Next Article in Journal
She’s Like Why You Speak English While Dreaming?”: A Corpus-Based Study of Quotative Markers Used by Chinese Speakers of L2 English
Next Article in Special Issue
Are There Aspectless Tensed Clauses in Turkish?
Previous Article in Journal
Linguistic Repertoires: Modeling Variation in Input and Production: A Case Study on American Speakers of Heritage Norwegian
Previous Article in Special Issue
Person Agreement with Anaphors: Evidence from Tatar
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Morphology of Case and Possession in Balkar: Evidence that Oblique Cases Contain Accusative †

Department of Linguistics, University of Konstanz, Universitätsstraße 10, 78464 Konstanz, Germany
Thanks to Tanya Bondarenko, Lena Delikanova, Mitya Privoznov, Sergei Tatevosov, Beata Moskal, Deniz Özyıldız, the Moscow State University fieldwork group, as well as audiences at MIT, the 39th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics: 9–10 April 2021, and the 16th Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics: 30 September to 2 October 2022, hosted by the Department of Linguistics at the University of Rochester.
Languages 2023, 8(1), 50; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8010050
Submission received: 16 August 2022 / Revised: 18 November 2022 / Accepted: 6 January 2023 / Published: 7 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Theoretical Studies on Turkic Languages)

Abstract

:
This paper uses facts about case allomorphy and possessive morphology in Balkar, a Turkic language spoken in southern Russia, to contribute to the examination of the internal structure of case. A number of recent findings in morpho-syntactic research indicate that case markers have a richer internal structure than their surface appearance typically suggests. Specifically, many works in this vein argue based on cross-linguistic facts about phenomena such as suppletion and syncretism that case features are organized into an implicational containment hierarchy. In this hierarchy, accusative case contains the features of the nominative, and the accusative is itself a sub-part of oblique cases. Many arguments for case containment have relied on diagnostics that are less direct than surface-level morpho-syntactic analysis. In this paper, I argue that there is a part of Balkar grammar that shows the containment of accusative case by obliques in a surface-evident way. While such containment is not normally evident in Balkar, I argue that in certain possessed oblique NPs we see an overt expression of the accusative, except when phonological factors interfere. I go on to discuss other related topics about Balkar and the case containment hypothesis more generally.

1. Introduction

This paper uses facts about case allomorphy and possessive morphology in Balkar (Turkic) to contribute to the examination of the internal structure of case. Recent findings in morpho-syntactic research indicate that case markers frequently have a richer internal structure than their surface appearance typically suggests. Specifically, many works in this vein argue based on cross-linguistic facts about phenomena such as suppletion and syncretism that case features are organized into an implicational containment hierarchy. See, for instance, Caha (2009, 2013); Moskal (2015a); Moskal and Smith (2016); Smith et al. (2019); Zompi (2017), and Davis (2021a).1 The main goal of this paper is to argue that Balkar provides further evidence for this finding.

1.1. Background on Case Containment

Though differing in terminology and implementation, many of the works just cited argue for (at least) the case containment relationships schematized in (1) below. In (1a), we see that the node bearing nominative case combines directly with the noun, whereas in (1b) we see that accusative case contains the structure corresponding to nominative case. Finally, (1c) states that oblique cases (locative, ablative, dative, etc.) contain the accusative, which in turn contains the nominative as just mentioned:
(1)
Basic case containment relations
Languages 08 00050 i001
Some works such as Caha (2009, 2013) argue for a more articulated hierarchy in which the different oblique cases have different positions, with some obliques containing others (see Section 5.2 below). In contrast, Zompì (2017), Smith et al. (2019) and Davis (2021a) argue for a more compressed hierarchy like that in (1) above, which considers all oblique cases to have the same position in the hierarchy. This relatively conservative containment hierarchy will be sufficient for the majority of this paper’s analysis. Specifically, the main goal of this paper is to argue that Balkar provides new evidence that oblique cases contain accusative as a sub-part. I argue that adopting this hypothesis facilitates a simple analysis of an otherwise idiosyncratic case pattern in this language.

1.2. Preview of the Balkar Facts

The Balkar facts reported here were gathered through fieldwork elicitation with about ten native Balkar speakers in August 2019, in the village Verkhnyaya Balkaria in the Kabardino-Balkarian Republic, Russia. This language is often referred to as “Karachay-Balkar”, though here I use the term “Balkar” since the speakers consulted in this fieldwork identify themselves as Balkar people. Fieldwork elicitation sessions consisted of asking speakers to translate test sentences into Balkar (both orally and in writing), and to rate the acceptability of pre-prepared Balkar test sentences. The patterns shown here are consistent across speakers.
To preview the relevant Balkar data, see the facts in (2) below. While accusative case in Balkar is usually expressed by a suffix -nI as in (2a), when adjacent to the third person possessive marker -(s)I this case is instead expressed as -n, as (2b) shows. Importantly, in oblique case contexts that contain -(s)I, what I argue to be the accusative variant -n is also contained by the oblique case marker. We see this in a locative context by comparing (2c) with (2d). The locative case is usually expressed simply by a suffix -dA as (2c) shows. However, in a context containing -(s)I this case suffix is preceded by -n, as (2d) shows. I argue that this is an instance of surface-evident case containment.2
(2)
The core Balkar facts
  • Basic Balkar accusative
    menbala-köröme
    1sgchild-accsee
    ‘I see a child’
  • Accusative allomorphy in possessed NPs
    menfatima-nɨbala--n/*nɨkörgemme
    1sgfatima-genchild-3poss-accsaw
    ‘I saw Fatima’s child’
  • Usual Balkar locative
    tepse-deküsünturadɨ
    table-locvasestands
    ‘A vase is on a table’
  • The accusative allomorph appears within the possessed locative
    fatima-nɨtepse-si-*(n)deküsünturadɨ
    fatima-genchair-3poss-locvasestands
    ‘There is a vase on Fatima’s table’
I will show that this case containment is clear for the Balkar locative and ablative, but not the dative, in which we also expect to see containment of accusative morphology. I will argue that this exception is due to the interference of a phonological factor. The entire case paradigm for Balkar, which I discuss step by step in this paper, is shown in the table in (3) below. Notice that in un-possessed contexts, accusative and genitive case are syncretic in Balkar. I will assume that this is a coincidence, but I discuss genitive case in more detail later on.
(3)
Balkar case morphology
nomacclocabldatgen
Typical-⌀-nI-dA-dAn-nA/-gA-nI
Possessed-⌀-n-ndA-ndAn-nA/-gA-nI
While Balkar does not systematically show case containment, we see in the second row of this table that in possessed contexts, a potential instance of case containment is evident. Specifically, this occurs when the possessor is third person, as we’ll see. In this paper, I will address the behavior of each of these cases in a way that is consistent with my arguments that this sub-part of Balkar grammar shows containment of accusatives by obliques.

1.3. Previous Work on Surface-Evident Case Containment

If a case containment hierarchy like that in (1) is universal, then we expect to see this containment expressed overtly in at least some languages, as I argue is so in Balkar. While overt case containment is not typical, it is indeed attested. For instance, Smith et al. (2019) observe that in two languages, Khanty and Kalderaš Romani, we see containment of nominative, accusative, and dative forms in precisely the way we expect, as in (4) below:
(4)
Examples of surface-evident case containment
(Adapted from Smith et al. 2019: p. 1037)
  • Khanty
    nomaccdat
    1sgmama:-ne:mma:-ne:m-na
    3sgluwluw-e:lluw-e:l-na
    1plmuŋmuŋ-e:wmuŋ-e:w-na
  • Kalderaš Romani
    nomaccdat
    ‘brother’phralphral-ésphral-és-kə
    ‘brothers’phral-(à)phral-énphral-én-gə
    ‘girl’rakl-írakl-rakl-já-kə
    ‘girls’rakl-járakl-já-nrakl-já-n-gə
See further Caha (2010), who shows accusative containment in languages including Kazakh, Wakhi, and West Tocharian, as well as Caha and Türk (2021), who discuss accusative containment in Turkish and Azeri. While surface-evident case containment patterns thus do exist, this is likely not a pervasive morphological phenomenon, since many arguments for case containment have relied on diagnostics that are less direct than surface-level morpho-syntactic analysis. For instance, much research in this vein draws inferences from patterns of suppletion and syncretism, as mentioned above and discussed further in Section 2 below. If the analysis of Balkar in this paper is correct, then this language provides yet another piece of evidence for accusative containment in Turkic (in addition to at least Azeri, Kazakh, and Turkish).

1.4. Contents of the Paper

In Section 2, I provide background on a theory about how case allomorphy relates to the case containment hypothesis. In Section 3, I describe the relevant facts about Balkar case and possessive morphology in detail. Section 4 provides the core analysis, focusing on accusative containment by locative and ablative. Section 5 discusses the Balkar dative, which I argue does not show accusative containment due to a phonological problem. Here I also show evidence for potential containment of locative by ablative. Section 6 discusses the Balkar genitive. Section 7 addresses why surface-evident case containment is not a typical state of affairs in human language. Section 8 concludes.

2. Background on Case Containment and Case Allomorphy

This paper builds on research examining a generalization about the relationship between structural containment and the distribution of allomorphy. A number of recent works in morpho-syntax argue for a generalization like that in (5) below. This generalization, which is essential for the analysis of this paper, is supported by facts about adjective suppletion (Bobaljik 2012), the morphology of in/ex-clusivity (Moskal 2018), and case-sensitive pronoun suppletion (Smith et al. 2019):
(5)
Generalization about allomorphy rules in syntactic containment hierarchies3
If an element α undergoes allomorphy in the context of a syntactic feature/category β , then α will also undergo allomorphy in more complex contexts that entail the presence of β .
Importantly, when we combine this generalization with the hypothesis that oblique cases contain accusative case (1c), we make the additional prediction in (6):
(6)
Prediction about allomorphy in oblique cases given case containment
An allomorphy process triggered by accusative case should also be triggered by oblique cases, since the former is a part of the latter.
Smith et al. (2019) argue based on a cross-linguistic study of pronominal suppletion that this prediction (among other related ones) is correct. Some relevant data are provided in (7) below. Here we see patterns where there is a pronominal root (in bold) whose form in both accusative and dative environments is the same, setting aside some minor phonological differences (vowel height in Latin, accent in Lithuanian, vowel deletion in Russian). For analogous findings about cross-linguistic case syncretism, see Caha (2009).
(7)
Case-sensitive suppletion in Indo-European 1st person singular pronouns
(Adapted from Smith et al. 2019: p. 1042)
nomaccdat
Germanichmichmir
Greekegōemeemoi
Latinegomihi
Lithuanianàšmanèmán
Russianjamenjamnje
As Smith et al. (2019) do, in this paper I will formalize my analysis using the Distributed Morphology theory (Embick and Marantz 2008; Halle and Marantz 1993; Harley and Noyer 1999, a.o.). This theory hypothesizes that morpho-phonological form is assigned to the terminal nodes of a syntactic structure after it is built. The rules that achieve this are termed Vocabulary Insertion (VI) rules. We can use this theory to understand patterns like that in (7) above in the following way: if there is a VI rule that changes the form of a pronoun in the accusative case, then in oblique contexts that same VI rule will apply, since oblique structures also contain the accusative. For example, consider the Russian data from (7). If oblique contexts contain accusative case (even if we do not see it expressed by a distinct morpheme), then we will accurately describe the alternation in the Russian 1st person pronoun with the VI rules in (8) below (simplifying the phonology for convenience).
(8)
Some VI rules for the Russian 1st person singular pronoun
  • N [ 1 p . s g ] ↔ ja
  • N [ 1 p . s g ] ↔ m(e)nj /    [acc]
The VI rule in (8a) states that the form ja is simply default, since it is not specified for a particular context. In contrast, the rule in (8b) is context sensitive: this rule determines that the 1st person singular pronoun should be morphologically expressed as m(e)nj when local to accusative case. Importantly, notice that if oblique case structures contain accusative case, the rule in (8b) will be triggered in both accusative and oblique case environments. This is exactly the right prediction, as we can see in (7) above. The same sort of analysis can be applied to numerous languages, which Smith et al. (2019) take as evidence that oblique structures do indeed contain the accusative.
In this paper, I argue that similar considerations explain an otherwise idiosyncratic pattern in the case system of Balkar. We will see that an instance of context-sensitive allomorphy in Balkar reveals an instance of surface-evident case containment, indicating that oblique cases contain the accusative. In the next section, we will see the relevant Balkar facts in detail.

3. The Balkar Facts

Balkar has not been discussed in much (English language) linguistic literature, though it is the topic of some work in syntax and semantics (Lyutikova et al. 2006; Tatevosov 2008; Bondarenko and Davis, n.d.). Its case morphology has not been studied in detail, and the phenomenon in focus in this paper has as far as I know never been analyzed.
The phenomenon in focus here relates to the form of accusative case in Balkar, and its relationship to the morphological expression of certain oblique cases. In this language, accusative and genitive case are typically both expressed as -nI, as shown respectively by (9) and (10) below. Like most Turkic languages, Balkar has vowel harmony for frontness/backness and roundness. In this paper we will see /I/, a harmonizing high vowel, and /A/, a harmonizing low vowel. Depending on phonological context, the first can be realized as [i], [ɨ], [y], or [u], while the second will be realized as [e] or [a]. I set the details of vowel harmony aside, since they do not affect this paper’s analysis.
(9)
Typical accusative -nI
  • menbala-köröme
    1sgchild-accsee
    ‘I see a child’
  • kerimfatima-kördü
    kerimfatima-accsaw
    ‘Kerim saw Fatima’
  • kerimali-kördü
    kerimali-accsaw
    ‘Kerim saw Ali’
(10)
Genitive -nI
  • bala-illew-uqanʁa-daturadɨ
    child-gentoy-3posstable-locstands
    ‘A/the child’s toy is (stands) on the table’
  • biz-niqonšu-buz-nukištig-ičičʁantutdu
    1pl-genneighbor-1pl.poss-gencat-3possmousecaught
    ‘Our neighbor’s cat caught a mouse’4
  • siz-niillew-ügüzqanʁa-daturadɨ
    2pl-gentoy-2pl.posstable-locstands
    ‘Your (pl) toy is (stands) on the table’
When a Balkar noun is possessed, it carries a suffix agreeing with the person and number features of its possessor. Some instances of such morphology can be seen in the possessed NPs in (10) above. For this paper, only the 3rd person possessive suffix -(s)I is relevant, which I gloss as “3poss”. We see a straightforward instance of this morpheme in a basic nominative NP below.5
(11)
Third person possessive suffix
  • fatima-nɨbala--⌀it-nesɨladɨ
    fatima-genchild-3poss-nomdog-datpetted
    ‘Fatima’s child petted the dog’
Importantly, accusative case is expressed as -n rather than -nI when adjacent to 3poss, as we see in (12) below. Since examples like (11) above and others throughout this paper show us that -(s)I is a stand-alone morpheme, the most straightforward analysis of these examples is that -(s)I behaves as usual here, while the expression of accusative case alone is modified.6
(12)
Accusative allomorphy with 3rd person possessive suffix
  • menfatima-nɨbala--n/*nɨkörgemme
    1sgfatima-genchild-3poss-accsaw
    ‘I saw Fatima’s child’
  • menfatima-nɨkištig-ɨ-n/*nɨkörgemme
    1sgfatima-gencat-3poss-accsaw
    ‘I saw Fatima’s cat’
  • menfatima-nɨustaz-ɨ-n/*nɨkörgemme
    1sgfatima-genteacher-3poss-accsaw
    ‘I saw Fatima’s teacher’
  • menfatima-nɨsabij-i-n/*niköröme
    1sgfatima-genchild-3poss-accsee
    ‘I see Fatima’s child’
  • kerimkesineqonšu-su-nurdu
    kerimself’sneighbor-3poss-acchit
    ‘Kerim hit his neighbor’
The accusative allomorph -n is not permitted in non-possessed forms, as (13) shows:
(13)
No accusative allomorphy in un-possessed NPs
  • menbala-nɨ/*nkörgemme
    1sgchild-accsaw
    ‘I saw a child’
  • menqonšu-nu/*nkörgemme
    1sgneighbor-accsaw
    ‘I saw a neighbor’
  • menali-ni/*nkörgemme
    1sgali-accsaw
    ‘I saw Ali’
  • mentereze-ni/*nkörgemme
    1sgwindow-accsaw
    ‘I saw a window’
This accusative allomorph is also impossible with possessive suffixes other than the 3rd person one, as we see in (14) below. Note that while the 3poss morpheme -(s)I ends in a vowel, the possessive suffixes for first and second persons end in consonants (/-(I)m, -(I)bIz, -(I)ŋ, -(I)gIz/), as (14) shows. Since Balkar has a maximum syllable size of CVC, it is conceivable that the accusative allomorph -n does not occur with other possessive markers because it would create an illegal CC coda. In (14), use of an epenthetic vowel to break up the potential cluster was attempted, though this was not accepted. Since Balkar sometimes shows place assimilation in nasal consonants (as we see happening to the accusative -nI in some of the examples below), this was also attempted as a way of rescuing use of the -n accusative, but ultimately rejected. Speakers offered no way of improving examples using accusative -n with non-3rd-person possessors, other than to use the usual accusative -nI instead.7
(14)
No accusative allomorphy in non-3rd-person possessive contexts
  • -nI accusative with first person singular possession
    kerimmenibala-m-/sabij-im-miköredi
    kerim1sg.genchild-1sg.poss-acc/child-1sg.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees my child.’
  • No -n accusative with first person singular possession
    *kerimmenibala-m-(ɨ)n/m/sabij-im-(i)n/mköredi
    kerim1sg.genchild-1sg.poss-acc/child-1sg.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees my child.’
  • -nI accusative with first person plural possession
    kerimbiz-nibala-bɨz-/sabij-ibiz-niköredi
    kerim1pl-genchild-1pl.poss-acc/child-1pl.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees our child.’
  • No -n accusative with first person plural possession
    *kerimbiz-nibala-bɨz-(ɨ)n/sabij-ibiz-(i)nköredi
    kerim1pl-genchild-1pl.poss-acc/child-1pl.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees our child.’
  • -nI accusative with second person singular possession
    kerimsenibala-ŋ-ŋɨ/sabij--ŋiköredi
    kerim2sg.genchild-2sg.poss-acc/child-2sg.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees your (sg) child.’
  • No -n accusative with second person singular possession
    *kerimsenibala-ŋ-(ɨ)n/ŋ/sabij-iŋ-(i)n/ŋköredi
    kerim2sg.genchild-2sg.poss-acc/child-2sg.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees your (sg) child.’
  • -nI accusative with second person plural possession
    kerimsiz-nibala-ʁɨz-nɨ/sabij-igiz-niköredi
    kerim2pl-genchild-2pl.poss-acc/child-2pl.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees your (pl) child.’
  • No -n accusative with second person plural possession
    *kerimsiz-nibala-ʁɨz-(ɨ)n/sabij-igiz-(i)nköredi
    kerim2pl-genchild-2pl.poss-acc/child-2pl.poss-accsees
    ‘Kerim sees your (pl) child.’
In summary, it is conceivable that the -n accusative variant would be able to occur in all possessive contexts if its distribution were not restricted by phonological considerations. However, for the purposes of this paper it is sufficient to assume that the -n accusative is only triggered by the 3poss morpheme, presumably via a rule of morpho-syntactically conditioned contextual allomorphy. I provide an explicit set of VI rules for Balkar case and possessive morphology in Section 4 below. Before that, there are a number of other facts that must be described.
Importantly, there is evidence that the alternation between the usual accusative -nI and its variant -n is morpho-syntactically conditioned, rather than the product of a phonological process. Though genitive case is usually syncretic with accusative in Balkar, genitive case remains -nI even when adjacent to the 3poss suffix (15):
(15)
3possdoes not trigger allomorphy for genitive case
  • [fatima-nɨbala--nɨ/*n]kištig-i
    fatima-genchild-3poss-gencat-3poss
    ‘Fatima’s child’s cat’
  • fatimakerim-niqonšu-su-nu/*nxatasɨndanüj-deqalʁandɨ
    fatimakerim-genneighbor-3poss-genbecausehome-locstayed
    ‘Fatima stayed home because of Kerim’s neighbor’
  • anɨqonšu-su-nukištig-ičičʁantutdu
    3sg.genneighbor-3poss-gencat-3possmousecaught
    ‘His/her neighbor’s cat caught a mouse’
This fact shows that accusative and genitive case are distinct entities in Balkar morpho-syntax, despite their usual syncretism, and also shows that phonology is not responsible for the accusative alternation just described. Thus, I argue that the alternation between the accusative variants -nI and -n is indeed syntactically conditioned. See Section 6 for further discussion of genitive-accusative syncretism in Balkar and beyond.

An Analogous Process in Locative and Ablative Cases

A process interestingly similar to that just shown for accusative case is also evident in the locative and ablative cases. Usually, locative is -dA and ablative is -dAn, as we see respectively in (16) and (17) below:
(16)
Locative case
  • kitabšindig-deturadɨ
    bookchair-locstands
    ‘A book is on a chair’
  • tepse-deküsünturadɨ
    table-locvasestands
    ‘A vase is on a table’
  • fatimakerim-niqonšu-su-nuxatasɨndanüj-deqalʁandɨ
    fatimakerim-genneighbor-3poss-genbecausehome-locstayed
    ‘Fatima stayed home because of Kerim’s neighbor’
(17)
Ablative case
  • fatima-ʁapismoustaz-dankeldi
    fatima-datletterteacher-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from the teacher’
  • fatima-ʁapismobala-dankeldi
    fatima-datletterchild-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from a child’
  • sabij-denqalʁanlabarɨ-sɨ-dačaiištile
    child-ablbesidesall-3poss-datteadrank
    ‘Everyone besides the child drank tea’
However, when 3poss is present, these case morphemes are necessarily preceded by an additional [n], as shown in (18) and (19):
(18)
Additional [n] in locative with 3rd person possessive suffix
  • kitabanɨšindig-i-*(n)deturadɨ
    book3sg.genchair-3poss-locstands
    ‘The book is on his/her chair’
  • fatima-nɨtepse-si-*(n)deküsünturadɨ
    fatima-genchair-3poss-locvasestands
    ‘There is a vase on Fatima’s table’
  • bala-lar-ɨ-*(n)da
    child-pl-3poss-loc
    ‘On his/her children’
(19)
Additional [n] in ablative with 3rd person possessive suffix
  • fatima-ʁapismoqonšu-su-*(n)dankeldi
    fatima-datletterneighbor-3poss-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from her neighbor’
  • fatima-nɨsabij-i-*(n)denqalʁanlabarɨ-sɨ-dačaiištile
    fatima-genchild-3poss-ablbesidesall-3poss-datteadrank
    ‘Everyone besides Fatima’s child drank tea’
  • fatima-nɨbala--*(n)danqalʁanlabarɨ-sɨ-dačaiičgendile
    fatima-genchild-3poss-ablbesidesall-3poss-datteadrank
    ‘Everyone besides Fatima’s child drank tea’
This additional [n] does not arise in un-possessed oblique NPs, as (20) shows. While there is a potential consonant cluster problem (which no epenthesis can ameliorate) in examples (20c-d) which involve consonant-final nouns, this issue does not arise in the vowel-final noun examples in (20a-b). In these examples any potential consonant cluster could be resolved into legal CVC syllables. For example */tepse-nde/ (table-loc) in (20a) could be syllabified as [tep.sen.de], which does not violate Balkar phonotactics. Nevertheless, such forms are impossible, showing that the distribution of this [n] has to do with morpho-syntax, not phonology.
(20)
No additional [n] in un-possessed locative / ablative NPs
  • kitabtepse-(*n)deturadɨ
    booktable-locstands
    ‘The book is on a table’
  • fatima-ʁabala-(*n)dankeldi
    fatima-datchild-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from a child’
  • *kitabšindig-(i)n(i)deturadɨ
    bookchair-locstands
    ‘The book is on a chair’
  • *fatima-ʁapismoustaz-(ɨ)n(ɨ)dankeldi
    fatima-datletterteacher-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from a teacher’
This [n] also does not arise in oblique NPs with possessive suffixes other than the 3rd person one, as we see in (21) below. Since all possessive suffixes other than -(s)I end in consonants, and because the locative and ablative suffixes begin with consonants, the use of the accusative variant -n between these elements could be banned due to generating illegal consonant clusters. In the examples of (21), epenthesis was used to attempt to ameliorate this potential issue, though this did not succeed.8 Therefore there is, as far as I know, no way for the relevant -n to occur with possessive suffixes other than the 3rd person one.
(21)
No additional [n] in locative / ablative NPs with other possessive suffixes
  • kitabmenišindig-im-(*n/*in/*ni)deturadɨ
    book1sg.genchair-1sg.poss-locstands
    ‘A book is on my chair’
  • kitabbiz-nišindig-ibiz-(*n/*nɨ/*ɨn)deturadɨ
    book1pl-genchair-1pl.poss-locstands
    ‘A book is on our chair’
  • kitabsenišindig--(*n/*nɨ/*ɨn)deturadɨ
    book2sg.genchair-2sg.poss-locstands
    ‘A book is on your (sg) chair’
  • kitabsiz-nišindig-igiz-(*n/*nɨ/*ɨn)deturadɨ
    book2pl-genchair-2pl.poss-locstands
    ‘A book is on your (pl) chair’
  • fatima-ʁapismomeniustaz-ɨm-(*n/*ɨn/*nɨ)dankeldi
    fatima-datletter1sg.genteacher–1sg.poss-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from my teacher’
  • fatima-ʁapismobiz-niustaz-ɨbɨz-(*n/*ɨn/*nɨ)dankeldi
    fatima-datletter1pl-genteacher-1pl.poss-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from our teacher’
  • fatima-ʁapismoseniustaz-ɨŋ-(*n/*ŋ/*ɨn/*nɨ)dankeldi
    fatima-datletter2sg.genteacher-2sg.poss-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from your (sg) teacher’
  • fatima-ʁapismosiz-niustaz-ɨʁɨz-(*n/*ɨn/*nɨ)dankeldi
    fatima-datletter2pl-genteacher-2pl.poss-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from your (pl) teacher’
In summary, the 3rd person possessive suffix causes accusative case to be realized as -n rather than the usual -nI. This same suffix triggers the addition of an initial [n] in the oblique cases locative and ablative. In the next section, I argue that the additional [n] we see in these oblique cases is in fact the accusative -n, and that these facts thus provide evidence for case containment. In the following sections, I address the remaining oblique case, dative, as well as a number of other related topics.

4. The Case Containment Analysis of Balkar

As previewed above, in this analysis I will use the Distributed Morphology theory (Halle and Marantz 1993; Harley and Noyer 1999, a.o.), for which morpho-phonological form is assigned to the terminal nodes of a syntactic structure after it is built. The rules that achieve this are termed Vocabulary Insertion (VI) rules. In (22) below, we see an initial set of VI rules which, in the context of the case containment hypothesis, will correctly predict the Balkar facts (aside from one further detail about accusative case that I address shortly).
(22)
VI rules for Balkar nominal morphology
  • nom↔ -⌀
  • gen↔ -nI
  • acc↔ -n / [3poss    ]
  • acc↔ -nI / elsewhere
  • obl[loc]↔ -dA
  • obl[abl]↔ -dAn
  • 3poss↔ -(s)I
Notice that here there are two VI rules for accusative case: one that realizes it as -n in the presence of 3poss (22c), and another that realizes it as -nI otherwise (22d). I assume that the syncretism of these cases in non-possessed NPs in Balkar is accidental homophony, as the above rules show, rather than the reflection of any deep morpho-syntactic relationship between accusative and genitive case. See Section 6 for more discussion of this point, however.
I assume that possessive suffixes express a functional head between N and the case layer. This position could be D, or a dedicated possession phrase above NP but below D (see Huang (2020) and references therein), though my analysis does not depend on a specific proposal about this. For concreteness, I also assume that syntactic terminals are assigned linear order before VI rules apply to them (Arregi and Nevins 2012; Embick 2010; Haugen and Siddiqi 2016). In (23a) below, we see the structure for a nominative NP with 3poss, and in (23b), we see how this structure is linearized and then subjected to VI:
(23)
Nominative NP with3poss
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i002
  • Linearization and VI
    N3possnom
    N-(s)I-⌀
Assuming case containment, by adding an acc feature to the structure in (23a) above, we construct an accusative NP. If 3poss were absent, the acc feature would be realized as -nI here, but in the presence of 3poss, it will instead be realized as -n (24):
(24)
Accusative NP with3poss
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i003
  • Linearization and VI
    N3possnomacc
    N-(s)I-⌀-n
Given case containment, all that differentiates an oblique NP with 3poss from the above is the inclusion of the obl feature on top of acc. In this situation, 3poss causes acc to be realized as -n as we saw above, and the oblique feature will be expressed by usual oblique marking of whatever sort, as diagrammed in (25):
(25)
Oblique NP with3poss
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i004
  • Linearization and VI
    N3possnomaccobl
    N-(s)I-⌀-n-dA/dAn
The diagrams in (23–25) above accurately describe the form of Balkar NPs containing 3poss. Importantly, we see that an explanation for the form of these NPs arises automatically when we adopt the hypothesis that oblique cases contain accusative case.

4.1. On Allomorphy, Adjacency, and the Role of Nominative Case

The VI rule in (22c) above that causes acc to be realized as -n in the presence of 3poss is a rule of contextual allomorphy. Previous works have argued that contextual allomorphy requires either linear adjacency (Embick 2010) or structural adjacency (Bobaljik 2012) between an allomorph and the element that triggers its use. Notice that in (24) and (25), acc is not structurally adjacent to 3poss because nom intervenes. Whether acc is linearly adjacent to 3poss depends on whether the null exponent for nom is really a part of the linear representation or not, though Embick (2010) argues that null nodes are ‘pruned’ from the representation and thus do not interrupt allomorphy.
In contrast, Moskal (2015a, 2015b) and Moskal and Smith (2016) argue that allomorphic dependencies are not constrained by strict (structural or linear) adjacency, but instead depend on the allomorph and trigger being in the same morpho-syntactic cycle. Assuming that the nominal phrase is a cycle (or more specifically a phase in the sense of Chomsky (2000, 2001), a.o.), this approach correctly permits 3poss to trigger allomorphy of acc in (23–25).9
Alternatively, if there is not in fact a syntactic head/feature corresponding to nominative case (see McFadden 2018 and references therein), this puzzle dissolves. This perspective is natural for the analysis of Balkar since its nominative nouns are simply unmarked, as is the case in Turkic languages generally. In (26) and (27) below, we see concretely that the adjacency issue is removed if nominative case is not in fact present in the syntactic representation:
(26)
Accusative NP with3poss(revised)
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i005
  • Linearization and VI
    N3possacc
    N-(s)I-n
(27)
Oblique NP with3poss: Accusative case expressed by its -n allomorph (revised)
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i006
  • Linearization and VI
    N3possaccobl
    N-(s)I-n-dA/dAn

4.2. The Absence of Accusative Morphology in Un-Possessed Oblique NPs

What I have said so far makes an inaccurate prediction about un-possessed oblique NPs. If the accusative sub-part of an oblique NP is realized as -n when 3poss is present, then when an oblique NP lacks 3poss, the accusative feature it contains should be assigned its default expression -nI. This is not what happens in reality. This incorrectly predicted form is illustrated in (28) below:
(28)
Incorrect prediction: -nI will occur in obliques when3possis absent
  • Structure
    Languages 08 00050 i007
  • Linearization and VI
    N([nom])accobl
    N-⌀*-nI-dA/dAn
In fact, as we have seen, the assumed accusative feature receives no overt morphological form in such contexts, as (29) below shows explicitly. Here we see that attempting to include the accusative -nI in the relevant position is ungrammatical. This is a puzzle for what I have proposed so far: if oblique case structures indeed contain accusative case, the typical accusative morpheme ought to be able to occur inside of the oblique marker when the possessive marker is not present to trigger use of the accusative allomorph -n.
(29)
No accusative -nI in un-possessed oblique NPs
  • kitabtepse-(*ni)-deturadɨ
    booktable-(acc)-locstands
    ‘A book is on the table’
  • kitabšindig-(*ni)-deturadɨ
    bookchair-(acc)-locstands
    ‘A book is on the chair’
  • fatima-ʁapismoustaz-(*nɨ)-dankeldi
    fatima-datletterteacher-(acc)-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from the teacher’
  • fatima-ʁapismozaščɨk-(*nɨ)-dankeldi
    fatima-datletterboy-(acc)-ablcame
    ‘A letter came to Fatima from the boy’
I suggest that the realization of acc in Balkar is governed by the three ordered VI rules in (30):
(30)
Ordered VI rules for the Balkar accusative
  • acc↔ -n / [3poss    ]
  • acc↔ -⌀ / [    obl]
  • acc↔ -nI / elsewhere
First the rule in (30a) is attempted, which will realize acc as -n if 3poss is present. If (30a) cannot apply, then (30b) is attempted, which assigns acc a null exponent if obl is present. If both (30a) and (30b) fail to apply, the elsewhere rule in (30c) is then triggered, which realizes acc as -nI.
It is worth asking why these VI rules are ordered in this way.10 In Distributed Morphology, it is often hypothesized that more specific rules take precedence over more general ones. This is why, for instance, a context-sensitive VI rule is selected over a default rule (yielding what we call allomorphy) when the right context is present: the VI rule that defines a specific context of application is more specific than a default rule. Given these considerations, it is natural that the VI rules in (30a) and (30b) take precedence over the default rule in (30c). The issue is how to ensure that the rule in (30a) is attempted before that in (30b). This result would emerge as desired if it is possible to identify some other syntactic feature that the rule in (30a) might be making reference to: if the context for (30a) involves (at least) two features, then it will necessarily take precedence over the rule in (30b) when possible, since (30b) is sensitive to just one feature. For this purpose, we might take advantage of the proposal that accusative case contains nominative case, and say that the rule in (30a) should in fact be stated as in (31):
(31)
A revised VI rule for accusative -n
acc↔ -n / [3poss nom    ]
VI rules that specify multiple features for their context of application are argued for by Moskal and Smith (2016). It is possible that the relevant additional feature is not the nominative one per se, but rather some other feature present in the NP/DP.11 The Balkar data do not point towards a more specific proposal about what this additional feature might be.

5. More Details about Oblique Cases: On Dative and the Relationship between Ablative and Locative

In this section, I discuss further facts about oblique cases in Balkar. First I address dative case, where accusative containment is not evident. I argue that this is due to a phonological factor. I then show that Balkar may provide evidence for a more articulated containment relationship between oblique cases, along the lines of Caha (2009).

5.1. The Lack of Containment in the Balkar Dative

Balkar has one other oblique case—dative. This case has at least two forms: -nA and gA.12 The distribution of the two is in part phonologically determined, but some of the time they are in free variation, as in (32):
(32)
Dative free variation
  • alimsurat-nɨsabij-ge/neberdi
    alimpicture-accchild-datgave
    ‘Alim gave the picture to a child’
  • alimsurat-nɨustaz-lar-ʁa/naberdi
    alimpicture-accteacher-pl-datgave
    ‘Alim gave the picture to the teachers’
Importantly, in the context of 3poss the dative form -nA must be used (33):
(33)
Dative -nA with [3poss]
  • alimsurat-nɨanɨustaz-ɨ-na/*ʁaberdi
    alimpicture-acc3sg.genteacher-3poss-datgave
    ‘Alim gave the picture to her/his teacher’
  • alimsurat-nɨanɨbala-sɨ-na/*ʁaberdi
    alimpicture-acc3sg.genchild-3poss-datgave
    ‘Alim gave the picture to his/her child’
Since dative case is -na when 3poss is present, given the above arguments about the containment of accusative by oblique cases, we expect the assumed accusative feature contained by dative case to be realized as -n here, yielding the possessed dative form -(s)Inna. This contains two adjacent instances of [n]. As (33) above shows, in fact we see the morphology -(s)Ina, which contains only a single [n]. Explicitly testing the form -(s)Inna received a negative judgment (34):
(34)
Dative with3posscannot be -(s)Inna
alimsurat-nɨa-la-nɨbala-lar-ɨ-(*n)-naberdi
alimpicture-acc3sg-pl-genchild-pl-3poss-(acc)-datgave
‘Alim gave the picture to their children’
I hypothesize that the underlying form of such datives is in fact /-(s)Inna/ as predicted, but that a phonological process reduces the /nn/ cluster to a single [n].
There is independent evidence for such a process from Balkar pronominal morphology. As (35) below shows, accusative/genitive plural pronouns in this language are simply built from the nominative (unmarked) form, with the addition of a suffix -nI. However, all the singular accusative/genitive forms involve the addition of only an element -i to the nominative form, though this is less clear for the 3rd person singular pronoun, which undergoes an alternation from ol to a(n). The 1st and 2nd person singular forms are sufficient to make my point, however.
(35)
Balkar nominative and accusative/genitive pronouns
1sg2sg3sg1pl2pl3pl
nommensenolbizsizala
acc/genmenisenianibiznisiznialani
Setting aside the accusative allomorphy described above, we have seen that accusative / genitive morphology in Balkar is generally -nI, never -i. It fits the facts in (35) to posit that the first and second person singular pronouns in accusative / genitive case are underlyingly /men-nI/ and /sen-nI/, but that a phonological process simplifies the /nn/ cluster to [n] in these forms. Balkar speakers indeed pronounce these forms as [meni] and [seni], though some speakers suggested spelling them as “menni” and “senni”. This is unsurprising, since under the analysis proposed above, these spelling suggestions match the expected underlying representation that I have posited. Furthermore and as expected, the string [nn] is generally absent from the Balkar data available to me. Thus, there is independent evidence for my hypothesis that in dative NPs with 3poss, the underlying form is /-(s)I-n-na/ as expected, but that the /nn/ cluster is reduced to [n] for phonological reasons.
There is additional evidence for a phonological constraint penalizing the cluster [nn]. When a name ending in /n/ like “Aslan” is marked with the dative suffix -nA, the result is unacceptable, unless the initial /n/ of the dative marker is exceptionally changed to the velar nasal [ŋ]. This modification in place of articulation eliminates the potential [nn] cluster:
(36)
Usual accusative -na incompatible with a /n/-final name
alimsurat-nɨaslan-ŋa/*naberdi
alimpicture-accaslan-datgave
‘Alim gave the picture to Aslan’
As Smith et al. (2019) discuss, it is possible that not all oblique cases have exactly the same structure (a concept that is also relevant to Section 5.2 below). It is thus possible that the containment of accusative case by obliques is not universal. However, the hypothesis that the Balkar dative does contain the accusative leads us to suspect the influence of a phonological process which there are, luckily, two independent pieces of evidence for in Balkar. It is therefore reasonable to maintain the containment of accusative by dative case in this language.

5.2. Containment of Obliques

In this paper, I have assumed following previous literature that the various oblique cases (locative, ablative, dative) are manifestations of a single [obl] node in the functional spine of the NP/DP (which, as argued, contains the accusative). It is conceivable that different oblique cases are distinguished by different sub-varieties of this feature, which we might call [obl1] [obl2] [obl3] or [oblloc] [oblabl] [obldat] for concreteness, as I did in the VI rules in (22) above. However, another possibility is that the different obliques have different structures. Caha (2009) argues for the more complex case hierarchy in (37), in which some obliques are contained by others:
(37)
A more complex case hierarchy
[[[[[[ nom ] acc ] gen ] dat ] instr ] com ]
Caha (2013) presents a variation of this proposal, which also includes locative and ablative, based on facts about case syncretism in Classical Armenian:
(38)
Another more complex case hierarchy
nom - acc - loc - gen - dat - abl - inst
The hierarchy in (38) places locative case quite low, between accusative and genitive. However, notice that if we interpret this hierarchy as reflecting a containment relationship, (38) states that locative is contained by ablative. Genitive and dative sit between them, though the relation of these cases to case containment proposals is debated: see discussion in Harðarson (2016); Smith et al. (2019); Starke (2017), and Davis (2021a). Balkar morphology is consistent with the general proposal that locative case is contained by ablative. In Balkar, the locative is -dA, and the ablative is -dAn, as shown once again in (39) below:
(39)
Locative is -dA, ablative is -dAn
  • kitabšindig-deturadɨ
    bookchair-locstands
    ‘A book is on a chair’
  • sabij-denqalʁanlabarɨ-sɨ-dačaiištile
    child-ablbesidesall-3poss-datteadrank
    ‘Everyone besides the child drank tea’
These facts permit an analysis wherein the locative is -dA, and the ablative is -n (coincidentally like the accusative allomorph), which overtly contains the locative:
(40)
Ablative case contains locative
  • Languages 08 00050 i008
  • N([nom])acclocabl
    N-⌀-⌀-dA-n
Thus, Balkar potentially has yet another instance of surface-evident case containment.
If we are to take seriously the idea that there is a containment relationship between the various oblique cases, then other questions arise about what we might expect to see in Balkar. Following the hierarchy in (38), for instance, we predict the possibility of dative morphology being contained by the ablative case. This and additional similar predictions we might draw from these more granular hierarchies are not correct for Balkar. Thus, overall, adopting a relatively simplified case hierarchy that does not structurally distinguish the various obliques is most natural for this language (which would entail that the resemblance between ablative and locative here is a coincidence). While works like Caha (2009, 2013) have reasons for positing more complex case hierarchies, the facts about Balkar do not allow us to probe this issue more deeply. Since morphology is subject to considerable cross-linguistic idiosyncrasy, we do not expect every language to realize syntactic structures in the same way. Furthermore as I discuss in Section 7 below, there is likely an independent reason why overtly realizing containment structures is a marked option, which would make the presence of more articulated case hierarchies all the more difficult to detect.

6. On Genitive Syncretism in Balkar and Beyond

Earlier in this paper, I assumed that the usual syncretism of accusative and genitive case as -nI in Balkar is accidental, since these two cases pattern differently with regard to allomorphy, and since only accusative appears to be contained by oblique cases. I have therefore tacitly assumed throughout this paper that genitive does not participate in case containment in Balkar. While the status of genitive in case containment theories is controversial as mentioned above, we saw in (37–38) that Caha does indeed place genitive in the hierarchy, near accusative. Davis (2021a) makes a similar proposal based on the occasional syncretism and general morpho-phonological similarity of accusative and genitive in Buryat (Mongolic).
There is cross-linguistic evidence that accusative-genitive syncretism is well-attested. Such syncretism is rare as a language-wide phenomenon, as Baker (2015) notes, citing Lander (2008) for the statement that only Balkar and Martuthunira have it.13 However, at least some syncretism of genitive with accusative (among other cases) is not rare in of itself.14 As pointed out by a reviewer, Baker notes that partial syncretism of accusative and genitive is common in the Slavic family, and Baerman et al. (2005) report the presence of accusative-genitive syncretism in the languages just mentioned as well as Finnish.15 Another reviewer notes that genitive is often syncretic with accusative in Uzbek (see data in Gribanova 2013), and that the usually null genitive case in Sakha is realized like accusative case in possessed contexts (Baker and Vinokurova 2010).
Since there is a systematic (though variably attested) cross-linguistic relationship between accusative and genitive morphology, it is reasonable to wonder whether the usual syncretism of these cases in Balkar is really an accident, as I have done here. Davis (2021a) argues that in Buryat, at least, these cases are different realizations of the same underlying syntactic element, and thus posits that genitive has the same position as accusative in the case hierarchy (since they are not structurally distinct). In a similar vein, Caha (2010) argues that in Vlakh Romani, Kazakh, Wakhi, and West Tocharian, there is evidence that genitive case contains the accusative, as shown in the more articulated hierarchies in (37) and (38) above, respectively from Caha (2009) and Caha (2013). Caha’s specific proposals about how this hierarchy determines the morphological relationship between accusative and genitive case cannot be straightforwardly summarized here, since Caha adopts a morphological framework that depends on very different axioms (Nanosyntax, Starke 2009, a.o.). In Balkar, if genitive case in fact contained the accusative, then in possessed genitive NPs with 3poss we would expect the following hypothetical structure and corresponding morphological forms, which would include the accusative allomorph -n inside of the genitive -nI:
(41)
Hypothetical containment of accusative by genitive in Balkar
  • Languages 08 00050 i009
  • N3poss(nom)accgen
    N-(s)I-⌀-n-nI
Such forms are not attested in Balkar. However, notice that the resulting hypothetical form would contain an [nn] cluster: N-(s)I-n-nI. As discussed in Section 5.1 above, there is independent evidence that Balkar does not permit such sequences. It is therefore possible that genitive case in Balkar does contain the accusative, but that Balkar phonology prevents us from seeing the [nn] sequence that would make this evident, precisely as I argued for dative case. Since it is difficult to prove that this is so, and because the morpho-syntactic relationship between accusative and genitive is complex, I will not resolve this puzzle decisively here.

7. Why Overt Case Containment Is Not Typical

If case containment (perhaps with some variations in the underlying hierarchy) is cross-linguistically universal, it is worth asking why surface-evident case containment is not a typical state of affairs. While it is indeed attested, it is clearly a relatively marked method of expressing case. There are a few concepts from previous research in morpho-syntax which lead us to expect that surface-evident case containment should indeed be relatively uncommon. In Distributed Morphology, there is no obligation for all syntactic terminals to be morphologically expressed. It is routine for analyses using this theory to posit that many nodes in a structure lack a corresponding VI rule (or are expressed via the null morpheme ⌀). Importantly, a number of works have pursued some version of the hypothesis that morphology in fact prefers to express syntactic structures with the least number of morphemes possible, given the VI rules available in the language in question.
Pesetsky (2013) argues (chiefly in the context of Russian) for the following morphological rule, which would prevent the appearance of multiple case morphemes:
(42)
The One-Suffix Rule (Pesetsky 2013, p. 11, ex. 7)
Delete all but the outermost case suffix.
Assuming case containment, nominative case simply corresponds to a single node [nom], to which the the One-Suffix Rule is not applicable. However, accusative case structurally corresponds to the features [nom acc], but the One-Suffix Rule will not allow both of these case features to be independently expressed: rather, ultimately only [acc] can be expressed in this structure. Similarly, in an oblique structure, though the case nodes [nom acc obl] are by hypothesis all present, only the outermost node [obl] will actually be morphologically expressed if the One-Suffix Rule applies. In contrast, in a language where the One-Suffix Rule does not apply (or is perhaps violable), there would indeed be the potential for surface-evident case containment.
The cross-linguistic tendency against surface-evident case containment could also be explained in terms of a requirement to efficiently express the content of syntactic structures. Embick and Marantz (2008) argue (building on previous observations) that when it is possible for a structure to be expressed either by a multi-word/morpheme form, or by a single word/morpheme, the latter option must be selected. In a similar vein, Siddiqi (2009) argues for the principle of Minimize Exponence, which states that a syntactic structure must be morphologically expressed by as few morphemes as possible. Haugen and Siddiqi (2016) make use of this principle in the context of a theory where multiple adjacent terminal nodes can be expressed simultaneously by one morpheme—an operation termed spanning (Bye and Svenonius 2012; Merchant 2015; Svenonius 2016). If it is possible for one morpheme to express multiple terminals at the same time (provided that its VI rule is defined appropriately), then Minimize Exponence will prefer morpho-syntactic derivations where all of the case nodes present in a given nominal structure are expressed by a single morpheme if possible. For instance, the schematized VI rules in (43) below describe a grammar where nominative, accusative, and oblique cases are each expressed by a single morpheme, despite case containment. The morpheme X expresses the nominative node alone, while the morpheme Y expresses the nominative and accusative nodes together (yielding what is termed accusative morphology), whereas the morpheme Z expresses the nodes for nominative, accusative, and oblique case all together (yielding what is called oblique morphology).
(43)
A schema for VI rules for case in a language without overt case containment
  • X ↔ nom
  • Y ↔ nom acc
  • Z ↔ nom acc obl16
A similar result is argued for by Caha (2009) under different morphological assumptions. The Nanosyntax framework Caha adopts allows one morpheme to express multiple terminals by being inserted into non-terminal positions which dominate multiple heads. Caha argues that most case morphemes simultaneously express all case nodes present via this process. This contrasts with spanning approaches, which do not allow morpheme insertion at non-terminal positions, but yield similar results by allowing one morpheme to “stretch” across multiple terminals. Most importantly, if there really is a Minimize Exponence principle, then regardless which of these mechanisms is assumed, the possibility of expressing multiple case nodes with just one morpheme will lead to a tendency for languages to not individually express each node in case containment structures.
As Haugen and Siddiqi (2016) discuss, a principle like Minimize Exponence is implicit in much research, but somewhat controversial. If this principle were universal, surface-evident case containment would never be possible. However, it does not seem that Minimize Exponence can be universal anyway. Davis (2021a) makes this point using facts from Barguzin Buryat (Mongolic). In this language, it is possible for accusative (or genitive) case and the plural to be expressed together as a single portmanteau morpheme (44a), but it is also possible for them to be expressed via separate morphemes (44b):
(44)
Accusative/plural portmanteau versus multi-morphemic form in Barguzin Buryat
  • Single morpheme portmanteau form (Davis 2021a, ex. 23a)
    bibuuza-nuuʃaəd j əəb
    1sgdumpling-pl2.accate
    ‘I ate dumplings’
  • Multi-morphemic form (Davis 2021b, ex. 22b)
    bibuuza-nuud-iijəəd j əəb
    1sgdumpling-pl1-acceat
    ‘I eat dumplings’
Thus, while the Minimize Exponence principle may describe a general tendency in human language to express syntactic structures with fewer morphemes, this cannot be a universal rule. From this perspective, it is not surprising that surface-evident case containment is uncommon, but attested.

8. Concluding Remarks

A variety of recent works in morpho-syntax have argued for a case containment hierarchy, based on cross-linguistic facts about phenomena such as suppletion and syncretism. I have argued that an aspect of Balkar grammar, involving the allomorphy of accusative case in certain possessive contexts, provides transparent evidence that oblique cases contain the accusative. Specifically, I have proposed that we achieve a simple analysis of an otherwise idiosyncratic pattern in Balkar if we adopt the view that accusative case is a sub-part of oblique cases—a result that converges with a great deal of recent work on the syntactic structure of case. This result is clear in the locative and ablative cases, but not dative. I argued that this difference can be attributed to an independent phonological confound. The majority of this paper assumed that the usual syncretism of accusative and genitive in Balkar is an accident. Since there is a clear cross-linguistic relationship between accusative and genitive morphology, I also discussed how we might incorporate genitive case into the hierarchy in a way that is compatible with this paper’s arguments. Finally, I proposed that independent morphological factors are responsible for the fact that overt case containment is not default in human language. Nevertheless, a growing body of work shows that it is attested, as I have argued here for Balkar.

Funding

The fieldwork research that this article is based on was funded by the Ken Hale Fund at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, and by the Russian Foundation for Basic Research (grant No. 19-012-00627 A).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This research was granted an exemption (Exempt ID #E-1440) by the Institutional Review Board of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (COUHES), due to posing no risk to the participants.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are contained in this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

abl = ablative, acc = accusative, dat = dative, loc = locative, nom = nominative, pl = plural, poss = possessive, sg = singular, 1 = first person, 2 = second person, 3 = third person

Notes

1
Several of the works cited here discuss case containment in terms of the dependent case theory of Marantz (1991), which involves an alternative ontology of case categories. Smith et al. (2019), for instance, argues that this theory’s categories connect usefully to facts about phenomena such as case-driven suppletion. Since this theory is not relevant for this paper, here I maintain more neutral terminology—nominative, accusative, et cetera.
2
The capitalized letters in these morphemes represent under-specified vowels that harmonize depending on the surrounding phonological context. See Section 3 for discussion of Balkar vowel harmony. Balkar uses a Cyrillic-based orthography, but for convenience I transliterate it using the International Phonetic Alphabet with the following substitutions: /y/ = ü, /ø/ = ö, /ʃ/ = š, [ʧ] = č.
3
This generalization is designed to rule out the possibility of what the literature calls “ABA” patterns. The term ABA describes a paradigm in whose first cell no special morphological rule applies (yielding a default form A), in whose second cell an allomorphy/suppletion rule can be triggered (yielding form B), and in whose third cell the triggering of that rule unexpectedly fails (thus the default form A re-emerges). The works just cited argue that ABA patterns are unattested due to generally being superseded by ABB patterns, for reasons relating to the way that morphological rules interact with syntactic containment. Davis (2021a) and Middleton (2020, 2021) both argue, respectively using facts about plural suppletion and pronominal suppletion, that when a single morpheme expresses multiple syntactic nodes in portmanteau fashion exceptional ABA patterns can and do occur. When portmanteau morphology is not at issue, however, we expect the generalization in (5) to hold.
4
The object ‘mouse’ in (10b) lacks accusative case marking. Balkar is a differential object marking language, like many other Turkic languages, in which some objects are not case marked. Since such nouns lack case, I do not analyze them here.
5
The third person possessive agreement marker (-s)I contains the harmonizing /I/, and also has an initial /s/ only when affixing to an element that ends in a vowel. Altogether then, this morpheme can be realized in the following eight different ways: [-i], [-ü], [-ɨ], [-u], [-si], [-sü], [-sɨ], [-su].
6
If we instead suppose that these examples involve an alternation that causes 3poss to be realized as -(s)In, it would be necessary to stipulate that accusative case happens to be null in this context. This analysis would still be consistent with the core arguments of this paper. Since the process that results in -(s)In occurs both in accusative as well as locative and ablative contexts (as we will see shortly), it would be sufficient to claim that the presence of accusative case in the underlying structure triggers -(s)In in both typical accusative environments, and in locative/ablative ones, since oblique cases contain accusative.
7
The examples in (14) each show both a consonant final and vowel final noun, since these involve slightly different forms of the possessive suffix (an initial vowel appearing on the suffix in the former context). There proved to be no relevant difference in the behavior of these variants.
8
While the examples of (21) do not attempt amelioration via nasal assimilation of the relevant [n], no assimilation would occur anyway in examples (b), (d), (f) and (h), which are nevertheless unacceptable.
9
For arguments for the phase-hood of nominal phrases see Bošković (2016); Davis (2021b), and references therein.
10
Though it is possible that VI rule ordering is simply a feature of the system: the original arguments for Distributed Morphology in Halle and Marantz (1993) make use of stipulated VI rule ordering at several points.
11
This would be necessary if we take seriously the hypothesis discussed above that nominative case is not actually present in the morpho-syntactic representation. As discussed above, however, this analysis of Balkar is not in fact dependent on that hypothesis, since as Moskal and Smith (2016) discuss allomorphy is in principle possible even without strict adjacency.
12
-gA is realized as /ge/ when harmonizing with a front vowel, and /ʁa/ when harmonizing with a back vowel.
13
As we see in this paper, it is not true that accusative-genitive syncretism applies throughout Balkar grammar, since the two come apart in possessed contexts. Nevertheless, the two are indeed otherwise syncretic in Balkar.
14
I thank my reviewers for bringing this point to my attention.
15
Baerman et al. (2005) also mention that accusative is syncretic with dative in Eastern Armenian, Ngiyambaa, and Phalura, and that accusative, genitive, and dative are all syncretic in Bonan.
16
Of course, it is necessary to morphologically distinguish the different oblique cases. As discussed in Section 5.2 above, we can define different varieties of the [obl] feature in order to achieve this.

References

  1. Arregi, Karlos, and Andrew Nevins. 2012. Morphotactics: Basque Auxiliaries and the Structure of Spellout. Dordrecht: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baerman, Matthew, Dunstan Brown, and Greville Corbett. 2005. The Syntax-Morphology Interface: A Study of Syncretism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker, Mark. 2015. Case: Its Principles and Parameters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Baker, Mark, and Nadya Vinokurova. 2010. Two modalities of case assignment: Case in sakha. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28: 593–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bobaljik, Jonathan. 2012. Universals in Comparative Morphology: Suppletion, Superlatives, and the Structure of Words. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bondarenko, Tanya, and Colin Davis. n.d. Cross-Clausal Scrambling and Subject Case in Balkar: On Multiple Specifiers and the Locality of Movement. Syntax. Accepted.
  7. Bošković, Željko. 2016. Getting really edgy: On the edge of the edge. Linguistic Inquiry 47: 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bye, Patrik, and Peter Svenonius. 2012. Non-concatenative morphology as an epiphenomenon. In The Morphology and Phonology of Exponence. Edited by Jochen Trommer. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 427–95. [Google Scholar]
  9. Caha, Pavel. 2009. The Nanosyntax of Case. Ph.D. thesis, University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway. [Google Scholar]
  10. Caha, Pavel. 2010. The parameters of case marking and spell out driven movement. Linguistic Variation Yearbook 10: 32–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Caha, Pavel. 2013. Explaining the structure of case paradigms through the mechanisms of nanosyntax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 31: 1015–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Caha, Pavel, and Utuk Türk. 2021. Nanosyntactic Analysis of Turkish Case System. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Turkic and Languages in Contact with Turkic. New York: Linguistic Society of America. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist Inquiries. In Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Edited by Roger Martin, David Michaels, Juan Uriagereka and Samuel Jay Keyser. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 89–155. [Google Scholar]
  14. Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by Phase. In Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Edited by Michael Kenstowicz. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  15. Davis, Colin. 2021a. Case-sensitive plural suppletion in Barguzin Buryat: On case containment, suppletion typology, and competition in morphology. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 6: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Davis, Colin. 2021b. On parasitic gaps in relative clauses and extraction from NP. In Proceedings of the 51st Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society. Amherst: GLSA Publications. [Google Scholar]
  17. Embick, David. 2010. Localism versus Globalism in Morphology and Phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press Linguistic Inquiry Monographs. [Google Scholar]
  18. Embick, David, and Alec Marantz. 2008. Architecture and blocking. Linguistic Inquiry 39: 1–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gribanova, Vera. 2013. Copular clauses, clefts, and putative sluicing in Uzbek. Language 89: 830–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The View From Building 20. Edited by Ken Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
  21. Harðarson, Gíslí Rúnar. 2016. A case for a weak case contiguity hypothesis—A reply to Caha. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 34: 1329–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Harley, Heidi, and Rolf Noyer. 1999. Distributed morphology. Glot International 4: 3–9. [Google Scholar]
  23. Haugen, Jason, and Daniel Siddiqi. 2016. Towards a restricted realization theory: Multi-morphemic monolistemicity, portmanteaux, and post-linearization spanning. In Morphological Metatheory. Edited by Heidi Harley and Daniel Siddiqi. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 343–86. [Google Scholar]
  24. Huang, Nick. 2020. How subjects and possessors can obviate phasehood. Linguistic Inquiry 53: 427–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lander, Yury. 2008. Varieties of genitive. In The Oxford Handbook of Case. Edited by Andrej Malchukov and Andrew Spencer. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 581–92. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lyutikova, Ekaterina, Michail Ivanov, Anna Pazelskaya, Sergei Tatevosov, and Andrey Shluinsky. 2006. Struktura sobitija i semantika glagola v karachayevo-balkarskom jazyke (Event structure and verb semantics in Karachay-Balkar). Moscow: IMLI RAN. [Google Scholar]
  27. Marantz, Alec. 1991. Case and licensing. In Proceedings of the 8th Eastern States Conference on Linguistics ESCOL. Edited by German Westphal, Benjamin Ao and Hee-Rahk Chae. Ithaca: Cornell Linguistics Club Publications, pp. 234–53. [Google Scholar]
  28. McFadden, Thomas. 2018. *ABA in stem-allomorphy and the emptiness of the nominative. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3: 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Merchant, Jason. 2015. How much context is enough? Two cases of span-conditioned stem allomorphy. Linguistic Inquiry 46: 273–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Middleton, Hannah Jane. 2020. *ABA Syncretism Patterns in Pronominal Morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, University College London, London, UK. [Google Scholar]
  31. Middleton, Jane. 2021. Pseudo-ABA patterns in pronominal morphology. Morpohlogy 31: 329–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Moskal, Beata. 2015a. Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
  33. Moskal, Beata. 2015b. Limits on allomorphy: A case-study in nominal suppletion. Linguistic Inquiry 46: 363–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Moskal, Beata. 2018. Excluding exclusively the exclusive: Suppletion patterns in clusivity. Glossa 3: 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Moskal, Beata, and Peter Smith. 2016. Towards a theory without adjacency: Hyper-contextual VI rules. Morphology 26: 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pesetsky, David. 2013. Russian Case Morphology and the Syntactic Categories. Cambridge: MIT Press Linguistic Inquiry Monographs. [Google Scholar]
  37. Siddiqi, Daniel. 2009. Syntax within the Word: Economy, Allomorphy, and Argument Selection in Distributed Morphology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Smith, Peter, Beata Moskal, Ting Xu, Jungmin Kang, and Johathan Bobaljik. 2019. Case and number suppletion in pronouns. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 37: 1029–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Starke, Michal. 2009. Nanosyntax: A short primer to a new approach to language. In Nordlyd 36.1, Special Issue on Nanosyntax. Edited by Peter Svenonius, Gillian Ramchand, Michal Starke and Knut Tarald Taraldsen. Tromsø: CASTL, pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  40. Starke, Michal. 2017. Resolving (DAT = ACC) ≠ GEN. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Svenonius, Peter. 2016. Spans and words. In Morphological Metatheory. Edited by Daniel Siddiqi and Heidi Harley. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 201–22. [Google Scholar]
  42. Tatevosov, Sergei. 2008. Subevental structure and non-culmination. In Empirical Issues in Syntax and Semantics. Edited by O. Bonami and P. C. Hofherr. Paris: Colloque de Syntaxe et Sèmantique à Paris, vol. 7, pp. 393–422. [Google Scholar]
  43. Zompì, Stanislao. 2017. Case Decomposition Meets Dependent-Case Theories. Master’s thesis, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Davis, C. The Morphology of Case and Possession in Balkar: Evidence that Oblique Cases Contain Accusative. Languages 2023, 8, 50. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8010050

AMA Style

Davis C. The Morphology of Case and Possession in Balkar: Evidence that Oblique Cases Contain Accusative. Languages. 2023; 8(1):50. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8010050

Chicago/Turabian Style

Davis, Colin. 2023. "The Morphology of Case and Possession in Balkar: Evidence that Oblique Cases Contain Accusative" Languages 8, no. 1: 50. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8010050

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop