Intelligent Eye-Tracker-Based Methods for Detection of Deception: A Survey
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Eye Tracking Examination for Deception Detection
- non-invasive data collection without physical contact (which is important during questioning especially dangerous or unpredictable persons),
- self-calibration without human assistance,
- suitability for widespread use due to mobile technology advancements,
- the ability to collect data covertly, which may prevent the subject from using countermeasures,
- shorter examination time compared to regular deception detection tests with the polygraph.
2.1.1. Concealed Information Test
2.1.2. Tests Based on Reading Behavior
2.2. Effects of Countermeasures in Deception Detection
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
CIT | Concealed Information Test |
CQT | Comparison Question Test |
CM | Countermeasures |
RCT | Relevant Comparison Test |
PD | Pupil Diameter |
MRI | Magnetic Resonance Imaging |
EEG | Electroencephalography |
HR | Heart rate |
References
- Vrij, A.; Granhag, P.A.; Mann, S.; Leal, S. Outsmarting the liars: Toward a cognitive lie detection approach. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 20, 28–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DePaulo, B.M.; Kirkendol, S.E.; Tang, J.; O’Brien, T.P. The motivational impairment effect in the communication of deception: Replications and extensions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 1988, 12, 177–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, R. Scientific (analytic) theory of polygraph testing. APA Mag. 2016, 49, 69–82. [Google Scholar]
- Herbowski, P. Badanie Poligraficzne Jako Metoda Weryfikacji Wersji’sledczych; Wydawnictwo Instytutu Badawczego: Warsaw, Poland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. Detection of guilty knowledge in real-life criminal investigations. J. Appl. Psychol. 1990, 75, 521–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patrick, C.J.; Iacono, W.G. Validity of the control question polygraph test: The problem of sampling bias. J. Appl. Psychol. 1991, 76, 229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mi, J.X.; Gao, Y.; Yuan, S.; Li, W. Accurate and Robust Eye Center Localization by Deep Voting. IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. Video Technol. 2023, 33, 4070–4082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schag, K.; Rauch-Schmidt, M.; Wernz, F.; Zipfel, S.; Batra, A.; Giel, K.E. Transdiagnostic investigation of impulsivity in alcohol use disorder and binge eating disorder with eye-tracking methodology—A pilot study. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yaneva, V.; Ha, L.A.; Eraslan, S.; Yesilada, Y.; Mitkov, R. Detecting high-functioning autism in adults using eye tracking and machine learning. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2020, 28, 1254–1261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Suslow, T.; Husslack, A.; Kersting, A.; Bodenschatz, C.M. Attentional biases to emotional information in clinical depression: A systematic and meta-analytic review of eye tracking findings. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 274, 632–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Building Games with Tobii Eye & Head Tracking. Available online: https://developer.tobii.com/pc-gaming/ (accessed on 27 February 2023).
- Fu, H.; Zhu, H.; Xue, P.; Hu, X.; Guo, X.; Liu, B. Eye-tracking study of public acceptance of 5G base stations in the context of 360 the COVID-19 pandemic. Eng. Constr. Archit. Manag. 2023, 30, 3416–3437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sielicka-Różynska, M.; Jerzyk, E.; Gluza, N. Consumer perception of packaging: An eye-tracking study of gluten-free cookies. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2021, 45, 14–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunaratne, N.M.; Fuentes, S.; Gunaratne, T.M.; Torrico, D.D.; Ashman, H.; Francis, C.; Gonzalez Viejo, C.; Dunshea, F.R. Consumer acceptability, eye fixation, and physiological responses: A study of novel and familiar chocolate packaging designs using eye-tracking devices. Foods 2019, 8, 253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tonkin, C.; Ouzts, A.D.; Duchowski, A.T. Eye tracking within the packaging design workflow: Interaction with physical and virtual shelves. In Proceedings of the 1st Conference on Novel Gaze-Controlled Applications, Karlskrona, Sweden, 26–27 May 2011; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Leitner, M.C.; Hutzler, F.; Schuster, S.; Vignali, L.; Marvan, P.; Reitsamer, H.A.; Hawelka, S. Eye-tracking-based visual field analysis (EFA): A reliable and precise perimetric methodology for the assessment of visual field defects. BMJ Open Ophthalmol. 2021, 6, e000429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Speth, J.; Vance, N.; Czajka, A.; Bowyer, K.W.; Wright, D.; Flynn, P. Deception detection and remote physiological monitoring: A dataset and baseline experimental results. In Proceedings of the 2021 IEEE International Joint Conference on Biometrics (IJCB), Shenzhen, China, 4–7 August 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Verschuere, B.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Theory of the Concealed Information Test. In Memory Detection; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2011; pp. 128–148. [Google Scholar]
- Yarbus, A.L. Eye Movements During Perception of Complex Objects. In Eye Movements and Vision; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1967; pp. 171–211. [Google Scholar]
- Hyönä, J.; Kaakinen, J.K. Eye Movements During Reading. In Eye Movement Research; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 239–274. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 124, 372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Just, M.A.; Carpenter, P.A. The Psychology of Reading and Language Comprehension; Allyn & Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Schuetzler, R.M. Countermeasures and Eye Tracking Deception Detection. 2012. Available online: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/isqafacproc/28/ (accessed on 29 September 2023).
- Honts, C.R.; Hodes, R.L.; Raskin, D.C. Effects of physical countermeasures on the physiological detection of deception. J. Appl. Psychol. 1985, 70, 177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Honts, C.R.; Devitt, M.K.; Winbush, M.; Kircher, J.C. Mental and physical countermeasures reduce the accuracy of the concealed knowledge test. Psychophysiology 1996, 33, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elaad, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Countering countermeasures in the concealed information test using covert respiration measures. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2009, 34, 197–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peth, J.; Suchotzki, K.; Gamer, M. Influence of countermeasures on the validity of the Concealed Information Test. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 1429–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Millen, A.E.; Hope, L.; Hillstrom, A.P. Eye spy a liar: Assessing the utility of eye fixations and confidence judgments for detecting concealed recognition of faces, scenes and objects. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020, 5, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwedes, C.; Wentura, D. The revealing glance: Eye gaze behavior to concealed information. Mem. Cogn. 2011, 40, 642–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peth, J.; Kim, J.S.; Gamer, M. Fixations and eye-blinks allow for detecting concealed crime related memories. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 88, 96–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seymour, T.L.; Baker, C.A.; Gaunt, J.T. Combining blink, pupil, and response time measures in a concealed knowledge test. Front. Psychol. 2013, 3, 614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Proudfoot, J.G.; Jenkins, J.L.; Burgoon, J.K.; Nunamaker, J.F. More Than Meets the Eye: How Oculometric Behaviors Evolve Over the Course of Automated Deception Detection Interactions. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 2016, 33, 332–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwedes, C.; Wentura, D. Through the eyes to memory: Fixation durations as an early indirect index of concealed knowledge. Mem. Cogn. 2016, 44, 1244–1258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Millen, A.E.; Hope, L.; Hillstrom, A.P.; Vrij, A. Tracking the truth: The effect of face familiarity on eye fixations during deception. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 930–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lancry-Dayan, O.C.; Nahari, T.; Ben-Shakhar, G.; Pertzov, Y. Do you know him? Gaze dynamics toward familiar faces on a Concealed Information Test. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2018, 7, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Millen, A.E.; Hancock, P.J.B. Eye see through you! Eye tracking unmasks concealed face recognition despite countermeasures. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2019, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenzweig, G.; Bonneh, Y.S. Concealed information revealed by involuntary eye movements on the fringe of awareness in a mock terror experiment. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 14355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, I.Y.; Karabay, A.; Mathot, S.; Bowman, H.; Akyürek, E.G. Concealed identity information detection with pupillometry in rapid serial visual presentation. Psychophysiology 2022, 60, e14155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klein Selle, N.; Suchotzki, K.; Pertzov, Y.; Gamer, M. Orienting versus inhibition: The theory behind the ocular-based Concealed Information Test. Psychophysiology 2022, 60, e14186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, A.E.; Hacker, D.J.; Webb, A.K.; Osher, D.; Kristjansson, S.D.; Woltz, D.J.; Kircher, J.C. Lyin’ eyes: Ocular-motor measures of reading reveal deception. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2012, 18, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hacker, D.J.; Kuhlman, B.B.; Kircher, J.C.; Cook, A.E.; Woltz, D.J. Detecting Deception Using Ocular Metrics During Reading. In Credibility Assessment: Scientific Research and Applications; Elsevier Academic Press: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 159–216. [Google Scholar]
- Bovard, P.P.; Kircher, J.C.; Woltz, D.J.; Hacker, D.J.; Cook, A.E. Effects of direct and indirect questions on the ocular-motor deception test. Polygr. Forensic Credibil. Assess. J. Sci. Field Pract. 2019, 48, 40–59. [Google Scholar]
- Vrij, A.; Granhag, P.A. Eliciting cues to deception and truth: What matters are the questions asked. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 1, 110–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckley, J.P. Detection of deception researchers needs to collaborate with experienced practitioners. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 1, 126–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pollina, D.A.; Dollins, A.B.; Senter, S.M.; Krapohl, D.J.; Ryan, A.H. Comparison of Polygraph Data Obtained From Individuals Involved in Mock Crimes and Actual Criminal Investigations. J. Appl. Psychol. 2004, 89, 1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradley, M.M.; Miccoli, L.; Escrig, M.A.; Lang, P.J. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 2008, 45, 602–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zuckerman, M.; DePaulo, B.M.; Rosenthal, R. Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1981, 14, 1–59. [Google Scholar]
- Potts, A.C. 1, 2, 3 Crimes You’re Out: Ocular-Motor Methods for Detecting Deception in a Multiple-Issue Screening Protocol. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
Authors | Year | CIT Variant | Diagnostic Features | Analysis Tools | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Schwedes et al. [29] | 2011 | CIT with simultaneous presentation of facial images as stimuli | fixation duration | MANOVA | While concealing knowledge, fixations on the faces lasted longer than fixations on the non-selected, unfamiliar faces in the neutral display. Furthermore, the fixation durations were longer when chosen known faces were presented compared to known but not selected faces. Lying participants were correctly detected in 64.9% of cases |
Peth et al. [30] | 2013 | Sequential CIT with questions regarding central and peripheral objects | number and duration of fixations; duration; number of blinks | ANOVA, Area Under Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve | Participants from guilty group exhibited reduced blink rates and fewer but more extended fixations on the central crime details. This pattern persisted even after the stimulus was removed. The best achieved AUC value across different arousal conditions for both the 0–5 s and 5–10 s intervals was 0.72. These results were obtained using the number of fixations on central details. |
Symour et al. [31] | 2013 | Sequential CIT with face images as stimuli | response time, blink rate, pupil size, and pupil slope | ANOVA, Receiver Operating Characteristic Curves | The use of measurements of pupil size, pupil slope, and pre-response blink rate separately can lead to effective categorization; however, better results were achieved while combining all of the features. Incorporation of eye behavior characteristics into the classification resulted in a slightly improved result compared to an analysis based solely on reaction time. The results of the compound classification procedure used in the paper showed the highest results (100%) while using combined measures approach, namely, RT + pupil size. |
Proudfoot et al. [32] | 2016 | CIT with simultaneous presentation of stimuli in the form of facial images | pupil dilation, eye-gaze dwell time on the center of the screen | latent growth curve modeling, Area Under Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve | Both pupil dilatation and eye-gaze dwell time change in a distinct manner during the course of an interaction, and these patterns can possibly be indicators of deception, irrespective of the presence of relevant stimuli. The classification model achieved a 73.9% true positive rate and a 13% false positive rate. |
Schwedes et al. [33] | 2016 | Simultaneous CIT (traditional and in “oddball” version) | fixation duration | ANOVA, Area Under Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve | The second fixation proves to be an efficient marker for concealed information detection both immediately after the mock crime and (in a reduced manner) after one week has elapsed. ROC analyses on the second fixation detecting concealed knowledge showed an AUC of 0.61. |
Millen et al. [34] | 2017 | Sequential CIT with images of faces varying in familiarity as a stimuli | number of fixations, number of regions visited, number of independent clusters of fixations on an interest area, and proportion of fixations in the inner regions of the face | RM ANOVA | Number of fixations was lower for known faces regardless of familiarity level; number of fixations was a good marker for recognition detection in case of personally familiar faces. |
Lancry-Dayan et al. [35] | 2018 | CIT with short-term memory task (both parallel and single display) | in parallel display: gaze dwell time during the first phase (1–1000 ms) and the second phase (1000–5000 ms), number of visits, and number of fixations; in single display: mean fixation duration, response time, and accuracy in short-term memory task | ANOVA, Receiver Operating Characteristic Curves, Support Vector Machine | During short-term memory task, participants firstly fixated more on the familiar face; then, the strong tendency to avoid it was presented. Avoidance was still evident, even after participants received explicit instructions on how to perform CM. The within-subject SVM classification analysis revealed correct classification rates of 92.2%, 91.3%, and 88.7% for non-concealed, concealed, and countermeasure experiments, respectively. The intersubject analysis showed average accuracies of 93.4%, 90.8%, and 88.7%. |
Millen et al. [36] | 2019 | Sequential CIT with face images as stimuli | number of fixations, average fixation duration, proportion of fixations in the inner part of the face, and number of visited areas of interest on the face | Area Under Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve | Longer fixation durations as well as lower number of fixations in the inner regions of the face were found for guity group regardless of conditions. During familiar face recognition, 57% of participants in the standard guilty condition and 83.5% of participants in the countermeasures condition exhibited a lower proportion of fixations on the inner face regions. |
Millen et al. [28] | 2020 | Sequential CIT with images of faces, scenes, and objects varying in familiarity as stimuli | number of fixations, number of different interest areas of the image viewed, number of return fixations to previously viewed areas of interest, proportion of fixations made to the inner regions of the image, and average fixation duration | Area Under Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve | Deception was characterized by a lower number of fixations for all stimuli classes across all levels of familiarity with higher confidence ratings for higher familiarity levels, definitive distinction of honest answers was not possible based on other fixation measures for objects different than faces. The best AUC scores for both personally familiar (0.83) and newly learned faces (0.67) based on the full trial were achieved using the number of fixations. |
Rosenzweig et al. [37] | 2020 | Sequential CIT with face, name, and residency used as stimuli | microsaccade rate modulation, microsaccade reaction time (msRT), and Oculomotor Modulation Function (OMF) | Paired t-test | There was a significant difference in the mean msRT between the groups. However, this measure alone was not sufficient to assess identify probes within a group. On the other hand, the deviation of the OMF was 100% successful in identifying probes in the ‘guilty’ group. |
Chen et al. [38] | 2022 | CIT extension called rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) | pupil size | Sample-by-sample linear mixed effects analysis on the group level and leave-one-out t-test analysis on the individual level | The pupil size observed during a RSVP task may yield valuable insights into concealed identity information. Although most of the participants qualitatively showed the desired effect on their real name, individual analysis revealed that it was not statistically significant for most of them. |
klein Selle et al. [39] | 2022 | Sequential CIT with cards as stimuli | pupil size, number of fixations, number of blinks, fixation duration | ANOVA | Changes in fixation characteristics and the number of blinks occurred only in the concealed condition, while pupil dilation occurred in both conditions (concealing and revealing knowledge). This suggests that inhibition theory is relevant for the first two and orientation theory is relevant for the latter. |
Authors | Year | Deception Detection Test | Diagnostic Features | Analysis Tools | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cook et al. [40] | 2012 | Comparison Question Test | Response time, response accuracy, pupil diameter, number of fixations, first-pass duration, second-pass duration | RMANOVA, Classificatory Discriminant Analysis (linear and jackknife) | Individuals who were found guilty exhibited greater pupil dilation when responding deceptively to statements. Also, fixation duration, reading, and reviewing times were shorter for those statements than for the ones they answered truthfully. The presented method allowed for the classification of 46 out of 56 guilty participants (82.2%) and 50 out of 56 innocent participants (89.3%). |
Hacker et al. [41] | 2014 | The Relevant Comparison Test | pupil diameter, response time, response accuracy, number of fixations, first-pass duration, and second-pass duration | RMANOVA, discriminant function analysis | The distinctions between participants belonging to guilty and innocent groups can be determined by examining their pupil dilation and reading behaviors. Crime statements were associated with shorter first-pass reading times compared to neutral statements. Participants who were found guilty exhibited a lower number of fixations when reading statements related to the crime they committed. Presented method was evaluated during field studies. It resulted in the correct classification of 83. 7% of innocent participants and 72.5% of guilty participants. |
Bovard et al. [42] | 2019 | The Relevant Comparison Test | number of fixations, first-pass duration, reread duration, pupil diameter, and blink rate | RMANOVA | Participants from guilty group showed a decrease in fixation number and spent less time reading and rereading statements related to the crime they had committed compared to the control group. Another marker indicating information concealment was increased pupil diameter. Under the distributed condition, the decision model attained an accuracy of 84%, correctly identifying 90% of innocent participants and 78% of guilty participants. In the blocked condition, the accuracy rates were 76%, comprising 74% for innocent individuals and 78% for guilty ones. |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Celniak, W.; Słapczyńska, D.; Pająk, A.; Przybyło, J.; Augustyniak, P. Intelligent Eye-Tracker-Based Methods for Detection of Deception: A Survey. Electronics 2023, 12, 4627. https://doi.org/10.3390/electronics12224627
Celniak W, Słapczyńska D, Pająk A, Przybyło J, Augustyniak P. Intelligent Eye-Tracker-Based Methods for Detection of Deception: A Survey. Electronics. 2023; 12(22):4627. https://doi.org/10.3390/electronics12224627
Chicago/Turabian StyleCelniak, Weronika, Dominika Słapczyńska, Anna Pająk, Jaromir Przybyło, and Piotr Augustyniak. 2023. "Intelligent Eye-Tracker-Based Methods for Detection of Deception: A Survey" Electronics 12, no. 22: 4627. https://doi.org/10.3390/electronics12224627
APA StyleCelniak, W., Słapczyńska, D., Pająk, A., Przybyło, J., & Augustyniak, P. (2023). Intelligent Eye-Tracker-Based Methods for Detection of Deception: A Survey. Electronics, 12(22), 4627. https://doi.org/10.3390/electronics12224627