, p < 0.001), sunflower pollen (t = 4.949, p < 0.001), poppy pollen (t = 3.319, p = 0.001), and heather pollen (t = 3.666, p < 0.001) (Figure 5). Within both therapeutic and prophylactic treatments, the infection was significantly hampered in microcolonies fed heather and sunflower pollen compared to those fed either willow, poppy or multifloral pollen (Therapeutic: heather vs. willow, t = 4.228, p < 0.001; heather vs. poppy, t = 4.760, p < 0.001; heather vs. multifloral, t = 5.614, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. willow, t = 6.837, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. poppy, t = −7.408, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. multifloral, t = 8.276, p < 0.001; Prophylactic: heather vs. willow, t = 4.408, p < 0.001; heather vs. poppy, t = 4.401, p < 0.001; heather vs. multifloral, t = 3.888, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. willow, t = 5.784, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. poppy, t = −5.722, p < 0.001; sunflower vs. multifloral, t = 5.201, p < 0.001) (Figure 5, Table S1). While sunflower and heather pollen displayed the same effect on the infection dynamic during the prophylactic treatments (t = −1.332, p = 0.184), the hampering effect of sunflower pollen on the infection dynamic was stronger than that of heather pollen during the therapeutic treatments (t = −2.557, p = 0.011) (Figure 5, Table S1).
When dissecting the influence of pollen on host resistance, parasite establishment appeared significantly more hindered in therapeutic treatments compared to the prophylactic ones (willow, t = 3.434, p = 0.001; sunflower, t = 6.139, p < 0.001; poppy, t = 2.042, p = 0.045; and heather, t = 4.552, p < 0.001), except for the multifloral pollen (t = 1.088, p = 0.280) (Figure 6A, Table S1). In both prophylactic and therapeutic treatments, parasite reached higher densities at the beginning of infection in microcolonies fed poppy pollen while establishment was more impeded in microcolonies fed sunflower pollen (Figure 6A, Table S1). By contrast, parasite growth was higher in therapeutic treatments compared to the prophylactic ones with a significant difference in microcolonies fed heather pollen (t = −2.709, p = 0.009). While the parasite displayed the lowest growth in microcolonies fed poppy pollen irrespective of the treatments (Therapeutic: poppy vs. willow, t = 2.546, p = 0.013; poppy vs. sunflower, t = 2.595, p = 0.012; poppy vs. heather, t = −3.108, p = 0.003; poppy vs. multifloral, t = 2.465, p = 0.016; Prophylactic: poppy vs. multifloral, t = 4.183, p < 0.001), the highest growth was observed in microcolonies fed heather pollen in therapeutic treatments (heather vs. poppy, t = −3.108, p = 0.003) and in microcolonies fed multifloral pollen in prophylactic treatments (multifloral vs. willow, t = −2.615, p = 0.011; multifloral vs. sunflower, t = 2.732, p = 0.008; multifloral vs. poppy, t = 4.183, p < 0.001; multifloral vs. heather, t = 3.142, p = 0.003) (Figure 6B, Table S1).

3.2. Self-Medicative Behaviour

Regardless of their infection status, bumble bee workers collected more sunflower pollen (i.e., higher daily amount) than heather pollen (Infected, t = −2.23, p = 0.027; Uninfected, t = −3.09, p = 0.002) and more multifloral pollen than either sunflower (Infected, t = 2.65, p = 0.009; Uninfected, t = 2.88, p = 0.004) or heather pollen (Infected, t = −9.32, p < 0.001; Uninfected, t = −6.83, p < 0.001) (Figure 7, Table S2).
Overall, pollen preference of bumble bee workers did not change according to their infection status (heather vs. multifloral, χ2 = 0.227, df = 1, p = 0.634; sunflower vs. multifloral, χ2 = 0.600, df = 1, p = 0.439; heather vs. sunflower, χ2 = 0.071, df = 1, p = 0.790), and bumble bee workers did not prefer medicinal pollen (heather or sunflower) over multifloral pollen (heather vs. multifloral, t = 4.157, df = 8, p = 0.003; sunflower vs. multifloral, t = 2.252, df = 8, p = 0.054), even when infected (heather vs. multifloral, t = 5.202, df = 8, p = 0.001; sunflower vs. multifloral, t = 1.974, df = 8, p = 0.084) (Figure 8A, Table S2). Regarding pollen preference within treatments, bumble bee workers only preferred multifloral pollen over heather pollen, irrespective of their infection status (infected, heather vs. multifloral, t = 5.202, df = 8, p = 0.001; uninfected, t = 4.157, df = 8, p = 0.003). Besides, parasite establishment (χ2 = 5.49, df = 2, p = 0.064) and parasite growth (χ2 = 2.54, df = 2, p = 0.281) appeared similar between all treatments (Figure 8B).

4. Discussion

4.1. Is Prevention Better Than Cure?

4.1.1. Pollen Diets Override Parasite Impacts

Crithidia infection (i.e., infected vs. uninfected microcolonies fed on multifloral pollen) did not significantly affect microcolony growth, individual immunocompetence or worker behaviour, as expected based on previous studies e.g., [39]. However, we found pollen species-dependant effects on host tolerance in infected microcolonies. Most notably, sunflower pollen led to the highest larval ejection, as well as to the lowest brood mass and pollen efficiency. Such detrimental effects of sunflower pollen on microcolony growth and worker behaviour have already been stressed in previous studies conducted on healthy bumble bees, and are likely due to a lack of essential amino acids, toxic specialised metabolites, and hardened exine [39,40,41]. Microcolonies fed heather pollen also displayed increased larval ejection compared to those fed other pollen diets (except sunflower), but it was significant only in the prophylactic treatments. The only differences observed between prophylactic and therapeutic treatments concerned the brood mass (microcolony growth) and the pollen efficiency (worker behaviour), which tended to be higher in prophylactic treatments compared to therapeutic ones, with significant effects in microcolonies fed the multifloral diet. As the feeding-inoculation sequence was the only methodological difference between prophylactic (i.e., feeding before inoculation) and therapeutic (i.e., inoculation before feeding) treatments, we propose three non-mutually exclusive rationales to explain these observations: (i) given that C. bombi likely require nutrients from pollen to develop [38], parasite establishment in therapeutic treatments may prevent the efficient use of pollen for brood building in the first days of feeding; (ii) given that C. bombi infection was found to reduce ovary size [42], parasite establishment in therapeutic treatments may have delayed egg laying; and (iii) given that C. bombi is known to be more virulent when combined with food deprivation [43,44,45], the concomitant nutritional and parasitic stresses could explain why bumble bee microcolonies showed reduced brood mass in therapeutic treatments.
Regarding individual immunocompetence and survival, the worker mortality in infected microcolonies did not differ from uninfected control, regardless of the inoculation or diet treatment. The only notable observation is that the worker mortality in infected microcolonies prophylactically fed willow pollen was quite high compared to the other treatments, differing significantly from infected microcolonies prophylactically fed heather pollen, and those therapeutically fed willow. Since laboratory alive parasite stocks used for the inoculation were kept by transmitting infection across commercial colonies only fed willow pollen for a year, we suggest that a selection of C. bombi strains well-suited for willow pollen may have inadvertently occurred. This selection would then have led to higher infection costs in prophylactic treatments with willow pollen. This hypothesis echoes the study of Palmer-Young et al. [46] who demonstrated such a selection for phytochemically resistant strains, and is supported by the higher parasite load three days after inoculation in microcolonies prophylactically fed willow pollen. However, it should be interpreted with caution, as it partly contrasts with the study of Palmer-Young et al. [47] who showed that C. bombi cells developing in vitro resistance towards the phytochemical eugenol did not have a higher infectiousness than naïve parasite cells in eugenol-fed bumble bees. Regarding fat body content, although it is commonly used as a proxy for individual immunocompetence e.g., [31,34,38,48], underlying mechanisms remain poorly understood, making difficult clear interpretations of any effect. In prophylactic treatments, poppy pollen led to slightly higher fat body content than sunflower and multifloral pollen, whereas heather pollen led to reduced fat body content when compared to any other diet in therapeutic treatments. These differences among diets could be partly explained by pollen central metabolites: while discussing around the multifloral pollen nutritional properties is not feasible since 25% of the mix is not utterly defined (i.e., orchard mix), both sunflower and heather pollen are characterized by the presence of peculiar sterols (i.e., delta-7 sterols) and lower amino acid concentrations compared, for instance, to poppy pollen [28,49,50,51]. However, although central metabolites likely play a role towards fat body content, the feeding-inoculation sequence seems to impact this metric as results differed between prophylactic and therapeutic treatments. Overall, we advocate further studies to disentangle the roles of pollen diet and feeding-inoculation sequence in influencing parasite strain selection and infectiousness.

4.1.2. Specific Pollen Diets Hamper Parasite Establishment

Pollen diets impacted bumble bee worker resistance towards infection, as shown by differences in parasite loads and dynamics across treatments. More specifically, both sunflower and heather pollen impeded Crithidia development in both therapeutic and prophylactic treatments. While therapeutic effects of sunflower pollen have already been demonstrated in Bombus impatiens e.g., [52,53], it contrasts with a previous study conducted on B. terrestris [39]. This discrepancy could be explained by the difference between experiment durations. Indeed, when focusing on the first ten days post-inoculation in Gekière et al. [39], results appeared quite similar between experiments (i.e., significant reduction in parasite load). This highlights that effects of pollen diet on parasite load clearly vary according to the time span considered after inoculation, and that great caution around parasite load should be paid when studying medicinal effects. In both therapeutic and prophylactic treatments, our results highlighted that diets influence the parasite dynamic mainly at the beginning of infection, but have rather limited impacts once the infection is well-established. Actually, it seems that parasite load often plateaus or displays a chaotic pattern after a while (usually 10 days post-inoculation) [54,55], which has been observed for most treatments herein. For instance, both sunflower and heather pollen hampered parasite establishment compared to willow pollen but no difference was detected in parasite growth, and poppy pollen seemed to boost parasite establishment while growth index was slighter afterwards.
The most stunning results concern the discrepancies in the parasite load dynamic between prophylactic and therapeutic treatments: except for multifloral pollen, parasite establishment was reduced in workers fed therapeutically, resulting in a more gradual infection dynamic compared to workers fed prophylactically. This observation highlights the importance of distinguishing prophylaxis and therapy when dealing with infection and medicative behaviours. For instance, Koch et al. [56] found a preventive but not curative effect of the phytochemical callunene on Crithidia-infected bumble bees. They showed this molecule did not reach the hindgut where Crithidia cells principally establish and thus could not hamper existing infection. By contrast, they measured a high concentration of callunene in the foregut, damaging Crithidia cells before they headed for the hindgut, thereby preventing infection. Later, Koch et al. [57] demonstrated the opposite for the phytochemical tiliaside. This molecule did not damage Crithidia cells in vitro, or in the foregut, since it required to be deglycosylated during gut passage to become active against Crithidia in the hindgut, which highlighted the complexity and diversity of medicinal effects. Here, we propose a simpler explanation directly related to our experimental design, and more specifically to the feeding-inoculation sequence: whereas parasites directly had access to pollen through the whole gut upon inoculation in prophylactic treatments (i.e., feeding before inoculation), which likely favoured their development (i.e., parasite establishment [38,58]), parasites had to reach the hindgut without pollen in therapeutic treatments (i.e., inoculation before feeding), which likely reduced the number of establishing parasite cells. All these observations support the proposition that parasite establishment dictate the subsequent infection dynamic.

4.2. Are Infected Bumble Bees Able to Self-Medicate?

4.2.1. The Importance of Parasite Virulence

During our choice experiment, bumble bee workers did not change their feeding behaviour according to their infection status. Such an absence of self-medicative behaviour is quite unexpected as bumble bees are known to display a huge plasticity in their feeding behaviours [59], and as self-medication has strongly been suggested for these species [24]. This might be due to an important limitation we pinpointed in our study. Actually, one condition for self-medication to occur against infection is the significant alleviation of host fitness costs induced by the parasite through the consumption of medicinal resource [15]. However, we did not observe any detrimental effect of Crihidia on bumble bee fitness under optimal breeding parameters, without flying and foraging activities, which prevents us from properly conclude about alleviation of fitness costs. For this condition to be met, we advocate the use of a more virulent parasite for bumble bees, such as Apicystis bombi [60], as done in the caterpillar Spodoptera littoralis by using a lethal parasite to demonstrate self-medicative behaviours (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) [61]. Surprisingly, no therapeutic effect on host resistance has been observed in bumble bee workers that were offered the two medicinal resources (i.e., sunflower and heather pollen). This could be explained by the similar parasite establishment in all treatments, probably because parasite benefited from willow pollen provided in the nest chamber for establishing within the gut.

4.2.2. The Neglected Role of Sociality

Since the initial self-medication framework formulated by Clayton and Wolfe [62], major changes have been adopted, notably with the concept of “kin” or “social medication” [26]. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that animals may medicate not only themselves but also their genetic kin, including offspring and other genetic relatives e.g., [63]. This adds a degree of complexity when investigating self-medicative behaviours since costs and benefits must be considered at both individual and social levels [21], especially in exclusively social species such as B. terrestris [64]. Although we used microcolonies to take into account this social issue in our experimental design, we may have missed social medicative behaviours that would have been observed in queenright colonies [65,66]. Moreover, as no brood clump has the time to be built in nest chambers, brood effect on worker feeding behaviour e.g., [67] was ruled out during our experiment, which prevents a fully complete picture of the self-medication. It would therefore be interesting to run future experiments using queenright colonies with well-developed brood, and allowing behaviours related to flying and foraging activities.

5. Conclusion and Future Directions

5.1. Designing an Adequate Control Diet

One limitation in our experimental design was the feeding-inoculation sequence that led to an inoculation right after a four-day “pollen starvation” period in therapeutic bioassays, resulting in an additional stress for infected bumble bees. One way to overcome this limitation would be to rely on a diet shift rather than on a starvation–feeding transition, by providing bumble bee microcolonies with a specialized metabolite-free and nutritionally well-balanced diet prior to feeding them with pollen candidates [39]. However, hitherto, researchers have failed to develop a specialized metabolite-free artificial diet which supports bumble bee colony growth [48,68]. Moreover, even such an artificial diet could lead to experimental limitation as central metabolites (i.e., carbohydrates, proteins and lipids) play pivotal roles in host–parasite interactions (e.g., see [69] for the importance of sugars for in vitro Crithidia bombi growth). Yet, we are not aware of any study tackling the relationships between in vivo Crithidia bombi growth and central metabolite content in the gut lumen. Although the Bombus-Crithidia system has been extensively studied [70], understanding such relationships may be the missing piece of the puzzle to fully grasp the Bombus-Crithida dynamic.

5.2. Giving Parasites a Chance

Parasites are often viewed as negative for pollinator health, even being pinpointed as one of the main factors of pollinator decline in the literature e.g., [71,72]. As a consequence, a growing number of studies have tried to find ways to control parasite infection in bees, notably through natural medicines found in floral resources [73]. However, as recently emphasized by Brown [74], while parasites negatively impact the health of pollinators sensu stricto (i.e., at the individual level), they enhance the health of pollinators sensu lato (i.e., at the community level). For instance, by limiting the populations of dominant generalist pollinators, parasites enable the survival and success of their rarer competitors [75]. Besides, infections have evolved a regulatory relationship with pollinator immunity—by being exposed to a low-virulent but abundant parasite, pollinators are protected against subsequent exposure to a high-virulent parasite (i.e., immune priming [76,77]). In fact, parasite communities per se are not responsible for pollinator decline but human-disturbed natural pollinator–pathogen dynamics are [78]. One may therefore argue the relevance of implementing floral enhancement schemes to reduce parasite prevalence in non-disturbed landscapes, whereas access to medicinal resources could significantly support bee populations in man-modified landscapes [73,79]. Self-medication should then be extended to other environmental stressors, mainly driven by human activities.

5.3. Mixing Behaviour, Narrow Diet and Self-Medication

One condition for self-medication to occur is that feeding on medicating resources should be detrimental to uninfected individuals [11,25]. Such detrimental effects may be due to (i) the excessive consumption of pharmaceutically active compounds, which often demonstrated little or no medicinal effects (i.e., hormetic effect [46,80]), or (ii) the poor nutritive value of medicating resources compared to nutritionally optimal resources [81]. In fact, although self-medication is used by organisms to reduce the costs of other strategies (e.g., activation of immune defense during infection), it does not mean that self-medication is devoid of any costs, especially if medicating resources are of poor nutritive values. In that case, the benefits of ingesting medicating resources are expected to exceed those of ingesting highly nutritive resources in infected organisms. In generalist bees, a trade-off between medicinal and nutritive benefits may be reached by mixing pollen from several plant species during foraging and within the nest [82]. For bee species displaying such mixing behaviours such as B. terrestris, feeding plasticity is then likely to be echoed by a change in the relative proportions of medicating and nutritive resources in infected individuals (i.e., increasing the proportion of therapeutic resources), rather than by an exclusive switch to medicating resources. This mixing strategy should therefore be considered when trying to detect self-medicating behaviours, even if it makes it less obvious for observers. As such a feeding plasticity is unexpected to occur in specialist species, the concept of self-medication could be limited to generalist species. However, it is also plausible that some medicating resources are actually highly nutritive, allowing specialization on one resource with both nutritive and medicating benefits, but it should be quite rare. In addition, specialist organisms could ingest or harvest non-food items for medicinal purpose, as already highlighted in social species (i.e., plant material or resin for nest construction, [20]). Hitherto, investigation of self-medication has been limited to generalist bee species, and mainly considered through the lens of food items.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biology12040497/s1, Table S1: Prophylactic and therapeutic effects of different pollen diets; Table S2: Pollen collection in microcolonies offered a choice between two pollen diets.

Author Contributions

M.V. devised the project, the main conceptual ideas and the experimental design; M.V. and A.G. have jointly supervised the work; L.M. and A.G. carried out the experiments; L.M. collected the data; M.V., L.M. and A.G. performed the analyses; M.V. and A.G. wrote the paper with support from L.M.; D.M. helped with the acquisition of the financial support for the project. All authors provided critical feedback and helped shape the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was part of the ARC ‘Actions de Recherche Concertées’ project ‘METAFLORE, 2019–2023′. A.G. is supported by a F.R.S.-FNRS PhD grant ‘Aspirant’.

Data Availability Statement

Upon acceptance, datasets generated and analysed during the current study will be provided via the figshare repository (Experiment 1: 10.6084/m9.figshare.21941318; Experiment 2: 10.6084/m9.figshare.21941315).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank D. Evrard for his help in colony maintenance as well as the numerous people that helped for microcolony dissection. We are very grateful to the Laboratory of Cellular Biology (UMons) for access to the device needed to the microscope analyses. We also thank François Dittlo for providing us with poppy and heather pollen, as well as Stan Chabert for providing us with sunflower pollen. We are grateful to Lacey Li for her support of the publication of this article. For the purpose of Open Access, a CC-BY public copyright licence has been applied by the authors to the present document and will be applied to all subsequent versions up to the Author Accepted Manuscript arising from this submission.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Model Species

Bee species—The buff-tailed bumble bee Bombus terrestris L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombini) is one of the most abundant and widespread bumble bee species in the West Paleartcic [83] Because this eusocial species is highly generalist by foraging on hundreds of different plant species and numerous plant families [83,84], it plays a relevant role as a pollinator in wild and cultivated plant communities e.g., [85]. Bombus terrestris is an easy to rear pollen-storing species, with workers accumulating pollen and nectar in different containers, and feeding larvae progressively during their entire development [86]. It is an annual eusocial bumble bee in temperate regions, with young overwintering queens emerging and founding colonies in spring. At the climax of colony development (mid-summer), new queens and males are reared (i.e., production of sexuals). The sexuals leave the nest to mate in autumn, and young mated queens enter diapause to emerge post hibernation and found their nests the following spring [87]. Colonies of B. terrestris (two-days-old workers) used in this study were obtained from a commercial supplier (Biobest, Westerlo, Belgium) in March 2022 (experiment on medicinal effects) and June 2022 (experiment on self-medication).
Parasite species—The protozoan Crithidia bombi (Euglenozoa: Trypanosomatidae) is a common and widespread endoparasite of Bombus spp. (prevalence of 10–30% in bumble bee populations, sometimes reaching up to 80% in early summer, [42]). Its transmission may occur during visitation of contaminated flowers (horizontal transmission mode) as well as within colonies (horizontal and vertical transmission modes) but only between adults as C. bombi is not transmitted to bumble bee larvae [88,89] although larvae can act as a transmission hub within the colony [90]. This parasite attaches via its flagellum in the bumble bee gut and causes several subtle effects on its host [56,91]. Indeed, although Crithidia bombi infection is not lethal for healthy individuals, infected individuals may display smaller ovary size, impaired associative learning and flower handling, decreased likelihood of reproduction in the wild as well as reduced success in founding colonies [44,92,93,94]. Crithidia bombi was obtained from faeces of three infected B. terrestris queens collected at the Mont Panisel in Mons (Belgium), in March 2021. Laboratory-alive parasite stocks were kept at the University of Mons by inoculating commercial B. terrestris colonies.
Floral species—Willows (Salix spp., Malpighiales: Salicaceae) are distributed almost worldwide but show higher diversity in the northern hemisphere [95], including several species native to Europe (S. alba, S. caprea, S. fragilis). Willow trees provide a consistent source of pollen to support pollinator populations in spring when other flower species are often few and sparse (i.e., peak flowering time from late March to May in European temperate regions). Willow catkins are widely described as insect-pollinated [96] and provide highly nutritive pollen [50]. Willows serve then as an important pollen resource for insect pollinators that emerge after winter, including the buff-tailed bumble bee queens. Pollen loads of Salix species used in this study were purchased from the company “Ruchers de Lorraine” (Nancy, France).
Common sunflower Helianthus annuus L. (Asterales: Asteraceae) is a major annual crop worldwide, native to central North America and massively planted in Europe [97,98]. Sunflower provides a major pollen resource easily accessible in summer (i.e., peak flowering time from July to September in temperate regions), during the growth of bumble bee colonies. Although Asteraceae pollen is often considered as unsuitable because of its low protein content and its lack in essential amino acids, it also has high lipid content which is essential for reproduction and development of bees [40,41,99,100]. Moreover, sunflower is visited by a vast array of bee species, including the buff-tailed bumble bee that has even been shown to be an efficient pollinator of sunflower [101,102,103]. Pollen loads of Helianthus annuus used in this study were provided by the INRAE (“Institut national de recherche pour l’agriculture, l’alimentation et l’environnement”, Avignon, France).
Red poppy Papaver rhoeas L. (Ranunculales: Papaveraceae) is an annual herbaceous species with a very wide distribution area, very common in Europe [104,105]. This temperate native provides a lot of pollen easily accessible to pollinators and is widely visited by bumble bees, honey bees and a range of solitary bees [106,107]. Although poppy flowers do not produce nectar [108], poppy is one of the preferred flower species of bumblebees [109] and appears beneficial in terms of nutrition [28]. Since poppy is widely included in flower seeds mixtures sown in strips and is flowering between May and October [109]), it constitutes an important pollen resource during the growth of bumble bee colonies and the production of sexuals. Pollen loads of Papaver rhoeas used in this study were provided by our partner beekeeper (Noaillan, France).
Heather Calluna vulgaris (L.) Hull (Ericales: Ericaceae) is a shrub species characteristic of moorland and heathland, and is found from Spain to Scandinavia in Europe as well as in North America [110]. Its stamens have a peculiar dehiscence mode that makes heather pollen mainly accessible to buzzing pollinators such as bumble bees [111]. However, its flowers are an important late season food source for pollinators, from late summer to autumn (i.e., peak flowering time from September to early November), with some bee species even specializing on its pollen [112]. Heather nectar is already known for its medicinal effect on Crithidia-infected bumble bees [56]. Pollen loads of Calluna vulgaris used in this study were provided by our partner beekeeper (Cazalis, France).

Appendix B. Parasite Reservoirs and Inoculation

Reservoir implementation—Faeces of infected wild queens of Bombus terrestris (Mont Panisel, Mons, Belgium, March 2021) were mixed with 50% sugar solution (w/w) in bottle caps that were provided inside commercial colonies. All inoculated colonies developed an infection and laboratory alive parasite stocks were kept until March 2022 by transmitting infection to new commercial colonies roughly every two months (i.e., seven transmissions until March 2022).
Controlled inoculation—Faeces were collected from 45 workers from three parasite reservoirs (i.e., 15 workers per stock colony) and mixed to obtain a multiple-strain inoculum, which minimizes the risk of specific genotype-genotype interactions [111,112]. The inoculum was diluted with 0.9% NaCl solution to make a 1 mL solution, and purified following a ‘triangulation’ method developed by Cole [113] and adapted by Brown’s research team [114,115]. The parasite cell concentration of the diluted inoculum was assessed using a Neubauer chamber and adjusted to 2500 cells µL−1 with a 40% sugar solution (w/w). Workers assigned to infected treatments were isolated in individual Nicot cages, starved for 5 h, and fed with 10 µL of inoculum (i.e., 25,000 C. bombi cells [38]). Only workers that consumed the whole inoculum were considered as infected.

References

  1. Margalef, R. Stress in ecosystems: A future approach. In Stress on Natural Ecosystems; Barret, G.W., Rosenberg, R., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 281–289. [Google Scholar]
  2. Costanza, R.; Norton, B.G.; Haskell, B.D. Ecosystem Health: New Goals for Environmental Management; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  3. Fenton, A.; Lello, J.; Bonsall, M.B. Pathogen responses to host immunity: The impact of time delays and memory on the evolution of virulence. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 2083–2090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. DeWitt, T.; Wilson, D.S.; Sih, A. Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1998, 13, 77–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Huey, R.B.; Hertz, P.E.; Sinervo, B. Behavioural drive versus behavioural inertia in evolution: A null model approach. Am. Nat. 2003, 161, 357–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Beaulieu, M.; Gillen, E.; Hahn, S.; Pape, J.M.; Fischer, K. Behavioural antioxidant strategies to cope with high temperatures: A study in a tropical butterfly. Anim. Behav. 2015, 109, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Forbey, J.S.; Harvey, A.L.; Huffman, M.A.; Provenza, F.D.; Sullivan, R.; Tasdemir, D. Exploitation of secondary metabolites by animals: A response to homeostatic challenges. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2009, 49, 314–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bolser, J.A.; Alan, R.R.; Smith, A.D.; Li, L.; Seeram, N.P.; Mcwilliams, S.R.; Williams, S.R.M.C. Birds select fruits with more anthocyanins and phenolic compounds during autumn migration. Wilson J. Ornithol. 2013, 125, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Manach, C.; Scalbert, A.; Morand, C.; Rémésy, C.; Jiménez, L. Polyphenols: Food sources and bioavailability. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 727–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Povey, S.; Cotter, S.C.; Simpson, S.J.; Lee, K.P.; Wilson, K. Can the protein costs of bacterial resistance be offset by altered feeding behaviour? J. Anim. Ecol. 2008, 78, 437–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Singer, M.S.; Mace, K.C.; Bernays, E.A. Self-medication as adaptive plasticity: Increased ingestion of plant toxins by parasitized caterpillars. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e4796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hart, B.L. Behavioural defences in animals against pathogens and parasites: Parallels with the pillars of medicine in humans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 3406–3417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Parker, B.J.; Barribeau, S.M.; Laughton, A.M.; de Roode, J.C.; Gerardo, N.M. Non-immunological defense in an evolutionary framework. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2011, 26, 242–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. De Roode, J.C.; Lefèvre, T.; Hunter, M.D. Self-medication in animals. Science 2013, 340, 150–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Beaulieu, M.; Schaefer, H.M. Rethinking the role of dietary antioxidants through the lens of self-medication. Anim. Behav. 2013, 86, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Potts, S.G.; Biesmeijer, J.C.; Kremen, C.; Neumann, P.; Schweiger, O.; Kunin, W.E. Global pollinator declines: Trends, impacts and drivers. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2010, 25, 345–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Vanbergen, A.J.; Insect Pollinator Initiative. Threats to an ecosystem service: Pressures on pollinators. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2013, 11, 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Michez, D.; Vanderplanck, M.; Engel, M.S. Chapter 5. Fossil bees and their plant associates. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships; Patiny, S., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011; pp. 103–164. ISBN 9780521198929. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cardinal, C.; Danforth, B.N. Bees diversified in the age of eudicots. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 2013, 280, 20122686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Simone-Finstrom, M.D.; Spivak, M. Increased resin collection after parasite challenge: A case of self-medication in honey bees? PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Spivak, M.; Goblirsch, M.; Simone-Finstrom, M. Social-medication in bees: The line between individual and social regulation. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2019, 33, 49–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gherman, B.I.; Denner, A.; Bobiș, O.; Dezmirean, D.S.; Mărghitas, L.A.; Schlüns, H.; Moritz, R.F.; Erler, S. Pathogen-associated self-medication behavior in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2014, 68, 1777–1784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Ferguson, J.A.; Northfield, T.D.; Lach, L. Honey bee (Apis mellifera) pollen foraging reflects benefits dependent on individual infection status. Microb. Ecol. 2018, 76, 482–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Baracchi, D.; Brown, M.J.; Chittka, L. Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? F1000Research 2015, 4, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Abbott, J. Self-medication in insects: Current evidence and future perspectives. Ecol. Entomol. 2014, 39, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. De Roode, J.C.; Hunter, M.D. Self-medication in insects: When altered behaviours of infected insects are a defense instead of a parasite manipulation. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2019, 33, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Regali, A.; Rasmont, P. Nouvelles méthodes de test pour l’évaluation du régime alimentaire chez des colonies orphelines de Bombus terrestris L. (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Apidologie 1995, 26, 273–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Tasei, J.N.; Aupinel, P. Nutritive value of 15 single pollens and pollen mixes tested on larvae produced by bumblebee workers (Bombus terrestris, Hymenoptera: Apidae). Apidologie 2008, 39, 397–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Pomeroy, N. Brood bionomics of Bombus ruderatus in New Zealand. Can. Entomol. 1979, 111, 865–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tasei, J.N.; Lerin, J.; Ripault, G. Sub-lethal effects of imidacloprid on bumblebees, B. terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae), during a laboratory feeding test. Pest Manag. Sci. 2000, 56, 784–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Roger, N.; Michez, D.; Wattiez, R.; Sheridan, C.; Vanderplanck, M. Diet effects on bumblebee health. J. Insect Phys. 2017, 96, 128–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Arrese, E.L.; Soulages, J.L. Insect fat body: Energy, metabolism, and regulation. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2010, 55, 207–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Ellers, J. Fat and eggs: An alternative method to measure the trade-off between survival and reproduction in insect parasitoids. Neth. J. Zool. 1995, 46, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Vanderplanck, M.; Michez, D.; Albrecht, M.; Attridge, E.; Babin, A.; Bottero, I.; Breeze, T.; Brown, M.; Chauzat, M.-P.; Cini, E.; et al. Monitoring bee health in European agro-ecosystems using wing morphology and fat bodies. One Ecosyst. 2021, 6, e63653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Chung, Y.; Rabe-Hesketh, S.; Dorie, V.; Gelman, A.; Liu, J. A nondegenerate penalized likelihood estimator for variance parameters in multilevel models. Psychometrika 2013, 78, 685–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Lenth, R. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means, R package version 1.8.1-1; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022.
  38. Logan, A.; Ruiz-González, M.X.; Brown, M.J.F. The impact of host starvation on parasite development and population dynamics in an intestinal trypanosome parasite of bumble bees. Parasitology 2005, 130, 637–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Gekière, A.; Semay, I.; Gérard, M.; Michez, D.; Gerbaux, P.; Vanderplanck, M. Poison or potion: Effects of sunflower phenolamides on bumble bees and their gut parasite. Biology 2022, 11, 545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Nicolson, S.W.; Human, H. Chemical composition of the “low quality” pollen of sunflower (Helianthus annuus, Asteraceae). Apidologie 2013, 44, 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. McAulay, M.K.; Forrest, J.R.K. How do sunflower pollen mixtures affect survival of queenless microcolonies of bumblebees (Bombus impatiens)? Arthropod. Plant. Interact. 2019, 13, 517–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Shykoff, J.A.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Incidence and effects of four parasites in populations of bumble bees in Switzerland. Apidologie 1991, 22, 117–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Brown, M.J.F.; Loosli, R.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Condition-dependent expression of virulence in a trypanosome infecting bumblebees. Oikos 2000, 91, 421–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Brown, M.J.F.; Schmid-Hempel, R.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Strong context-dependent virulence in a host-parasite system: Reconciling genetic evidence with theory. J. Anim. Ecol. 2003, 72, 994–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Brunner, F.S.; Schmid-Hempel, P.; Barribeau, S.M. Protein-poor diet reduces host-specific immune gene expression in Bombus terrestris. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2014, 281, 20140128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Palmer-Young, E.C.; Sadd, B.M.; Adler, L.S. Evolution of resistance to single and combined floral phytochemicals by a bumble bee parasite. J. Evol. Biol. 2017, 30, 300–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Palmer-Young, E.C.; Calhoun, A.C.; Mirzayeva, A.; Sadd, B.M. Effects of the floral phytochemical eugenol on parasite evolution and bumble bee infection and preference. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 2074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Gekière, A.; Michez, D.; Vanderplanck, M. Bumble Bee Breeding on Artificial Pollen Substitutes. J. Econ. Entomol. 2022, 115, 1423–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Weiner, C.N.; Hilpert, A.; Werner, M.; Linsenmair, K.E.; Blüthgen, N. Pollen amino acids and flower specialisation in solitary bees. Apidologie 2010, 41, 476–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Vanderplanck, M.; Moerman, R.; Rasmont, P.; Lognay, G.; Wathelet, B.; Wattiez, R.; Michez, D. How does pollen chemistry impact development and feeding behaviour of polylectic bees? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Vanderplanck, M.; Zerck, P.-L.; Lognay, G.; Michez, D. Sterol addition during pollen collection by bees: Another possible strategy to balance nutrient deficiencies? Apidologie 2020, 51, 826–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Giacomini, J.J.; Leslie, J.; Tarpy, D.R.; Palmer-Young, E.C.; Irwin, R.E.; Adler, L.S. Medicinal value of sunflower pollen against bee pathogens. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 14394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Giacomini, J.J.; Moore, N.; Adler, L.S.; Irwin, R.E. Sunflower pollen induces rapid excretion in bumble bees: Implications for host-pathogen interactions. J. Insect Physiol. 2022, 137, 104356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Schmid-Hempel, P.; Schmid-Hempel, R. Transmission of a pathogen in Bombus terrestris, with a note on division of labour in social insects. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1993, 33, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Otterstatter, M.C.; Thomson, J.D. Within-host dynamics of an intestinal pathogen of bumble bees. Parasitology 2006, 133, 749–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Koch, H.; Woodward, J.; Langat, M.K.; Brown, M.J.F.; Stevenson, P.C. Flagellum removal by a nectar metabolite inhibits infectivity of a bumblebee parasite. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 3494–3500.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Koch, H.; Welcome, V.; Kendal-Smith, A.; Thursfield, L.; Farrell, I.W.; Langat, M.K.; Brown, M.J.F.; Stevenson, P.C. Host and gut microbiome modulate the antiparasitic activity of nectar metabolites in a bumblebee pollinator. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2022, 377, 20210162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Conroy, T.J.; Palmer-Young, E.C.; Irwin, R.E.; Adler, L.S. Food limitation affects parasite load and survival of Bombus impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae) infected with Crithidia (Trypanosomatida: Trypanosomatidae). Environ. Entomol. 2016, 45, 1212–1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ruedenauer, F.A.; Raubenheimer, D.; Kessner-Beierlein, D.; Grund-Mueller, N.; Noack, L.; Spaethe, J.; Leonhardt, S.D. Best be(e) on low fat: Linking nutrient perception, regulation and fitness. Ecol. Lett. 2020, 23, 545–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Graystock, P.; Meeus, I.; Smagghe, G.; Goulson, D.; Hughes, W.O. The effects of single and mixed infections of Apicystis bombi and deformed wing virus in Bombus terrestris. Parasitology 2016, 143, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lee, K.P.; Cory, J.S.; Wilson, K.; Raubenheimer, D.; Simpson, S.J. Flexible diet choice offsets protein costs of pathogen resistance in a caterpillar. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 823–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Clayton, D.H.; Wolfe, N.D. The adaptive significance of self-medication. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1993, 8, 60–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Christe, P.; Oppliger, A.; Bancala, F.; Castella, G.; Chapuisat, M. Evidence for collective medication in ants. Ecol. Lett. 2003, 6, 19–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Rasmont, P.; Ghisbain, G.; Terzo, M. Bourdons d’Europe et des contrées voisines. In Hymenopètres d’Europe 3; NAP Editions: Paris, France, 2021; 632p. [Google Scholar]
  65. Van Oystaeyen, A.; Klatt, B.K.; Petit, C.; Lenaerts, N.; Wäckers, F. Short-term lab assessments and microcolonies are insufficient for the risk assessment of insecticides for bees. Chemosphere 2021, 273, 128518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wynants, E.; Wäckers, F.; Van Oystaeyen, A. Re-evaluation of a method used to study nutritional effects on bumble bees. Ecol. Entomol. 2022, 47, 959–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Kraus, S.; Gómez-Moracho, T.; Pasquaretta, C.; Latil, G.; Dussutour, A.; Lihoreau, M. Bumblebees adjust protein and lipid collection rules to the presence of brood. Curr. Zool. 2019, 65, 437–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Bortolotti, L.; Pošćić, F.; Bogo, G. Comparison of different pollens substitutes for the feeding of laboratory reared bumble bee (Bombus terrestris) colonies. J. Apicult. Sci. 2020, 64, 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Palmer-Young, E.C.; Thursfield, L. Pollen extracts and constituent sugars increase growth of a trypanosomatid parasite of bumble bees. PeerJ 2017, 5, e3297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Schmid-Hempel, P.; Wilfert, L.; Schmid-Hempel, R. Pollinator diseases: The Bombus-Crithidia system. In Wildlife Disease Ecology: Linking Theory to Data and Application; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2019; pp. 3–31. [Google Scholar]
  71. Dicks, L.V.; Breeze, T.D.; Ngo, H.T.; Senapathi, D.; An, J.; Aizen, M.A.; Basu, P.; Buchori, D.; Galetto, L.; Garibaldi, L.A.; et al. A global-scale expert assessment of drivers and risks associated with pollinator decline. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2021, 5, 1453–1461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Wilfert, L.; Brown, M.J.F.; Doublet, V. OneHealth implications of infectious diseases of wild and managed bees. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 2021, 186, 107506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Fitch, G.; Figueroa, L.L.; Koch, H.; Stevenson, P.C.; Adler, L.S. Understanding effects of floral products on bee parasites: Mechanisms, synergism, and ecological complexity. Int. J. Parasitol. Parasites Wildl. 2022, 17, 244–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Brown, M.J.F. Complex networks of parasites and pollinators: Moving towards a healthy balance. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2022, 377, 20210161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Hatcher, M.J.; Dick, J.T.A.; Dunn, A.M. How parasites affect interactions between competitors and predators. Ecol. Lett. 2006, 9, 1253–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Sadd, B.M.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Insect immunity shows specificity in protection upon secondary pathogen exposure. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, 1206–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Masri, L.; Cremer, S. Individual and social immunisation in insects. Trends Immunol. 2014, 35, 471–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Meeus, I.; Pisman, M.; Smagghe, G.; Piot, N. Interaction effects of different drivers of wild bee decline and their influence on host-pathogen dynamics. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2018, 26, 136–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Folly, A.J.; Koch, H.; Farrell, I.W.; Stevenson, P.C.; Brown, M.J.F. Agri-environment scheme nectar chemistry can suppress the social epidemiology of parasites in an important pollinator. Proc. R. Soc. B 2021, 288, 20210363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Rotchild, K.W.; Adler, L.S.; Irwin, R.E.; Sadd, B.M.; Stevenson, P.C.; Palmer-Young, E.C. Effects of short-term exposure to naturally occurring thymol concentrations on transmission of a bumble bee parasite. Ecol. Entomol. 2018, 43, 567–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Huffman, M.A. Current evidence for self-medication in primates: A multidisciplinary perspective. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1997, 40, 171–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Eckhardt, M.; Haider, M.; Dorn, S.; Müller, A. Pollen mixing in pollen generalist solitary bees: A possible strategy to complement or mitigate unfavourable pollen properties? J. Anim. Ecol. 2014, 83, 588–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Rasmont, P.; Coppée, A.; Michez, D.; De Meulemeester, T. An overview of the Bombus terrestris (L. 1758) subspecies (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Ann. Soc. Entomol. Fr. 2008, 44, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Kleijn, D.; Raemakers, I. A retrospective analysis of pollen host plant use by stable and declining bumble bee species. Ecology 2008, 89, 1811–1823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Kleijn, D.; Winfree, R.; Bartomeus, I.; Carvalheiro, L.G.; Henry, M.; Isaacs, R.; Klein, A.M.; Kremen, C.; M’Gonigle, L.K.; Rader, R.; et al. Delivery of crop pollination services is an insufficient argument for wild pollinator conservation. Nat. Commun. 2015, 6, 7414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Pendrel, B.A.; Plowright, R.C. Larval feeding by adult bumble bee workers (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1981, 8, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Goulson, D. Bumblebees: Behaviour, Ecology, and Conservation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  88. Durrer, S.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Shared use of flowers leads to horizontal pathogen transmission. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1994, 258, 299–302. [Google Scholar]
  89. Otterstatter, M.C.; Thomson, J.D. Contact networks and transmission of an intestinal pathogen in bumble bee (Bombus impatiens) colonies. Oecologia 2007, 154, 411–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Folly, A.J.; Koch, H.; Stevenson, P.C.; Brown, M.J.F. Larvae act as a transient transmission hub for the prevalent bumblebee parasite Crithidia bombi. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 2017, 148, 81–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Lipa, J.J.; Triggiani, O. Crithidia bombi sp. n. a flagellated parasite of a bumble-bee Bombus terrestris L. (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Acta Protozool. 1988, 27, 287–290. [Google Scholar]
  92. Shykoff, J.A.; Schmid-Hempel, P. Parasites delay worker reproduction in the bumblebee: Consequences for eusociality. Behav. Ecol. 1992, 2, 242–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Gegear, R.J.; Otterstatter, M.C.; Thomson, J.D. Does parasitic infection impair the ability of bumblebees to learn flower-handling techniques? Anim. Behav. 2005, 70, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Gegear, R.J.; Otterstatter, M.C.; Thomson, J.D. Bumble-bee foragers infected by a gut parasite have an impaired ability to utilize floral information. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 1073–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Newsholme, C. The Genus Salix; B. T. Batsford Ltd.: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  96. Faegri, K.; van der Pijl, L. The Principles of Pollination Ecology, 3rd ed.; Pergamon Press: Oxford, UK, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  97. Reagon, M.; Snow, A.A. Cultivated Helianthus annuus (Asteraceae) volunteers as a genetic “bridge” to weedy sunflower 1095 populations in North America. Am. J. Bot. 2006, 93, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Strange, K. USDA Foreign Agricultural Service Gain Report: Oilseeds and Products Annual 2016; USDA: Washington, DC, USA, 2016.
  99. Treanore, E.D.; Vaudo, A.D.; Grozinger, C.M.; Fleischer, S.J. Examining the nutritional value and effects of different floral resources in pumpkin agroecosystems on Bombus impatiens worker physiology. Apidologie 2019, 50, 542–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Manning, R. Fatty acids in pollen: A review of their importance for honey bees. Bee World 2001, 82, 60–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Hurd, P.D.; LeBerge, W.E.; Linsley, E.G. Principal sunflower bees of North America with emphasis on the Southwestern United States (Hymenoptera, Apoidea). Smithson. Contrib. Zool. 1980, 310, 1–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Meynié, S.; Bernard, R. Efficacité comparée de la pollinisation d’espèces sauvages d’Helianthus par plusieurs genres d’insectes. Agronomie 1997, 17, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Murat Asla, M.; Yavuksuz, C. Effect of honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) and bumblebee (Bombus terrestris L.) pollinators on yield and yield factors in sunflower (Helianthus annuus L.) production areas. J. Anim. Vet. Adv. 2010, 9, 332–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Holm, L.G.; Pancho, J.V.; Herberger, J.P.; Plucknett, D.L. A Geographic Atlas of World Weeds; Krieger Publishing Co.: Malabar, FL, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  105. Seebens, H.; Blackburn, T.M.; Dyer, E.E.; Genovesi, P.; Hulme, P.E.; Jeschke, J.M.; Pagad, S.; Pyšek, P.; Winter, M.; Arianoutsou, M.; et al. No saturation in the accumulation of alien species worldwide. Nat. Commun. 2017, 8, 14435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Hicks, D.M.; Ouvrard, P.; Baldock, K.C.; Baude, M.; Goddard, M.A.; Kunin, W.E.; Mitschunas, N.; Memmott, J.; Morse, H.; Nikolitsi, M.; et al. Food for pollinators: Quantifying the nectar and pollen resources of urban flower meadows. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0158117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Proctor, M.; Yeo, P. The Pollination of Flowers; Willam Collins Sons: London, UK, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  108. Koster, A. Plantenvademecum voor Tuin, Park en Landschap; Fontaine Uitgevers: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; 416p. [Google Scholar]
  109. Bertille, G.A. Effect of Different Flowers Strips on the Numbers of Visits and the Diversity of Pollinators. Master’s Thesis, Wageningen University and ISARA, Lyon, France, 2010; 50p. [Google Scholar]
  110. Diemont, W.H.; Heijman, W.J.; Siepel, H.; Webb, N.R. (Eds.) Economy and Ecology of Heathlands: Heathland Ecology and Management; KNNV Publishing: Zeist, The Netherlands, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  111. Buchmann, S.L. Buzz Pollination in Angiosperms; Van Nostrand Reinhold Company: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 73–113. [Google Scholar]
  112. Gimingham, C.H. Calluna Salisb. J. Ecol. 1960, 48, 455–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Schmid-Hempel, P.; Puhr, K.; Krüger, N.; Reber, C.; Schmid-Hempel, R. Dynamic and genetic consequences of variation in horizontal transmission for a microparasitic infection. Evolution 1999, 53, 426–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Schmid-Hempel, P.; Reber Funk, C. The distribution of genotypes of the trypanosome parasite, Crithidia bombi, in populations of its host, Bombus terrestris. Parasitology 2004, 129, 147–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Cole, R.J. The application of the “triangulation” method to the purification of Nosema spores from insect tissues. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 1970, 15, 193–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental design used for testing prophylactic and therapeutic effects of five different pollen diets. The experiment timeline including parasite inoculation (red arrow) and monitoring is presented. Uninfected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen diet served as control during the experiment.
Figure 1. Experimental design used for testing prophylactic and therapeutic effects of five different pollen diets. The experiment timeline including parasite inoculation (red arrow) and monitoring is presented. Uninfected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen diet served as control during the experiment.
Biology 12 00497 g001
Figure 2. Experimental setup (i.e., two-box system) used for the feeding choice experiment. Nest chamber was kept into the dark with one small willow candy for brood initiation; feeding chamber was provided with syrup, kept under 12 h natural light conditions and divided in two compartments for worker pollen choice. Workers were enabled to freely move between the two chambers through a tube connection. Three pollen combinations were used namely, heather/multifloral, sunflower/multifloral, and heather/sunflower. All combinations were tested with infected and uninfected microcolonies. Nine microcolonies were used per treatment (infection*pollen combination).
Figure 2. Experimental setup (i.e., two-box system) used for the feeding choice experiment. Nest chamber was kept into the dark with one small willow candy for brood initiation; feeding chamber was provided with syrup, kept under 12 h natural light conditions and divided in two compartments for worker pollen choice. Workers were enabled to freely move between the two chambers through a tube connection. Three pollen combinations were used namely, heather/multifloral, sunflower/multifloral, and heather/sunflower. All combinations were tested with infected and uninfected microcolonies. Nine microcolonies were used per treatment (infection*pollen combination).
Biology 12 00497 g002
Figure 3. Effects of treatments on the total mass of hatched offspring in B. terrestris micro-colonies. Data in blue represent the prophylactic treatments, data in gold represent the therapeutic treatments, and data in orange represent the uninfected control (i.e., uninfected microcolonies fed on multifloral pollen). Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparison across pollen diets within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen diets within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparison between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; ***, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Figure 3. Effects of treatments on the total mass of hatched offspring in B. terrestris micro-colonies. Data in blue represent the prophylactic treatments, data in gold represent the therapeutic treatments, and data in orange represent the uninfected control (i.e., uninfected microcolonies fed on multifloral pollen). Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparison across pollen diets within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen diets within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparison between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; ***, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Biology 12 00497 g003
Figure 4. Effects of treatments on B. terrestris micro-colonies. (A) Worker fat body, (B) Worker mortality, (C) Pollen efficiency, and (D) Larval ejection. Data in blue represent prophylactic treatments, data in gold represent therapeutic treatments, and data in orange represent the uninfected control (i.e., uninfected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen). Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparison across pollen within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparisons between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Figure 4. Effects of treatments on B. terrestris micro-colonies. (A) Worker fat body, (B) Worker mortality, (C) Pollen efficiency, and (D) Larval ejection. Data in blue represent prophylactic treatments, data in gold represent therapeutic treatments, and data in orange represent the uninfected control (i.e., uninfected microcolonies fed multifloral pollen). Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparison across pollen within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparisons between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Biology 12 00497 g004
Figure 5. Dynamics of infection within B. terrestris microcolonies. The color code refers to the pollen diet, and the line type refers to the bioassay categories. Generalized additive mixed effect models (GAMMs) were used to fit smoothers to the data showing mean trends (±95% confidence intervals) over time.
Figure 5. Dynamics of infection within B. terrestris microcolonies. The color code refers to the pollen diet, and the line type refers to the bioassay categories. Generalized additive mixed effect models (GAMMs) were used to fit smoothers to the data showing mean trends (±95% confidence intervals) over time.
Biology 12 00497 g005
Figure 6. Effects of treatments on host tolerance in B. terrestris microcolonies. (A) Effect on parasite establishment (i.e., three days after inoculation), (B) Effect on parasite growth. Data in blue represent prophylactic treatments, and data in gold represent therapeutic treatments. Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparisons across pollen within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparison between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.05; ***, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Figure 6. Effects of treatments on host tolerance in B. terrestris microcolonies. (A) Effect on parasite establishment (i.e., three days after inoculation), (B) Effect on parasite growth. Data in blue represent prophylactic treatments, and data in gold represent therapeutic treatments. Points are mean values of each treatment and error bars indicate the standard error of means. Letters indicate significance at p < 0.05; blue letters refer to comparisons across pollen within prophylactic treatments, gold letters refer to comparisons across pollen within therapeutic treatments, and statistical significance code refers to comparison between treatments (prophylactic vs. therapeutic) (n.s., p > 0.05; *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.05; ***, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons were performed based on a priori contrasts.
Biology 12 00497 g006
Figure 7. Amount of daily collected pollen (g, standardised by total mass of Bombus terrestris workers) in microcolonies being offered a choice between heather or sunflower pollen, sunflower or multifloral pollen, and heather or multifloral pollen. Generalised additive mixed effect models (GAMMs) were used to fit smoothers to the data showing mean trends (±95% confidence intervals) over time. Each dot represents one data point (i.e., standardised amount of collected pollen per day for each microcolony).
Figure 7. Amount of daily collected pollen (g, standardised by total mass of Bombus terrestris workers) in microcolonies being offered a choice between heather or sunflower pollen, sunflower or multifloral pollen, and heather or multifloral pollen. Generalised additive mixed effect models (GAMMs) were used to fit smoothers to the data showing mean trends (±95% confidence intervals) over time. Each dot represents one data point (i.e., standardised amount of collected pollen per day for each microcolony).
Biology 12 00497 g007
Figure 8. Self-medication behaviour in B. terrestris microcolonies. (A) Pollen preference across treatments (i.e., choice between two different pollen species in each treatment), and (B) effects of treatments on parasite establishment (i.e., three days after inoculation) and growth (n.s., p  >  0.05; **, p  <  0.01; ***, p  <  0.001).
Figure 8. Self-medication behaviour in B. terrestris microcolonies. (A) Pollen preference across treatments (i.e., choice between two different pollen species in each treatment), and (B) effects of treatments on parasite establishment (i.e., three days after inoculation) and growth (n.s., p  >  0.05; **, p  <  0.01; ***, p  <  0.001).
Biology 12 00497 g008
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vanderplanck, M.; Marin, L.; Michez, D.; Gekière, A. Pollen as Bee Medicine: Is Prevention Better than Cure? Biology 2023, 12, 497. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12040497

AMA Style

Vanderplanck M, Marin L, Michez D, Gekière A. Pollen as Bee Medicine: Is Prevention Better than Cure? Biology. 2023; 12(4):497. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12040497

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vanderplanck, Maryse, Lucie Marin, Denis Michez, and Antoine Gekière. 2023. "Pollen as Bee Medicine: Is Prevention Better than Cure?" Biology 12, no. 4: 497. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12040497

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop