Differential Effects of Input Quantity and Input Quality on Bilingual Development: A Study with Kurdish–English Adolescents
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- ew guł-î ciwan de-çên=êtHe/she flower-IZF beautiful AM-plant=3.SG‘He/She plants beautiful flowers.’
- guł-e ciwan-ekan=im bonkirdflower-IZF beautiful-DEF.PL=1.SG smell.PAST‘I smelt the beautiful flowers.’
1.1. English as a Source of Bilingualism in the Kurdistan Region of Iraq
1.2. Effects of the Amount of Language Exposure on Bilingual Development
1.3. Effects of Richness of the Language Environment on Bilingual Development
2. The Present Study
- What is the association of individual input quantity and quality factors of preschool/school input, home input, home media input, parental language proficiency1, and native-speaker input to Kurdish and English morphosyntax, vocabulary size, and lexical access ability?
- 2.
- What are the relative contributions of input quantity and input quality factors taken as two different sets to predicting variation in Kurdish and English morphosyntax, vocabulary size, and lexical access ability when internal and external factors of AO of language exposure, AT, nonverbal analytical reasoning, language learning aptitude, language output, maternal education, and paternal education are controlled for? Does input quantity or input quality explain more variance in the bilingual adolescents’ abilities?
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. Materials
3.2.1. Bilingual Language Experience Questionnaire
3.2.2. The LLAMA Aptitude Test
3.2.3. Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices
3.2.4. Grammaticality Judgement Task
3.2.5. The X-Lex Test
3.2.6. The Picture Naming Task
3.3. Data Preparation and Analysis
4. Results
4.1. Contribution of Individual Input Quantity and Quality Factors
4.2. Contribution of Input Quantity and Input Quality as Two Different Sets
5. Discussion
5.1. Effects of the Individual Input Quantity and Quality Factors on Bilingual Development
5.2. Input Quantity versus Input Quality
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | In the regression analyses, maternal English proficiency and paternal English proficiency were used as two independent factors in this study. However, due to multicollinearity issue, maternal Kurdish proficiency and paternal Kurdish proficiency were combined into parental Kurdish proficiency. |
2 | A few bilingual adolescents were exposed to another language (i.e., Dutch, French, German, or Finnish) from birth till three years of age due to migration and academic reasons. However, before reaching age four, these children and their families have returned to and resided in the Kurdistan Region permanently. Through personal contact, parents of these adolescents emphasized that their children experienced a complete loss of the aforementioned languages. |
References
- Abdulrahman, Roshna, Hossein Hassani, and Sina Ahmadi. 2019. Developing a fine-grained corpus for a less-resourced language: The case of Kurdish. arXiv arXiv:1909.11467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahmadi, Sina. 2021. A formal description of Sorani Kurdish morphology. arXiv arXiv:2109.03942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, Nina, Susan Graham, Heather Prime, Jennifer Jenkins, and Sheri Madigan. 2021. Linking quality and quantity of parental linguistic input to child language skills: A meta-analysis. Child Development 92: 484–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anthony, Laurence. 2022. AntConc. (Version 4.0.5) [Computer Software]. Tokyo: Waseda University. Available online: https://www.laurenceanthony.net/software (accessed on 21 January 2022).
- Arnaus Gil, Laia, and Amelia Jiménez-Gaspar. 2022. Catalan as a heritage language in Germany. Languages 7: 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berghoff, Robyn, and Emanuel Bylund. 2023. L2 activation during L1 processing is increased by exposure but decreased by proficiency. International Journal of Bilingualism 2023: 13670069231175270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2013. The impact of bilingualism on language and literacy development. In The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism, 2nd ed. Edited by Tej Bhatia and William Ritchie. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 624–48. [Google Scholar]
- Birdsong, David. 2018. Plasticity, variability and age in second language acquisition and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bohman, Thomas, Lisa Bedore, Elizabeth Peña, Anita Mendez-Perez, and Ronald Gillam. 2010. What you hear and what you say: Language performance in Spanish-English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 13: 325–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bridges, Kelly, and Erika Hoff. 2014. Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics 35: 225–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, Marc, Michaël Stevens, Paweł Mandera, and Emmanuel Keuleers. 2016. The impact of word prevalence on lexical decision times: Evidence from the Dutch lexicon project 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 42: 441–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, John, and Stanley Sapon. 1959. Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT). New York: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Chaouch-Orozco, Adel, Jorge González Alonso, and Jason Rothman. 2021. Individual differences in bilingual word recognition: The role of experiential factors and word frequency in cross-language lexical priming. Applied Psycholinguistics 42: 447–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chondrogianni, Vasiliki, and Theodoros Marinis. 2011. Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, morphology and complex syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1: 223–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Correia, Liliana, and Cristina Flores. 2017. The role of input factors in the lexical development of European Portuguese as a heritage language in Portuguese–German bilingual speakers. Languages 2: 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crystal, David. 2003. English as a Global Language, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- DeKeyser, Robert. 2000. The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22: 499–533. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/44486933 (accessed on 21 January 2022). [CrossRef]
- DeKeyser, Robert. 2020. Input is not a panacea. International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 79–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dicataldo, Raffaele, and Maja Roch. 2020. Are the effects of variation in quantity of daily bilingual exposure and socioeconomic status on language and cognitive abilities independent in preschool children? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17: 4570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eide, Kristin, and Arnstein Hjelde. 2023. Linguistic repertoires: Modeling variation in input and production: A case study on American speakers of heritage Norwegian. Languages 8: 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fedeli, Davide, Nicola Del Maschio, Simone Sulpizio, Jason Rothman, and Jubin Abutalebi. 2021. The bilingual structural connectome: Dual-language experiential factors modulate distinct cerebral networks. Brain and Language 220: 104978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gámez, Perla, and Susan Levine. 2013. Oral language skills of Spanish-speaking English language learners: The impact of high-quality native language exposure. Applied Psycholinguistics 34: 673–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gámez, Perla, Francisco Palermo, Jordan Perry, and Maily Galindo. 2023. Spanish-English bilingual toddlers’ vocabulary skills: The role of caregiver language input and warmth. Developmental Science 26: e13308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, Virginia, and Enlli Môn Thomas. 2007. Factors contributing to language transmission in bilingual families: The core study-adult interviews. In Language Transmission in Bilingual Families in Wales. Edited by V. Virginia Gathercole. Cardiff: University of Wales, pp. 59–182. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, Virginia, and Enlli Môn Thomas. 2009. Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12: 213–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gollan, Tanar, Rosa Montoya, Christine Fennema-Notestine, and Shaunna Morris. 2005. Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition 33: 1220–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gorba, Celia. 2023. Is full-time equivalent an appropriate measure to assess L1 and L2 perception of L2 speakers with limited L2 experience? Languages 8: 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gordon, Sidney, and Natalia Meir. 2023. English as a heritage language: The effects of input patterns and contact with Hebrew. International Journal of Bilingualism 2023: 13670069231155775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, François. 2013. Bilingualism: A short introduction. In The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism. Edited by François Grosjean and Ping Li. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 5–25. [Google Scholar]
- Grøver, Vibike, Joshua Lawrence, and Veslemøy Rydland. 2018. Bilingual preschool children’s second-language vocabulary development: The role of first-language vocabulary skills and second-language talk input. International Journal of Bilingualism 22: 234–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gutiérrez-Clellen, Vera, and Jacqueline Kreiter. 2003. Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 267–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Habib, Rania. 2017. Parents and their children’s variable language: Is it acquisition or more? Journal of Child Language 44: 628–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hellman, Andrea. 2011. Vocabulary size and depth of word knowledge in adult-onset second language acquisition. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 21: 162–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hintz, Florian, Suzanne Jongman, Marjolijn Dijkhuis, Vera van’t Hoff, James McQueen, and Antje Meyer. 2020. Shared lexical access processes in speaking and listening? An individual differences study. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46: 1048–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika. 2020. Lessons from the study of input effects on bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Becky, Yung-Hsiang Chang, Mingxia Zhi, and Luping Niu. 2020. The effect of input on bilingual adolescents’ long-term language outcomes in a foreign language instruction context. International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 8–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, Gisela, and Akiko Fuse. 2007. Acquisition of English grammatical morphology by native mandarin-speaking children and adolescents: Age-related differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50: 1280–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jia, Gisela, and Doris Aaronson. 2003. A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 131–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, Gisela, Jennifer Chen, HyeYoung Kim, Phoenix-Shan Chan, and Changmo Jeung. 2014. Bilingual lexical skills of school-age children with Chinese and Korean heritage languages in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development 38: 350–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Jacqueline, and Elissa Newport. 1989. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21: 60–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaltsa, Maria, Alexandra Prentza, and Ianthi Tsimpli. 2020. Input and literacy effects in simultaneous and sequential bilinguals: The performance of Albanian–Greek-speaking children in sentence repetition. International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 159–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kersten, Kristin, Christina Schelletter, Ann-Christin Bruhn, and Katharina Ponto. 2021. Quality of L2 input and cognitive skills predict L2 grammar comprehension in instructed SLA independently. Languages 6: 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- King, Kendall, and Lyn Fogle. 2006. Raising bilingual children: Common parental concerns and current research. CALdigest 2: 2–3. [Google Scholar]
- Kuo, Li-Jen, Yu-Min Ku, Zhuo Chen, and Melike Ünal Gezer. 2020. The relationship between input and literacy and metalinguistic development: A study with Chinese-English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 26–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malmasi, Shervin. 2016. Subdialectal differences in Sorani Kurdish. In Proceedings of the Third Workshop on NLP for Similar Languages, Varieties and Dialects (VarDial3). Osaka: COLING Osaka, pp. 89–96. [Google Scholar]
- Meara, Paul. 2005. LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests. Cardiff: Lognostics. [Google Scholar]
- Meara, Paul, and James Milton. 2003. X-Lex. The Swansea Levels Test. Newbury: Express Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Monaghan, Padraic, Ya-Ning Chang, Stephen Welbourne, and Marc Brysbaert. 2017. Exploring the relations between word frequency, language exposure, and bilingualism in a computational model of reading. Journal of Memory and Language 93: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muñoz, Carmen. 2008. Symmetries and asymmetries of age effects in naturalistic and instructed L2 learning. Applied Linguistics 29: 578–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muñoz, Carmen. 2014. Contrasting effects of starting age and input on the oral performance of foreign language learners. Applied Linguistics 35: 463–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ojima, Shiro, Hiroko Matsuba-Kurita, Naoko Nakamura, Takahiro Hoshino, and Hiroko Hagiwara. 2011. Age and amount of exposure to a foreign language during childhood: Behavioral and ERP data on the semantic comprehension of spoken English by Japanese children. Neuroscience Research 70: 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2011. Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1: 213–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Johanne, and Ruiting Jia. 2017. Bilingual children’s long-term outcomes in English as a second language: Language environment factors shape individual differences in catching up with monolinguals. Developmental Science 20: e12433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Johanne, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Xi Chen, and Alexandra Gottardo. 2020. How language environment, age, and cognitive capacity support the bilingual development of Syrian refugee children recently arrived in Canada. Applied Psycholinguistics 41: 1255–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patterson, Janet. 2002. Relationships of expressive vocabulary to frequency of reading and television experience among bilingual toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics 23: 493–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirce, Jonathan, Jeremy Gray, Sol Simpson, Michael MacAskill, Richard Höchenberger, Hiroyuki Sogo, Erik Kastman, and Jonas Lindeløv. 2019. PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods 51: 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, Elke, Ann-Sophie Noreillie, Kris Heylen, Bram Bulté, and Piet Desmet. 2019. The impact of instruction and out-of-school exposure to foreign language input on learners’ vocabulary knowledge in two languages. Language Learning 69: 747–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfenninger, Simone, and David Singleton. 2018. Starting age overshadowed: The primacy of differential environmental and family support effects on second language attainment in an instructional context. Language Learning 69: 207–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfenninger, Simone, and Johanna Lendl. 2017. Transitional woes: On the impact of L2 input continuity from primary to secondary school. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 7: 443–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pham, Giang, and Timothy Tipton. 2018. Internal and external factors that support children’s minority first language and English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49: 595–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Place, Silvia, and Erika Hoff. 2011. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development 82: 1834–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raven, John, John Raven, and John Court. 1998. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 1: General Overview. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John. 2000. The Raven’s progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology 41: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rogers, Vivienne, Paul Meara, Thomas Barnett-Legh, Clare Curry, and Emma Davie. 2017. Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association 1: 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rojas, Raúl, Aquiles Iglesias, Ferenc Bunta, Brian Goldstein, Claude Goldenberg, and Leslie Reese. 2016. Interlocutor differential effects on the expressive language skills of Spanish-speaking English learners. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 18: 166–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rothman, Jason, and Pedro Guijarro-Fuentes. 2010. Input quality matters: Some comments on input type and age-effects in adult SLA. Applied Linguistics 31: 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83: 1762–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ryan, Ève. 2021. French vocabulary development of early-elementary students in a dual language immersion program: The role of out-of-school input and output. International Journal of Bilingualism 25: 1200–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvey, Catriona, Özlem Demir-Lira, Susan Goldin-Meadow, and Stephen Raudenbush. 2021. Effects of time-varying parent input on children’s language outcomes differ for vocabulary and syntax. Psychological Science 32: 536–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass, Joan, and Mary Vanderwart. 1980. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 174–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorenson Duncan, Tamara, and Johanne Paradis. 2020. How does maternal education influence the linguistic environment supporting bilingual language development in child second language learners of English? International Journal of Bilingualism 24: 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, Gillian. 2006. The age-length-onset problem in research on second language acquisition among immigrants. Language Learning 56: 671–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, He, Bin Yin, Nur Amsah, and Beth O’brien. 2018. Differential effects of internal and external factors in early bilingual vocabulary learning: The case of Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics 39: 383–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, He, Rasmus Steinkrauss, Jorge Tendeiro, and Kees De Bot. 2016. Individual differences in very young children’s English acquisition in China: Internal and external factors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 550–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thordardottir, Elin. 2017. Amount trumps timing in bilingual vocabulary acquisition: Effects of input in simultaneous and sequential school-ag bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 23: 236–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon. 2013. Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 86–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon. 2016. Early child L2 acquisition: Age or input effects? Neither, or both? Journal of Child Language 43: 608–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon, Liv Persson, Tineke Prins, and Kees De Bot. 2015. An investigation of factors affecting early foreign language learning in the Netherlands. Applied Linguistics 36: 527–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon, Susanne Brouwer, Elise De Bree, and Josje Verhagen. 2019. Predicting bilingual preschoolers’ patterns of language development: Degree of non-native input matters. Applied Psycholinguistics 40: 1189–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verhagen, Josje, Folkert Kuiken, and Sible Andringa. 2022. Family language patterns in bilingual families and relationships with children’s language outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics 43: 1109–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vygotsky, Lev. 1978. Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wiener, Seth, and Natasha Tokowicz. 2021. Language proficiency is only part of the story: Lexical access in heritage and non-heritage bilinguals. Second Language Research 37: 681–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Mean | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|
Input quantity factors | |||
K.Pre/SchInp | 32.89 | 22.47 | 19.0–52.0 |
E.Pre/SchInp | 31.71 | 19.30 | 11.0–54.0 |
K.HomInp | 38.84 | 22.37 | 10.3–46.0 |
E.HomInp | 8.54 | 14.94 | 5.0–21.0 |
K.LoE | 16.65 | 1.11 | 12.0–19.0 |
E.LoE | 11.28 | 1.47 | 9.0–18.0 |
Input quality factors | |||
K.MatProf | 3.56 | 0.79 | 0–4 |
E.MatProf | 0.90 | 1.01 | 0–4 |
K.PatProf | 3.58 | 0.98 | 0–4 |
E.PatProf | 1.21 | 1.20 | 0–4 |
K.HomMed | 0.88 | 0.67 | 0–3 |
E.HomMed | 1.58 | 0.63 | 0–3 |
K.NatInp | 1.96 | 0.20 | 1–2 |
E.NatInp | 0.58 | 0.62 | 0–2 |
Other factors | |||
K.AO | 0.56 | 1.16 | 0–3.0 |
E.AO | 4.48 | 2.68 | 3–7.0 |
AT | 17.08 | 0.78 | 16.0–19.0 |
NonInte | 50.81 | 5.03 | 36.0–59.0 |
LangApt (raw score) | 209.21 | 57.76 | 40.0–345.0 |
LangApt (z-score) | 00 | 0.64 | −1.76–1.64 |
K.Output | 3.65 | 0.72 | 0–4 |
E.Output | 0.88 | 0.75 | 0–2.83 |
MatEdu | 2.48 | 1.56 | 0–6 |
PatEdu | 2.84 | 1.42 | 0–6 |
Outcome variables | |||
Kurdish morphosyntax | 62.59 | 3.93 | 46–70 |
English morphosyntax | 57.18 | 8.73 | 32–70 |
Kurdish vocabulary size | 4731.63 | 277.45 | 4000–5000 |
English vocabulary size | 3902.55 | 369.05 | 3500–5000 |
Kurdish lexical access a | 1156 | 60.13 | 417–3870 |
English lexical access | 1294 | 78.48 | 521–4992 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 | 21 | 22 | 23 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. AT | - | ||||||||||||||||||||||
2. NonInte | 0.307 ** | - | |||||||||||||||||||||
3. LangApt | 0.201 * | 0.303 ** | - | ||||||||||||||||||||
4. K. AO | 0.215 * | 0.138 | −0.065 | - | |||||||||||||||||||
5. E. AO | 0.146 | −0.049 | 0.025 | 0.026 | - | ||||||||||||||||||
6. K. LoE | 0.783 ** | 0.216 * | 0.217 * | −0.414 ** | 0.096 | - | |||||||||||||||||
7. E. LoE | 0.511 ** | 0.224 * | 0.089 | 0.111 | −0.727 ** | 0.423 ** | - | ||||||||||||||||
8. K. Pre/SchInp | 0.047 | 0.271 ** | 0.323 ** | −0.189 | −0.058 | 0.168 | −0.043 | - | |||||||||||||||
9. E. Pre/SchInp | −0.020 | 0.179 | 0.078 | −0.115 | −0.251 * | 0.128 | 0.213 * | 0.487 ** | - | ||||||||||||||
10. K. HomInp | 0.014 | −0.099 | −0.018 | −0.135 | −0.107 | 0.092 | 0.073 | 0.109 | 0.025 | - | |||||||||||||
11. E. HomInp | −0.091 | −0.062 | −0.123 | 0.067 | −0.095 | −0.101 | 0.053 | −0.145 | 0.142 | 0.046 | - | ||||||||||||
12. K. HomMed | −0.134 | −0.092 | −0.080 | 0.109 | −0.119 | −0.172 | 0.063 | −0.214 * | −0.166 | −0.049 | 0.172 | - | |||||||||||
13. E. HomMed | −0.202 * | −0.280 ** | −0.193 | −0.044 | −0.107 | −0.196 | 0.045 | −0.252 * | −0.066 | 0.090 | 0.290 ** | 0.213 * | - | ||||||||||
14. K. NatInp | −0.049 | 0.380 | 0.085 | −0.080 | 0.208 * | −0.030 | −0.151 | 0.053 | −0.205 * | 0.098 | −0.055 | −0.178 | −0.195 | - | |||||||||
15. E. NatInp | 0.061 | −0.085 | −0.075 | −0.019 | −0.031 | 0.068 | 0.072 | −0.129 | 0.214 * | −0.002 | 0.274 ** | 0.004 | 0.252 * | −0.126 | - | ||||||||
16. K. MatProf | −0.052 | 0.277 * | 0.029 | −0.243 * | −0.072 | 0.126 | 0.001 | 0.328 ** | 0.121 | 0.227 * | 0.046 | −0.023 | 0.067 | 0.002 | −0.153 | - | |||||||
17. E. MatProf | −0.169 | −0.193 | −0.260 ** | −0.008 | −0.203 * | −0.145 | 0.084 | −0.132 | −0.047 | 0.111 | 0.059 | 0.089 | 0.165 | −0.048 | 0.209 * | 0.068 | - | ||||||
18. K. PatProf | −0.074 | 0.156 | 0.066 | −0.163 | −0.091 | 0.046 | 0.015 | 0.271 ** | 0.106 | 0.075 | −0.022 | −0.064 | 0.041 | −0.005 | −0.038 | 0.534 ** | 0.121 | - | |||||
19. E. PatProf | −0.028 | −0.051 | −0.087 | 0.006 | −0.040 | −0.010 | 0.042 | −0.123 | −0.043 | −0.009 | 0.223 * | −0.082 | 0.216 * | −0.102 | 0.340 ** | −0.056 | 0.458 ** | 0.301 ** | - | ||||
20. K. Output | 0.111 | 0.168 | 0.068 | −0.007 * | −0.036 | 0.127 | 0.140 | −0.093 | −0.171 | 0.073 | −0.299 ** | 0.033 | −0.143 | 0.065 | −0.149 | 0.086 | −0.063 | −0.003 | −0.108 * | - | |||
21. E. Output | −0.039 | −196 | −0.140 | −0.079 | −0.262 ** | −0.069 | 0.172 | −0.056 | 0.132 | 0.256 * | 0.337 ** | 0.149 | 0.310 | −0.118 | 0.214 * | 0.042 | 0.361 ** | −0.019 | 0.255 * | −0.354 ** | - | ||
22. MatEdu | −0.157 | −0.040 | −0.116 | −0.074 | −0.109 | −0.078 | −0.050 | −0.009 | −0.068 | 0.158 | −0.046 | −0.019 | 0.181 | 0.035 | 0.161 | 0.256 * | 0.489 ** | 0.156 | 0.246 * | 0.086 | 0.051 | - | |
23. PatEdu | −0.030 | 0.032 | −0.176 | 0.088 | 0.101 | −0.068 | −0.160 | −0.080 | −0.013 | −0.081 | 0.054 | −0.121 | 0.120 | −0.092 | 0.117 | 0.077 | 0.327 ** | 0.214* | 0.420 ** | −0.222 * | 0.130 | 0.051 | - |
Variable | B | SE | β | T | Part |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | |||||
Constant | 44.712 | 12.836 | 3.483 | ||
HomInp | 0.089 | 0.078 | 0.348 * | 1.150 | 0.412 |
ParProf | 4.466 | 3.388 | 0.343 * | 1.318 | 0.592 |
R2 = 0.413, F(2, 36) = 6.624, p < 0.05. | |||||
English | |||||
Constant | 53.160 | 1.621 | 32.803 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 0.121 | 0.042 | 0.272 ** | 2.857 | 0.272 |
R2 = 0.074, F(1, 102) = 8.162, p < 0.01 |
Variable | B | SE | β | T | Part |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | |||||
Constant | 4138.158 | 437.968 | 9.449 | ||
ParProf | −36.516 | 70.313 | −0.184 * | −0.519 | −0.275 |
NatInp | 261.842 | 223.209 | 0.187 * | 1.173 | 0.267 |
R2 = 0.193, F(2, 37) = 6.567, p < 0.001 | |||||
English | |||||
Constant | 3821.835 | 90.648 | 42.161 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 4.494 | 2.556 | 0.259 *** | 1.758 | 0.365 |
HomInp | −0.391 | 5.091 | −0.212 * | −0.077 | −0.319 |
R2 = 0.252, F(2, 43) = 3.091, p < 0.05 |
Variable | B | SE | β | T | Part |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | |||||
Constant | 1.016 | 0.079 | 12.826 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 0.020 | 0.002 | 0.220 * | 0.126 | 0.341 |
HomMed | 0.161 | 0.076 | 0.318 * | 2.122 | 0.360 |
R2 = 0.213, F(2, 40) = 4.501, p < 0.05 | |||||
English | |||||
Constant | 1.686 | 0.158 | 10.705 | ||
Pre/SchInp | −0.006 | 0.003 | −0.306 * | 2.191 | −0.296 |
HomMed | −0.237 | 0.077 | −0.432 ** | −3.086 | −0.417 |
R2 = 0.393, F(3, 38) = 5.597, p < 0.01 |
Variable | B | SE | β | T | R2 | ∆R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | ||||||
Step 1 | 0.439 | 0.439 ** | ||||
Constant | −6.833 | 49.083 | −0.139 | |||
AT | 2.501 | 2.688 | 0.234 | 0.930 | ||
NonInte | 0.572 | 0.366 | 0.499 ** | 1.565 | ||
LangApt | 0.383 | 3.175 | 0.038 ** | 0.121 | ||
Output | −1.606 | 1.346 | −0.306 *** | −1.193 | ||
Step 2 | 0.470 | 0.031 ** | ||||
Constant | −42.716 | 70.805 | −0.603 | |||
AT | 4.415 | 3.829 | 0.413 | 1.153 | ||
NonInte | 0.626 | 0.382 | 0.546 * | 1.638 | ||
LangApt | 0.318 | 3.256 | 0.031 * | 0.098 | ||
Output | −2.636 | 1.988 | −0.502 *** | −1.326 | ||
HomInp | 0.074 | 0.103 | 0.289 *** | 0.720 | ||
Step 3 | 0.846 | 0.376 ** | ||||
Constant | −7.419 | 2.303 | −3.221 | |||
AT | −5.058 | 2.194 | 0.442 ** | −2.305 | ||
NonInte | 0.612 | 0.219 | 0.534 * | 2.798 | ||
LangApt | 5.312 | 2.181 | 0.525 * | 2.435 | ||
Output | −3.069 | 1.143 | −0.584 * | −2.685 | ||
HomInp | 0.199 | 0.066 | 0.775 * | 3.043 | ||
ParProf | 11.148 | 2.528 | 0.857 ** | 4.409 |
Variable | B | SE | Β | T | R2 | ∆R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | ||||||
Step 1 | 0.233 | 0.233 * | ||||
Constant | 3572.968 | 441.279 | 8.097 | |||
NonInte | 19.347 | 9.228 | 0.320 * | 2.097 | ||
PatEdu | 46.273 | 33.827 | 0.209 * | 1.368 | ||
Step 2 | 0.335 | 0.102 * | ||||
Constant | 3799.224 | 449.981 | 8.443 | |||
NonInte | 21.916 | 9.120 | 0.363 * | 2.403 | ||
Pat Edu | 65.149 | 34.766 | 0.294 * | 1.874 | ||
ParProf | −117.449 | 68.546 | −0.270 * | 1.713 | ||
Step 3 | 0.427 | 0.092 * | ||||
Constant | 2875.783 | 601.002 | 4.785 | |||
NonInte | 22.934 | 8.687 | 0.379 * | 2.640 | ||
PatEdu | 84.814 | 34.268 | 0.383 * | 2.475 | ||
ParProf | −130.791 | 65.484 | −0.301 * | 1.997 | ||
NatInp | 442.136 | 202.008 | 0.316 * | 2.189 | ||
English | ||||||
Step 1 | 0.287 | 0.287 * | ||||
Constant | 3681.486 | 1102.719 | 3.339 | |||
NonInte | 20.396 | 12.607 | 0.263 * | 1.618 | ||
LangApt | −139.454 | 76.874 | −0.267 * | 1.814 | ||
AO | 155.242 | 57.383 | 0.409 | 2.705 | ||
Output | 160.021 | 65.997 | 0.346 * | 2.425 | ||
AT | −107.999 | 75.969 | −0.235 * | −1.422 | ||
Step 2 | 0.438 | 0.151 *** | ||||
Constant | 3463.060 | 967.286 | 3.580 | |||
NonInte | 20.318 | 11.036 | 0.262 * | 1.841 | ||
LangApt | −127.110 | 67.387 | −0.243 ** | −1.886 | ||
AO | 217.201 | 53.118 | 0.572 *** | 4.089 | ||
Output | 197.846 | 58.730 | 0.428 ** | 3.369 | ||
AT | −132.428 | 66.854 | −0.288 * | −1.981 * | ||
Pre/SchInp | 7.866 | 2.191 | 0.453 *** | 3.590 | ||
Step 3 | 0.540 | 0.101 * | ||||
Constant | 3429.994 | 913.104 | 3.756 | |||
NonInte | 27.014 | 10.791 | 348 * | 2.503 | ||
LangApt | −65.977 | 65.672 | −0.317 ** | −2.527 | ||
AO | 232.437 | 50.545 | 0.612 *** | 4.599 | ||
Output | 252.967 | 60.086 | 0.547 *** | 4.210 | ||
AT | −154.907 | 63.806 | −0.337 * | −2.428 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 7.476 | 2.075 | 0.431 | 3.604 | ||
HomInp | −9.572 | 4.026 | −0.301 * | −2.378 |
Variable | B | SE | β | T | R2 | ∆R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kurdish | ||||||
Step 1 | 0.229 | 0.229 ** | ||||
Constant | 2.562 | 0.516 | 4.966 | |||
NonInte | −0.027 | 0.010 | −0.397 * | −2.710 | ||
LangApt | −0.087 | 0.072 | −0.178 * | −1.218 | ||
Step 2 | 0.270 | 0.042 ** | ||||
Constant | 2.726 | 0.520 | 5.239 | |||
NonInte | −0.032 | 0.010 | −0.472 ** | −3.085 | ||
LangApt | −0.118 | 0.074 | −0.241 * | −1.601 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 0.003 | 0.002 | 0.232 * | 1.475 | ||
Step 3 | 0.415 | 0.145 *** | ||||
Constant | 2.561 | 0.475 | 5.390 | |||
NonInte | −0.033 | 0.009 | −0.490 *** | −3.529 | ||
LangApt | −0.143 | 0.067 | −0.292 * | −2.125 | ||
Pre/SchInp | 0.005 | 0.002 | 0.373 * | 2.482 | ||
HomMed | 0.203 | 0.067 | 0.401 ** | 3.025 | ||
English | ||||||
Step 1 | 0.085 | 0.085 *** | ||||
Constant | 2.132 | 0.617 | 3.455 | |||
NonInte | −0.017 | 0.012 | −0.231 * | −1.450 | ||
LangApt | 0.136 | 0.086 | 0.253 * | 1.588 | ||
Step 2 | 0.111 | 0.026 ** | ||||
Constant | 1.995 | 0.630 | 3.168 | |||
NonInte | −0.012 | 0.013 | −0.167 * | −0.979 | ||
LangApt | 0.150 | 0.087 | 0.279 * | 1.734 | ||
Pre/SchInp | −0.003 | 0.003 | −0.179 ** | −1.058 | ||
Step 3 | 0.479 | 0.368 *** | ||||
Constant | 3.031 | 0.602 | 5.033 | |||
NonInte | −0.026 | 0.011 | −0.344 * | −2.336 | ||
LangApt | 0.201 | 0.071 | 0.373 ** | 2.812 | ||
Pre/SchInp | −0.005 | 0.003 | −0.286 * | −2.031 | ||
HomMed | −0.287 | 0.075 | −0.522 *** | −3.802 | ||
PatProf | 0.155 | 0.039 | 0.522 *** | 3.934 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Faraj, I.R.; Hamid, T.S. Differential Effects of Input Quantity and Input Quality on Bilingual Development: A Study with Kurdish–English Adolescents. Languages 2023, 8, 220. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030220
Faraj IR, Hamid TS. Differential Effects of Input Quantity and Input Quality on Bilingual Development: A Study with Kurdish–English Adolescents. Languages. 2023; 8(3):220. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030220
Chicago/Turabian StyleFaraj, Ismael Rafaat, and Twana Saadi Hamid. 2023. "Differential Effects of Input Quantity and Input Quality on Bilingual Development: A Study with Kurdish–English Adolescents" Languages 8, no. 3: 220. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030220
APA StyleFaraj, I. R., & Hamid, T. S. (2023). Differential Effects of Input Quantity and Input Quality on Bilingual Development: A Study with Kurdish–English Adolescents. Languages, 8(3), 220. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030220