3.1. Introduction
When
Wentworth (
1941) wrote about the English suffix -
dom, the paper was a refutation of the commonly asserted notion (including one strong statement from
Baugh 1935, p. 225—apparently removed from the second edition
Baugh 1951, p. 365) that the suffix was “dead”. Wentworth provides plenty of evidence that it was still alive and kicking.
Bauer et al. (
2013, p. 250) show that it is still alive into the twenty-first century. At the same time,
Bauer et al. (
2013, p. 31) say of the suffix -
th producing nouns that “it probably is” dead. So we have a situation where we know that even language professionals can be confused as to the status of suffixes with regard to their availability, but it seems that some do become unavailable, or at least moribund.
There seem to be a limited number of situations that give rise to the loss of productivity of morphological processes. Although the situations are treated individually below, they often operate in tandem, making the death of a process more likely when they do. Even when a given process ceases to be unavailable, words created by the process may last for hundreds of years, so a line must be drawn between established vocabulary (item-familiar words that might be analyzable at some level) and morphological productivity (the possibility of using the process in the creation of new vocabulary items). This distinction, though, is not new and will be familiar to readers.
The relevant situations that will be discussed here are as follows:
Affixes die when their potential bases are exhausted;
Affixes die when it is not clear how to form the base;
Affixes die when their bases become unrecognizable;
Affixes die when the meaning of the affix becomes unpredictable;
Processes die when they are not supported by sufficient examples;
Processed die when they go out of fashion;
Affixes die when their form is lost because of phonological change;
Affixes die when alternative affixes take over their role.
3.2. Exhausted Bases
There is a set of words in English which, at some time in history, must have been analyzable into morphs, but which today are mostly viewed as unanalyzable. The set is set out in (2).
(2) | here | there | where |
| hence | thence | whence |
| hither | thither | whither |
The reason that the set is now viewed as unanalyzable is that some of the words have fallen out of common use, are associated with high or religious style, or with historical usage. Even when the words remain, the pattern is sometimes not clear because, for instance, hence is used for reasoning rather than for location, while whence is used for location. It seems likely, however, that underlying this loss of paradigmatic information is the loss of productivity, and the lack of productivity is because there are simply no other bases to add the prefixes to or to observe the patterns in. If there are no plausible bases, the affixation cannot be productive.
Another example is the suffix -ern on the words northern, southern, eastern and western (I assume that south-western, etc., do not show extra uses of the same suffix, though that may be controversial). There are no more than four basic words in the set that can be used as a base for this suffix. It has nowhere to expand to.
A less clear example is provided by the prefix
step-. The basic meaning of
step- is that it is added to the name of a family relationship to indicate that the relationship does not arise from birth but as a result of one parent marrying for a second or subsequent time. A
step-brother is not a sibling born of the same parents but the son of someone who has married one’s parent as a second or subsequent spouse. Although
Johnson (
1755) thought the prefix was used only in the term
stepmother, there is a set of central family relationships to which
step- can be easily applied:
brother,
sister,
father,
mother (and synonyms). There has been some intervening productivity. The further one goes from these central relationships, the more awkward the label becomes.
Step-cousin and
step-uncle may be possible, but they are not central;
step-grandmother sounds odd, as does
step-father-in-law. Forms like
step-family and
step-relationship can be derived from these.
However,
step- has not become entirely unavailable because it has changed its meaning. Its new meaning is ‘inherited as a result of marriage’ (not yet covered in the
Oxford English Dictionary, henceforth
OED 2023). Thus according to the Urban Dictionary, a
step-car is a car owned by one’s spouse before the marriage (
http://www.urbandictionary.com/define.php?term=stepcar, accessed on 1 July 2023). This allows terms like
stepdog,
stepfriend (
Bauer et al. 2013, p. 244).
It seems that step- used up the possible bases for its original meaning, gained a few more bases by extending its range to some extent, but then was able to continue only by a change of meaning that made more bases available.
Something similar may be happening with -
ment.
Bauer (
1983, p. 49) comments on the apparent non-productivity of -
ment, but
Bauer et al. (
2013, pp. 199–200) find a number of rare but apparently new coinages, including
alertment,
ceasement,
conjoinment,
worriment. The preferred pattern for established -
ment nominalizations is for a disyllabic verbal base stressed on the second syllable (
adjustment,
appointment,
refreshment), or a trisyllable with a base formed with the prefix
en- (
embezzlement,
endorsement,
ennoblement). Other forms are found but are relatively rare (
Bauer et al. 2013, p. 198). It may be that relevant verbs with no competing nominalization marker were getting too hard to find, and that where such potential bases did exist, the nominalization was not frequently needed (
jugglement,
regardment are both attested but do not appear to be established). One reason for the lack of new -
ment nominalizations, therefore, may be the lack of suitable bases.
3.3. Unpredictable Bases
One of the English suffixes that is perhaps most frequently cited as being dead is the noun-forming suffix -
th (as in
warmth,
mowth). Those who object to such a characterization most usually cite
coolth as an example of productive use, but it seems that
coolth has been used, with humorous connotations, since at least 1547 (
OED 2023). Part of the difficulty with this suffix is that speakers are no longer secure about which words form part of the paradigm. If we look at those most likely to be accepted, separating out those with an adjectival base from those with a verbal base, we find the examples given in (3) and (4).
(3) | breadth, dearth, depth, length, strength, truth (~ troth), warmth, width |
(4) | birth, health, mowth (~ math), ruth, stealth |
For those words in (3), there are many examples where the base is formed by changing the vowel to /e/ (not always from the same vowel in the adjective) but there are also instances where no vowel change is made or where a different vowel change is made. In (4), there are two examples where /iː/ becomes /e/, but otherwise there is no pattern, and mowth is problematic, not only because it has an alternative form but also because few urban people use either, cutting hay being a rural occupation, but urban speakers forming a majority of language users. Overall, this leads to a situation where the speaker cannot form a new word because it is not clear what vowel should appear in the derivative.
3.4. Unrecognizable Bases
Not only do the bases for -th affixation become unpredictable in form, so that it is no longer obvious how to add the suffix to new forms, but also their form becomes, in some cases, unrecognizable, so that speakers cannot see them as part of a relevant paradigm. For example, few speakers link bear with birth, heal with health, dear with dearth, foul with filth or math (now, almost exclusively in aftermath) with mow. While there is a semantic factor involved here, some of this is simply a matter of form: /aʊ/ and /ɪ/ are not frequently alternating vowels, for example, and the orthography in bear and birth hide a link.
As a wider example, consider the now moribund (if not dead) suffix -
le, creating frequentative verbs. The meaning can still be seen in a few verbs like
crackle and
sparkle, where the base can be analyzed, but
babble,
giggle,
grumble,
mumble,
prattle,
rattle,
sniffle,
whistle and a host of others that
Marchand (
1969, p. 323) identifies as being part of the same paradigm no longer have (and in some cases never had) a recognizable base from which a meaning can be deduced.
3.5. Unpredictable Affix Meaning
If the meaning of the base has to be transparent for affixation to work, the same is true of the meaning of the affix. The observation is well-established:
Aronoff (
1976, p. 39), citing parallel comments from
Zimmer (
1964, p. 32), notes that “the surer one is of what a word means, the more likely one is to use it”. To illustrate, we can return to the -
le suffix. Where we have the relevant suffix (as listed by Marchand) and an apparently transparent base, we are not necessarily able to assign a meaning to the affix. Consider, for example,
crackle,
footle,
rattle,
scuffle,
suckle,
tootle, and the same remains true if we add bases that have undergone phonological modification, such as
dazzle (<
daze),
nuzzle (<
nose),
waddle (<
wade) and
snuffle (ultimately related to
sniff).
Suckle and
sniffle might appropriately be glossed as ‘to suck/sniff repeatedly’, but such a gloss does not work with
dazzle or
tootle, for instance.
3.6. Insufficient Support
In some cases, it seems that processes die when they are not supported by a sufficient range of examples. Remnants may be left behind, but productivity vanishes.
We can begin with ablaut. Ablaut is probably still available in English but in restricted constructions. For example, the pattern of reduplication with fixed vocalism illustrated by
fiddle-faddle,
jimjams,
mishmash,
shilly-shally is of limited productivity in that there are not many such formations in widespread use, with the result that evidence of its productivity is hard to come by. However, a recent advertising campaign in Australia and New Zealand to prevent skin cancer that advised the public to
slip,
slap,
slop (
sc. slip on loose clothing, slap on a hat and slop on sunscreen) suggests that it can still be used occasionally. Other uses of ablaut, and ablaut with other vowels, do not appear to be productive. This includes the ablaut found accompanying -
th suffixation, mentioned above, but also includes instances where ablaut alone links words. Some examples are given in (5).
(5) | a. | (causative) | fall/fell, lay/lie, rise/raise, sit/set |
| b. | (plural) | foot/feet, goose/geese, tooth/teeth |
| c. | (verb and noun) | abide/abode, bleed/blood, breed/brood, shoot/shot, sing/song |
In none of these cases is the number of cases sufficient to allow speakers to see an overall pattern: plural-marking as in (5b) is usually treated as exceptional, and the other instances are ignored (implicitly treated as unrelated).
Another pattern that has virtually died out, but not quite, is the pattern with preposition + verb that was in use in Old English. Some examples of the pattern are given in (6), but a few examples are still in general use today such as those with
over (
overlook,
oversee) and those with the preposition
with (as in
withdraw,
withhold), though even then, the semantics of the combination is not transparent (for these and other examples see
OED 2023).
(6) | athold (‘keep back, detain’), forhold (‘keep too long’), intake (‘capture’), ofhold (‘retain’), ondraw (‘draw on’ e.g., of night), outsee (‘see beyond a limit’), todraw (‘belittle’), overseek (‘search through’) |
The loss of the availability of this pattern may be related to the rise of the phrasal verb, which often seems to be more or less equivalent, the entire change possibly driven by the loss of verb-final syntax in Middle English. The construction type was still marginally productive in the nineteenth century but seems to have died out since then. Derivatives such as onlooker are probably by-products of the original construction.
Another example of a disappearing suffix is provided by
Marchand (
1969, p. 350), who cites the suffix -
ton meaning ‘fool’. This is found today only in the word
simpleton (and possibly in some surnames where it is not analyzable). Marchand has very few examples, even in older usage, and most of them are dialectal.
Just what constitutes a sufficient number of forms to allow for the extension of the pattern by means of productivity is an open question. It seems likely to me that
Yang’s (
2016) Tolerance Principle, that too much irregularity prevents a rule from being discerned, will be involved in some form, and Yang sets out parameters for what ‘too much’ might mean in such a context.