Exploring the Factors Influencing Continuance Intention to Use AI Drawing Tools: Insights from Designers
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
3. Theoretical Background and Research Hypothesis
3.1. Key Variables in ECM-ISC Model
3.2. Perceived Playfulness
3.3. Perceived Ease of Use
3.4. Subjective Norms
3.5. Perceived Risk
3.6. Perceived Switching Cost
3.7. Research Hypotheses
4. Research Design and Methods
4.1. Questionnaire Design
4.2. Pre-Test and Questionnaire Revision
4.3. Sampling and Data Collection
4.4. Data Processing and Methods
5. Data Analysis
5.1. Normality Test
5.2. Reliability Test
5.3. Explorative FACTOR analysis
5.4. Confirmative Factor Analysis
5.5. Path Analysis
6. Discussion
6.1. The Application of Expectation Confirmation Theory in Design Industry
6.2. Non-Significant Impact of Perceived Usefulness on the Intention to Continue Use
6.3. The Role of Perceived Ease of Use Redefined in AI Drawing Tools
6.3.1. The Tension between Creative Challenges and the Simplicity of Technology
6.3.2. The Mismatch between High Skill Levels and Expectations of Ease of Use
6.3.3. Subjective- and Emotion-Driven Artistic Creation
6.3.4. Specific Dynamics within Design Field
6.4. The Role of Perceived Playfulness in Enhancing Satisfaction with AI Drawing Tools
6.4.1. The Intrinsic Drivers of Satisfaction: The Multidimensional Role of Perceived Playfulness
6.4.2. The Connection between Satisfaction and Continued Intention to Use and the Transformative Role of Perceived Playfulness
6.5. Perceived Switching Costs: Striking a Balance between Traditional Tools and AI Drawing Tools
6.5.1. Learning and Adaptation
6.5.2. Emotional Investment and Brand Loyalty
6.5.3. Strategic Consideration
6.6. The Trade-Offs of Perceived Switching Cost and the Importance of Finding a Balance between Traditional Tools and AI Drawing Tools
6.6.1. Perceived Switching Costs: Striking a Balance between Traditional Tools and AI Drawing Tools
6.6.2. Perceived Switching Costs: Striking a Balance between Traditional Tools and AI Drawing Tools
6.6.3. Perceived Switching Costs: Striking a Balance between Traditional Tools and AI Drawing Tools
7. Conclusions
7.1. Theoretical Contribution
7.2. Practical Contribution
7.3. Limitations and Future Research
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Goodfellow, I.; Pouget-Abadie, J.; Mirza, M.; Xu, B.; Warde-Farley, D.; Ozair, S.; Courville, A.; Bengio, Y. Generative adversarial nets. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 2014, 27, 1384. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, T.; Mann, B.; Ryder, N.; Subbiah, M.; Kaplan, J.D.; Dhariwal, P.; Neelakantan, A.; Shyam, P.; Sastry, G.; Askell, A. Language models are few-shot learners. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 2020, 33, 1877–1901. [Google Scholar]
- Liang, J.; Pei, W.; Lu, F. Cpgan: Content-parsing generative adversarial networks for text-to-image synthesis. In Proceedings of the Computer Vision–ECCV 2020: 16th European Conference, Glasgow, UK, 23–28 August 2020; Proceedings, Part IV 16. pp. 491–508. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, J.-Y.; Park, T.; Isola, P.; Efros, A.A. Unpaired image-to-image translation using cycle-consistent adversarial networks. In Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision, Venice, Italy, 22–29 October 2017; pp. 2223–2232. [Google Scholar]
- Radford, A.; Metz, L.; Chintala, S. Unsupervised representation learning with deep convolutional generative adversarial networks. arXiv 2015, arXiv:1511.06434. [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, C.R.; McAdams, D.A.; Stone, R.B.; Kurtoglu, T.; Campbell, M.I. A validation study of an automated concept generator design tool. In Proceedings of the International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 10–13 September 2006; pp. 283–294. [Google Scholar]
- Brisco, R.; Hay, L.; Dhami, S. Exploring the role of text-to-image AI in concept generation. Proc. Des. Soc. 2023, 3, 1835–1844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oliver, R.L. A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. J. Mark. Res. 1980, 17, 460–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhattacherjee, A. Understanding information systems continuance: An expectation-confirmation model. MIS Q. 2001, 25, 351–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Limayem, M.; Hirt, S.G.; Cheung, C.M. How habit limits the predictive power of intention: The case of information systems continuance. MIS Q. 2007, 31, 705–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cao, N.; Yan, X.; Shi, Y.; Chen, C. AI-sketcher: A deep generative model for producing high-quality sketches. In Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Honolulu, HI, USA, 27 January–1 February 2019; pp. 2564–2571. [Google Scholar]
- Al Aani, S.; Bonny, T.; Hasan, S.W.; Hilal, N. Can machine language and artificial intelligence revolutionize process automation for water treatment and desalination? Desalination 2019, 458, 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polak, S.; Schiavo, G.; Zancanaro, M. Teachers’ perspective on artificial intelligence education: An initial investigation. In Proceedings of the CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems Extended Abstracts, Orleans, LA, USA, 29 April–May 2022; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Nikitas, A.; Michalakopoulou, K.; Njoya, E.T.; Karampatzakis, D. Artificial intelligence, transport and the smart city: Definitions and dimensions of a new mobility era. Sustainability 2020, 12, 2789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bitkina, O.V.; Kim, J.; Park, J.; Park, J.; Kim, H.K. User Stress in Artificial Intelligence: Modeling in Case of System Failure. IEEE Access 2021, 9, 137430–137443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barenkamp, M.; Rebstadt, J.; Thomas, O. Applications of AI in classical software engineering. AI Perspect. 2020, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gmeiner, F.; Yang, H.; Yao, L.; Holstein, K.; Martelaro, N. Exploring Challenges and Opportunities to Support Designers in Learning to Co-create with AI-based Manufacturing Design Tools. In Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Hamburg, Germany, 23–28 April 2023; pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- De la Vall, R.R.F.; Araya, F.G. Exploring the benefits and challenges of AI-language learning tools. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Humanit. Invent 2023, 10, 7569–7576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tang, X.; Han, X.; Li, J. Meta-analysis of ECM-ISC model: Evidence from Chinese literature. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on E-Education, E-Business, E-Management and E-Learning, Tokyo, Japan, 10–13 January 2019; pp. 443–447. [Google Scholar]
- Nie, L.; Oldenburg, B.; Cao, Y.; Ren, W. Continuous usage intention of mobile health services: Model construction and validation. BMC Health Serv. Res. 2023, 23, 442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yuebo, L.; Halili, S.H.; Razak, R.A. Factors Influencing the Online Learning Success of Adults in Open and Distance Education in Southwest China. Int. J. Inf. Educ. Technol. 2023, 13, 1615–1624. [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, X.; Huang, D.; Kong, Z. Formation and enhancement of user stickiness of fitness apps—A study based on ECM-ISC. In Proceedings of the 2021 International Conference on Information Technology and Contemporary Sports (TCS), Guangzhou, China, 15–17 January 2021; pp. 388–398. [Google Scholar]
- Ashfaq, M.; Yun, J.; Yu, S.; Loureiro, S.M.C. I, Chatbot: Modeling the determinants of users’ satisfaction and continuance intention of AI-powered service agents. Telemat. Inform. 2020, 54, 101473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joo, Y.J.; Park, S.; Shin, E.K. Students’ expectation, satisfaction, and continuance intention to use digital textbooks. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 69, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, F.D. Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS Q. 1989, 13, 319–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamid, A.A.; Razak, F.Z.A.; Bakar, A.A.; Abdullah, W.S.W. The effects of perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use on continuance intention to use e-government. Procedia Econ. Financ. 2016, 35, 644–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhattacherjee, A.; Limayem, M.; Cheung, C.M. User switching of information technology: A theoretical synthesis and empirical test. Inf. Manag. 2012, 49, 327–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, C.S.; Wu, S.; Tsai, R.J. Integrating perceived playfulness into expectation-confirmation model for web portal context. Inf. Manag. 2005, 42, 683–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thong, J.Y.; Hong, S.-J.; Tam, K.Y. The effects of post-adoption beliefs on the expectation-confirmation model for information technology continuance. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 2006, 64, 799–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, M.-C. Explaining and predicting users’ continuance intention toward e-learning: An extension of the expectation–confirmation model. Comput. Educ. 2010, 54, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, C.; Coughlin, J.F. PERSPECTIVE: Older adults’ adoption of technology: An integrated approach to identifying determinants and barriers. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 2015, 32, 747–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Venkatesh, V.; Davis, F.D. A theoretical extension of the technology acceptance model: Four longitudinal field studies. Manag. Sci. 2000, 46, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ulaan, R.V.; Pangemanan, S.S.; Lambey, L. The effect of perceived enjoyment on intention to shop online (The Study of Faculty of Economics and Business Sam Ratulangi University Manado). J. EMBA J. Ris. Ekon. Manaj. Bisnis Dan Akunt. 2016, 4. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, Z.; Benyoucef, M. From e-commerce to social commerce: A close look at design features. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2013, 12, 246–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hassenzahl, M. Experience Design: Technology for All the Right Reasons; Morgan & Claypool Publishers: California, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Mirra, G.; Pugnale, A. Expertise, playfulness and analogical reasoning: Three strategies to train Artificial Intelligence for design applications. Archit. Struct. Constr. 2022, 2, 111–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehmann, F.; Markert, N.; Dang, H.; Buschek, D. Suggestion lists vs. continuous generation: Interaction design for writing with generative models on mobile devices affect text length, wording and perceived authorship. In Proceedings of the Mensch und Computer 2022, Darmstadt, Germany, 4–7 September 2022; pp. 192–208. [Google Scholar]
- Yuan, S.; Liu, Y.; Yao, R.; Liu, J. An investigation of users’ continuance intention towards mobile banking in China. Inf. Dev. 2016, 32, 20–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, Q.; Gu, C.; Feng, Y.; Wei, W.; Tsai, W.-C. Study on the continuance intention in using virtual shoe-try-on function in mobile online shopping. Kybernetes 2022, 52, 4551–4575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trafimow, D.; Finlay, K.A. The importance of subjective norms for a minority of people: Between subjects and within-subjects analyses. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 22, 820–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manning, M. The effects of subjective norms on behaviour in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 48, 649–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Latimer, A.E.; Martin Ginis, K.A. The importance of subjective norms for people who care what others think of them. Psychol. Health 2005, 20, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aji, H.M.; Berakon, I.; Riza, A.F. The effects of subjective norm and knowledge about riba on intention to use e-money in Indonesia. J. Islam. Mark. 2020, 12, 1180–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonn, M.A.; Kim, W.G.; Kang, S.; Cho, M. Purchasing wine online: The effects of social influence, perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and wine involvement. J. Hosp. Mark. Manag. 2016, 25, 841–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Al Shamsi, J.H.; Al-Emran, M.; Shaalan, K. Understanding key drivers affecting students’ use of artificial intelligence-based voice assistants. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2022, 27, 8071–8091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Featherman, M.S.; Pavlou, P.A. Predicting e-services adoption: A perceived risk facets perspective. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2003, 59, 451–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, C.; Wu, Z.; Lai, Y.; Ou, W.; Liao, T.; Zheng, Z. Challenges and Remedies to Privacy and Security in AIGC: Exploring the Potential of Privacy Computing, Blockchain, and Beyond. arXiv preprint 2023, arXiv:2306.00419. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, C.; Fu, J.; Lyu, L. A pathway towards responsible ai generated content. arXiv preprint 2023, arXiv:2303.01325. [Google Scholar]
- Xie, J.; Ye, L.; Huang, W.; Ye, M. Understanding FinTech platform adoption: Impacts of perceived value and perceived risk. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 2021, 16, 1893–1911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, M.; Xing, X.; Duan, Y.; Cohen, J.; Mou, J. Will artificial intelligence replace human customer service? The impact of communication quality and privacy risks on adoption intention. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2022, 66, 102900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, S.C.; De Cicco, R.; Vlačić, B.; Elmashhara, M.G. Using chatbots in e-retailing–how to mitigate perceived risk and enhance the flow experience. Int. J. Retail Distrib. Manag. 2023, 51, 285–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chi, H.; Yeh, H.; Hung, W.-c. The moderating effect of subjective norm on cloud computing users’ perceived risk and usage intention. Int. J. Mark. Stud. 2012, 4, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, L.-Y.; Hsieh, Y.-J. Consumer electronics acceptance based on innovation attributes and switching costs: The case of e-book readers. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 2012, 11, 218–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, J.; Qiu, X. AI technology and online purchase intention: Structural equation model based on perceived value. Sustainability 2021, 13, 5671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joo, Y.J.; So, H.-J.; Kim, N.H. Examination of relationships among students’ self-determination, technology acceptance, satisfaction, and continuance intention to use K-MOOCs. Comput. Educ. 2018, 122, 260–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gupta, S.; Tomar, R. What Drives the User’s Continuous Usage Intention of OTT Video Platforms? Identifying the Role of Cognitive Absorption and Perceived Usefulness With the Impact of OTT Content on it. Int. J. Prof. Bus. Rev. Int. J. Prof. Bus. Rev. 2023, 8, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, R.-R.; Lee, J.-C. The supports provided by artificial intelligence to continuous usage intention of mobile banking: Evidence from China. Aslib J. Inf. Manag. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hong, S.; Kim, J.; Lee, H. Antecedents of use-continuance in information systems: Toward an inegrative view. J. Comput. Inf. Syst. 2008, 48, 61–73. [Google Scholar]
- Schepers, J.; Wetzels, M. A meta-analysis of the technology acceptance model: Investigating subjective norm and moderation effects. Inf. Manag. 2007, 44, 90–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dowling, G.R. Perceived risk: The concept and its measurement. Psychol. Mark. 1986, 3, 193–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moorthy, K.; Chun T’ing, L.; Ming, K.S.; Ping, C.C.; Ping, L.Y.; Joe, L.Q.; Jie, W.Y. Behavioral intention to adopt digital library by the undergraduates. Int. Inf. Libr. Rev. 2019, 51, 128–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chin, W.W. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Mod. Methods Bus. Res. 1998, 295, 295–336. [Google Scholar]
- Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Browne, M.W. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Test. Struct. Equ. Models 1992, 21, 230–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tucker, L.R.; Lewis, C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 1973, 38, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bentler, P.M. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 1990, 107, 238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bollen, K.A. Structural Equations with Latent Variables; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 1989; Volume 210. [Google Scholar]
- Mulaik, S.A.; James, L.R.; Van Alstine, J.; Bennett, N.; Lind, S.; Stilwell, C.D. Evaluation of goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 105, 430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, L.t.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chiu, C.-C.; Lee, L.-C. System satisfaction survey for the App to integrate search and augmented reality with geographical information technology. Microsyst. Technol. 2018, 24, 319–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dhiman, N.; Jamwal, M. Tourists’ post-adoption continuance intentions of chatbots: Integrating task–technology fit model and expectation–confirmation theory. Foresight 2022, 25, 209–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, S.-A.; Jeon, Y.J.J. The decision-making process regarding the continuance intention of using branded apps: An integrated approach to the PAM and the TPB. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag. 2023, 35, 4158–4176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abdul Rahim, N.F.; Abbasi, G.A.; Iranmanesh, M.; Christopher, N.; Amran, A. Determinants of continuous intention to use e-government services: An extension of technology continuance theory. J. Syst. Inf. Technol. 2023, 25, 245–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aprilia, C.; Amalia, R. Perceived Security and Technology Continuance Theory: An Analysis of Mobile Wallet Users’ Continuance Intention. Glob. Bus. Rev. 2023, 09721509221145831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzone, M.; Elgammal, A. Art, creativity, and the potential of artificial intelligence. Arts 2019, 8, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiwari, C.K.; Bhat, M.A.; Khan, S.T.; Subramaniam, R.; Khan, M.A.I. What drives students toward ChatGPT? An investigation of the factors influencing adoption and usage of ChatGPT. Interact. Technol. Smart Educ. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edelman, S. Curiosity and exploration. Retrieved May 1997, 11, 20. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, A.; Lu, Y.; Wang, B. Enhancing perceived enjoyment in social games through social and gaming factors. Inf. Technol. People 2016, 29, 99–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoon, S.; Lee, M.; Kim, Y. A Case Study on the Continuous Usage Intention of Artificial Intelligence Speaker in Product Service System Perspective. In Proceedings of the 2022 IEEE/ACIS 7th International Conference on Big Data, Cloud Computing, and Data Science (BCD), Danang, Vietnam, 4–6 August 2022; pp. 50–55. [Google Scholar]
- Dias, E.; Linde, M.; Rafiee, A.; Koomen, E.; Scholten, H. Beauty and brains: Integrating easy spatial design and advanced urban sustainability models. Plan. Support Syst. Sustain. Urban Dev. 2013, 469–484. [Google Scholar]
- Ghorbanzadeh, D.; Rahehagh, A. Emotional brand attachment and brand love: The emotional bridges in the process of transition from satisfaction to loyalty. Rajagiri Manag. J. 2021, 15, 16–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variables | Operational Definitions |
---|---|
Perceived usefulness (PU) | Perceived usefulness refers to users’ belief that using AI drawing tools to assist design can significantly enhance their work efficiency, convenience, and the degree to which they obtain inspiration. This concept is based on users’ subjective evaluation of the actual benefits and performance improvements that AI drawing tools can provide during the assisted design process. |
Perceived ease of use (PeoU) | Perceived ease of use describes the degree of effort users believe is required to learn and use AI drawing tools for design-related tasks. It involves users’ evaluation of the clarity, comprehensibility of the AI drawing tool’s interface, and the simplicity of the overall usage process. |
Satisfaction (SA) | Satisfaction reflects users’ overall perception of fulfillment after using AI drawing tools to assist design. It is based on a comprehensive evaluation of the tool’s performance, the extent to which it meets needs, and the level of enjoyment experienced during use. |
Expectation confirmation (EC) | Expectation confirmation is the perception of the match between users’ actual experience and their prior expectations after using AI drawing tools. This concept focuses on users’ evaluation of the design assistance outcomes provided by AI drawing tools exceeding their expectations. |
Perceived playfulness (PP) | Perceived playfulness refers to the fun and pleasure users experience when using AI drawing tools to assist design. It highlights the aspect of AI drawing tools as innovative technology that, beyond practicality, can also add elements of enjoyment and entertainment value to the design process. |
Perceived switching cost (PSC) | Perceived switching cost involves users’ assessment of the anticipated difficulties, time consumption, and level of effort required to integrate AI drawing tools into their existing design workflows. This reflects the various potential costs users perceive when considering replacing old tools with AI drawing tools. |
Continuance intention (CI) | Continuance intention refers to users’ intention to keep using AI drawing tools in the future. This includes users’ expectations to maintain or even increase their current frequency of use. |
Subjective norms (SN) | Subjective norms refer to the perceived support or expectation from significant others (such as friends, family, leaders, or colleagues) regarding the use of AI drawing tools to assist design. This concept reflects the role of social influence in the user’s decision-making process. |
Perceived risk (PR) | Perceived risk is the assessment of potential negative consequences that users worry about when using AI drawing tools to assist design, such as a decrease in innovation ability, a reduction in autonomous design capability, or the risk of design work leakage. |
Constructs | Coding | Item Content | Source |
---|---|---|---|
Perceived usefulness | PU1 | Using AI drawing tools for design assistance plays a significant role in inspiring me. | [25] |
PU2 | Using AI drawing tools for design assistance is highly convenient for me. | ||
PU3 | With AI drawing tools assisting in design, I can easily complete my work. | ||
Perceived ease of use | PEoU1 | The interaction with AI drawing tools is clear and intuitive. | [25] |
PeoU2 | Using AI drawing tools doesn’t require much mental effort on my part. | ||
PeoU3 | I find it easy to use AI drawing tools for design-related tasks. | ||
Satisfaction | SA1 | Overall, I am satisfied with using AI drawing tools for design assistance. | [9] |
SA2 | I feel delighted when using AI drawing tools for design assistance. | ||
SA3 | AI drawing tools meet my needs, which makes me very happy. | ||
Expectation confirmation | EC1 | The experience I gain from using AI drawing tools for design assistance exceeds my expectations. | [9] |
EC2 | The benefits provided by AI drawing tools surpass my expectations. | ||
EC3 | AI drawing tools greatly inspire my designs. | ||
Perceived playfulness | PP1 | I find it fascinating to use AI drawing tools. | [28] |
PP2 | I thoroughly enjoy the process of using AI drawing tools for design assistance. | ||
PP3 | Using AI drawing tools for design assistance brings me ease and pleasure. | ||
Perceived switching costs | PSC1 | Integrating AI drawing tools into my original design workflow to replace some of my previous tools is not bothersome. | [58] |
PSC2 | Incorporating AI drawing tools into my original design workflow doesn’t increase the complexity. | ||
PSC3 | Using AI drawing tools in my current design workflow doesn’t consume more of my time and energy. | ||
Continuance intention | CI1 | In the future, I plan to continue using AI drawing tools to replace some of my past tools. | [9] |
CI2 | I am more inclined to continue using AI drawing tools in the future. | ||
CI3 | In the future, I intend to maintain or even increase the frequency of using AI drawing tools. | ||
Subjective norms | SN1 | People important to me believe I should use AI drawing tools to assist in design. | [59] |
SN2 | My friends have supported me in using AI drawing tools to assist in design. | ||
SN3 | In work and study, leaders and teachers asked me to use AI drawing tools to assist in design. | ||
Perceived risk | PR1 | I am concerned that prolonged use of AI drawing tools might diminish my design innovation capabilities. | [60] |
PR2 | I am concerned that prolonged use of AI drawing tools might reduce my independent design capabilities. | ||
PR3 | I am concerned that prolonged use of AI drawing tools might lead to the leakage of my design works. |
Category | Frequency | Ratio (%) | |
---|---|---|---|
Group | Professional designers | 242 | 60.80 |
Students | 156 | 39.20 | |
Gender | Male | 205 | 51.51 |
Female | 193 | 48.49 | |
Age | 18–24 | 90 | 22.61 |
25–30 | 105 | 26.38 | |
31–40 | 111 | 27.89 | |
41–50 | 46 | 11.56 | |
51–60 | 46 | 11.56 | |
Design discipline | Product design | 71 | 17.84 |
Industrial design | 63 | 15.83 | |
Visual communication design | 48 | 12.06 | |
Environmental art design/architecture design | 54 | 13.57 | |
Fashion and apparel Design | 51 | 12.81 | |
Digital media design | 78 | 19.60 | |
Other design disciplines | 33 | 8.29 | |
Total | 398 | 100.0 |
Construct | Item | Max | Min | Mean | S. D. | Median | Kurtosis | Skewness |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | PU1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.480 | 1.913 | 5.000 | −0.962 | −0.326 |
PU2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.455 | 1.842 | 5.000 | −0.873 | −0.387 | |
PU3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.545 | 1.886 | 5.000 | −0.938 | −0.346 | |
Perceived Ease of Use | PEoU1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.508 | 1.863 | 5.000 | −0.890 | −0.375 |
PEoU2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.565 | 1.919 | 5.000 | −0.994 | −0.350 | |
PEoU3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.440 | 1.909 | 5.000 | −0.970 | −0.340 | |
Satisfaction | SA1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.465 | 1.899 | 5.000 | −0.930 | −0.334 |
SA2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.598 | 1.892 | 5.000 | −0.942 | −0.364 | |
SA3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.523 | 1.930 | 5.000 | −0.958 | −0.423 | |
Expectation Confirmation | EC1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.490 | 1.783 | 4.000 | −0.852 | −0.253 |
EC2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.573 | 1.835 | 5.000 | −0.831 | −0.402 | |
EC3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.585 | 1.768 | 5.000 | −0.843 | −0.318 | |
Perceived Playfulness | PP1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.688 | 1.926 | 5.000 | −0.932 | −0.470 |
PP2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.691 | 1.918 | 5.000 | −0.948 | −0.460 | |
PP3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.611 | 1.910 | 5.000 | −0.934 | −0.403 | |
Perceived Switching Cost | PSC1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.746 | 1.784 | 5.000 | −0.717 | −0.477 |
PSC2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.560 | 1.692 | 5.000 | −0.679 | −0.341 | |
PSC3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.646 | 1.738 | 5.000 | −0.683 | −0.391 | |
Continuance Intention | CI1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.585 | 1.833 | 5.000 | −0.773 | −0.454 |
CI2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.573 | 1.881 | 5.000 | −0.785 | −0.420 | |
CI3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.646 | 1.805 | 5.000 | −0.658 | −0.485 | |
Subjective Norm | SN1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.802 | 1.945 | 5.000 | −0.886 | −0.530 |
SN2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.560 | 1.807 | 5.000 | −0.694 | −0.501 | |
SN3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 4.696 | 1.818 | 5.000 | −0.697 | −0.447 | |
Perceived Risk | PR1 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 3.399 | 1.928 | 3.000 | −0.973 | 0.342 |
PR2 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 3.528 | 1.904 | 3.000 | −0.931 | 0.353 | |
PR3 | 1.000 | 7.000 | 3.327 | 1.920 | 3.000 | −0.962 | 0.458 |
Construct | Item | Corrected Item-to-Total Correlation | Cronbach’s α if Item Deleted | Cronbach’s α |
---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | PU1 | 0.757 | 0.839 | 0.880 |
PU2 | 0.777 | 0.821 | ||
PU3 | 0.768 | 0.829 | ||
Perceived Ease of Use | PEoU1 | 0.773 | 0.832 | 0.882 |
PEoU2 | 0.785 | 0.821 | ||
PEoU3 | 0.756 | 0.846 | ||
Satisfaction | SA1 | 0.739 | 0.843 | 0.875 |
SA2 | 0.754 | 0.830 | ||
SA3 | 0.788 | 0.798 | ||
Expectation Confirmation | EC1 | 0.706 | 0.816 | 0.855 |
EC2 | 0.737 | 0.787 | ||
EC3 | 0.736 | 0.788 | ||
Perceived Playfulness | PP1 | 0.761 | 0.832 | 0.879 |
PP2 | 0.751 | 0.841 | ||
PP3 | 0.784 | 0.811 | ||
Perceived Switching Cost | PSC1 | 0.726 | 0.773 | 0.846 |
PSC2 | 0.722 | 0.778 | ||
PSC3 | 0.692 | 0.805 | ||
Continuance Intention | CI1 | 0.758 | 0.819 | 0.873 |
CI2 | 0.745 | 0.831 | ||
CI3 | 0.765 | 0.813 | ||
Subjective Norm | SN1 | 0.755 | 0.796 | 0.864 |
SN2 | 0.740 | 0.810 | ||
SN3 | 0.731 | 0.818 | ||
Perceived Risk | PR1 | 0.765 | 0.835 | 0.881 |
PR2 | 0.774 | 0.827 | ||
PR3 | 0.768 | 0.832 |
Construct | Item | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | Factor 3 | Factor 4 | Factor 5 | Factor 6 | Factor 7 | Factor 8 | Factor 9 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | PU1 | 0.143 | 0.843 | 0.092 | 0.075 | 0.036 | 0.170 | 0.202 | 0.103 | 0.064 |
PU2 | 0.111 | 0.813 | 0.120 | 0.108 | 0.177 | 0.091 | 0.118 | 0.128 | 0.221 | |
PU3 | 0.027 | 0.808 | 0.127 | 0.177 | 0.167 | 0.122 | 0.100 | 0.184 | 0.151 | |
Perceived Ease of Use | PEoU1 | 0.099 | 0.157 | 0.115 | 0.834 | 0.151 | 0.127 | 0.143 | 0.092 | 0.128 |
PEoU2 | 0.183 | 0.126 | 0.167 | 0.823 | 0.173 | 0.157 | 0.070 | 0.149 | 0.096 | |
PEoU3 | 0.111 | 0.086 | 0.206 | 0.753 | 0.124 | 0.069 | 0.282 | 0.150 | 0.222 | |
Satisfaction | SA1 | 0.099 | 0.174 | 0.777 | 0.145 | 0.140 | 0.159 | 0.155 | 0.186 | 0.110 |
SA2 | 0.121 | 0.059 | 0.783 | 0.145 | 0.187 | 0.186 | 0.087 | 0.138 | 0.239 | |
SA3 | 0.111 | 0.121 | 0.837 | 0.179 | 0.090 | 0.113 | 0.192 | 0.155 | 0.068 | |
Expectation Confirmation | EC1 | 0.106 | 0.043 | 0.117 | 0.123 | 0.127 | 0.099 | 0.174 | 0.803 | 0.226 |
EC2 | 0.089 | 0.251 | 0.126 | 0.137 | 0.186 | 0.130 | 0.202 | 0.764 | 0.083 | |
EC3 | 0.151 | 0.177 | 0.263 | 0.124 | 0.167 | 0.140 | 0.095 | 0.769 | 0.080 | |
Perceived Playfulness | PP1 | 0.116 | 0.158 | 0.129 | 0.117 | 0.797 | 0.140 | 0.142 | 0.208 | 0.150 |
PP2 | 0.214 | 0.104 | 0.121 | 0.170 | 0.776 | 0.077 | 0.168 | 0.164 | 0.175 | |
PP3 | 0.114 | 0.144 | 0.182 | 0.186 | 0.783 | 0.198 | 0.203 | 0.113 | 0.147 | |
Perceived Switching Cost | PSC1 | 0.160 | 0.198 | 0.126 | 0.216 | 0.178 | 0.190 | 0.116 | 0.220 | 0.725 |
PSC2 | 0.114 | 0.202 | 0.198 | 0.232 | 0.174 | 0.199 | 0.158 | 0.107 | 0.729 | |
PSC3 | 0.230 | 0.150 | 0.158 | 0.071 | 0.201 | 0.272 | 0.197 | 0.147 | 0.680 | |
Continuance Intention | CI1 | 0.163 | 0.166 | 0.191 | 0.118 | 0.111 | 0.777 | 0.056 | 0.238 | 0.189 |
CI2 | 0.092 | 0.143 | 0.128 | 0.188 | 0.175 | 0.811 | 0.186 | 0.063 | 0.116 | |
CI3 | 0.189 | 0.119 | 0.165 | 0.065 | 0.118 | 0.764 | 0.223 | 0.096 | 0.266 | |
Subjective Norm | SN1 | 0.116 | 0.154 | 0.150 | 0.182 | 0.201 | 0.163 | 0.758 | 0.203 | 0.089 |
SN2 | 0.126 | 0.155 | 0.126 | 0.147 | 0.108 | 0.162 | 0.774 | 0.226 | 0.180 | |
SN3 | 0.136 | 0.157 | 0.175 | 0.145 | 0.201 | 0.132 | 0.787 | 0.067 | 0.131 | |
Perceived Risk | PR1 | −0.830 | −0.040 | −0.118 | −0.117 | −0.183 | −0.036 | −0.129 | −0.124 | −0.159 |
PR2 | −0.845 | −0.081 | −0.011 | −0.158 | −0.094 | −0.203 | −0.107 | −0.066 | −0.147 | |
PR3 | −0.842 | −0.149 | −0.176 | −0.076 | −0.104 | −0.139 | −0.095 | −0.116 | −0.060 | |
Eigenvalue (Rotated) | 2.561 | 2.540 | 2.477 | 2.467 | 2.425 | 2.409 | 2.401 | 2.375 | 2.111 | |
% of Variance (Rotated) | 9.486 | 9.408 | 9.175 | 9.137 | 8.981 | 8.921 | 8.892 | 8.797 | 7.819 |
Construct | Item | UnStd. | S.E. | p | Std. | AVE | CR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | PU1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.817 | 0.709 | 0.880 |
PU2 | 1.014 | 0.054 | 0.000 | 0.860 | |||
PU3 | 1.024 | 0.055 | 0.000 | 0.848 | |||
Perceived Ease of Use | PEoU1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.836 | 0.714 | 0.882 |
PEoU2 | 1.057 | 0.054 | 0.000 | 0.858 | |||
PEoU3 | 1.032 | 0.054 | 0.000 | 0.841 | |||
Satisfaction | SA1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.818 | 0.703 | 0.876 |
SA2 | 1.020 | 0.055 | 0.000 | 0.836 | |||
SA3 | 1.070 | 0.056 | 0.000 | 0.860 | |||
Expectation Confirmation | EC1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.775 | 0.663 | 0.855 |
EC2 | 1.111 | 0.067 | 0.000 | 0.836 | |||
EC3 | 1.064 | 0.064 | 0.000 | 0.831 | |||
Perceived Playfulness | PP1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.829 | 0.708 | 0.879 |
PP2 | 0.987 | 0.053 | 0.000 | 0.822 | |||
PP3 | 1.042 | 0.053 | 0.000 | 0.872 | |||
Perceived Switching Cost | PSC1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.816 | 0.648 | 0.847 |
PSC2 | 0.943 | 0.054 | 0.000 | 0.812 | |||
PSC3 | 0.940 | 0.056 | 0.000 | 0.787 | |||
Continuance Intention | CI1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.837 | 0.696 | 0.873 |
CI2 | 0.991 | 0.055 | 0.000 | 0.808 | |||
CI3 | 1.007 | 0.052 | 0.000 | 0.856 | |||
Subjective Norm | SN1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.846 | 0.680 | 0.864 |
SN2 | 0.905 | 0.049 | 0.000 | 0.824 | |||
SN3 | 0.889 | 0.050 | 0.000 | 0.804 | |||
Perceived Risk | PR1 | 1.000 | - | - | 0.838 | 0.711 | 0.881 |
PR2 | 1.003 | 0.052 | 0.000 | 0.851 | |||
PR3 | 0.999 | 0.053 | 0.000 | 0.841 |
Construct | PU | PEoU | SA | EC | PP | PSC | CI | SN | PR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | 0.842 | ||||||||
Perceived Ease of Use | 0.402 | 0.845 | |||||||
Satisfaction | 0.393 | 0.482 | 0.838 | ||||||
Expectation Confirmation | 0.454 | 0.441 | 0.497 | 0.814 | |||||
Perceived Playfulness | 0.430 | 0.487 | 0.467 | 0.504 | 0.841 | ||||
Perceived Switching Cost | 0.508 | 0.526 | 0.511 | 0.513 | 0.560 | 0.805 | |||
Continuance Intention | 0.437 | 0.431 | 0.487 | 0.447 | 0.469 | 0.605 | 0.834 | ||
Subjective Norm | 0.460 | 0.499 | 0.480 | 0.508 | 0.528 | 0.526 | 0.497 | 0.825 | |
Perceived Risk | −0.319 | −0.388 | −0.360 | −0.369 | −0.422 | −0.464 | −0.420 | −0.392 | 0.843 |
Construct | PU | PEoU | SA | EC | PP | PSC | CI | SN | PR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Usefulness | - | ||||||||
Perceived Ease of Use | 0.457 | - | |||||||
Satisfaction | 0.449 | 0.548 | - | ||||||
Expectation Confirmation | 0.523 | 0.508 | 0.575 | - | |||||
Perceived Playfulness | 0.490 | 0.553 | 0.533 | 0.581 | - | ||||
Perceived Switching Cost | 0.590 | 0.609 | 0.594 | 0.602 | 0.649 | - | |||
Continuance Intention | 0.499 | 0.491 | 0.557 | 0.518 | 0.536 | 0.704 | - | ||
Subjective Norm | 0.528 | 0.571 | 0.551 | 0.590 | 0.605 | 0.616 | 0.572 | - | |
Perceived Risk | 0.362 | 0.440 | 0.410 | 0.425 | 0.480 | 0.537 | 0.480 | 0.450 | - |
Indices | χ2/df | RMSEA | CFI | NNFI | TLI | IFI | PGFI | PNFI | PCFI | SRMR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Judgement criterion | <3 | <0.10 | >0.9 | >0.9 | >0.9 | >0.9 | >0.5 | >0.5 | >0.5 | <0.1 |
Results | 2.546 | 0.062 | 0.922 | 0.933 | 0.933 | 0.708 | 0.777 | 0.810 | 0.0997 | 2.546 |
Hypotheses | Path Analysis | UnStd. | Std. | S.E. | p | Support | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
H1 | SA | → | CI | 0.155 | 0.174 | 0.052 | 0.001 | yes |
H2 | PU | → | CI | 0.028 | 0.030 | 0.065 | 0.578 | no |
H3 | PU | → | SA | 0.152 | 0.145 | 0.063 | 0.025 | yes |
H4 | EC | → | SA | 0.451 | 0.394 | 0.092 | 0.000 | yes |
H5 | EC | → | PU | 0.367 | 0.334 | 0.082 | 0.000 | yes |
H6 | EC | → | PP | 0.728 | 0.635 | 0.064 | 0.000 | yes |
H7 | PP | → | CI | 0.056 | 0.063 | 0.050 | 0.170 | no |
H8 | PP | → | SA | 0.209 | 0.209 | 0.066 | 0.002 | yes |
H9 | PEoU | → | CI | −0.067 | −0.071 | 0.069 | 0.284 | no |
H10 | PEoU | → | PU | 0.118 | 0.116 | 0.067 | 0.054 | no |
H11 | SN | → | CI | 0.168 | 0.185 | 0.075 | 0.003 | yes |
H12 | SN | → | PU | 0.259 | 0.264 | 0.087 | 0.002 | yes |
H13 | SN | → | PEoU | 0.604 | 0.631 | 0.053 | 0.000 | yes |
H14 | SN | → | PR | −0.495 | −0.498 | 0.056 | 0.000 | yes |
H15 | PR | → | CI | −0.117 | −0.128 | 0.049 | 0.003 | yes |
H16 | PU | → | PSC | 0.353 | 0.421 | 0.046 | 0.001 | yes |
H17 | PEoU | → | PSC | 0.376 | 0.439 | 0.048 | 0.000 | yes |
H18 | PSC | → | CI | 0.491 | 0.443 | 0.087 | 0.001 | yes |
Relationship Path | Direct Effect | Indirect Effect | Total Effect | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | B–C Sig. | β | B–C Sig. | β | B–C Sig. | |||
PP | → | CI | 0.063 | 0.170 | 0.036 | 0.002 | 0.100 | 0.048 |
SN | → | CI | 0.185 | 0.003 | 0.223 | 0.000 | 0.408 | 0.000 |
PR | → | CI | −0.128 | 0.003 | / | / | −0.128 | 0.003 |
SA | → | CI | 0.174 | 0.001 | / | / | 0.174 | 0.001 |
PSC | → | CI | 0.443 | 0.001 | / | / | 0.443 | 0.001 |
PU | → | CI | 0.030 | 0.578 | 0.211 | 0.001 | 0.241 | 0.000 |
PEoU | → | CI | −0.071 | 0.284 | 0.222 | 0.000 | 0.151 | 0.013 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Fan, P.; Jiang, Q. Exploring the Factors Influencing Continuance Intention to Use AI Drawing Tools: Insights from Designers. Systems 2024, 12, 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems12030068
Fan P, Jiang Q. Exploring the Factors Influencing Continuance Intention to Use AI Drawing Tools: Insights from Designers. Systems. 2024; 12(3):68. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems12030068
Chicago/Turabian StyleFan, Pujunqian, and Qianling Jiang. 2024. "Exploring the Factors Influencing Continuance Intention to Use AI Drawing Tools: Insights from Designers" Systems 12, no. 3: 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems12030068
APA StyleFan, P., & Jiang, Q. (2024). Exploring the Factors Influencing Continuance Intention to Use AI Drawing Tools: Insights from Designers. Systems, 12(3), 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems12030068