Next Article in Journal
Physicians’ Words, Patients’ Response: The Role of Healthcare Counselling in Enhancing Beneficial Lifestyle Modifications for Patients with Cardiometabolic Disorders: The IACT Cross-Sectional Study
Previous Article in Journal
Correlation between Health and eHealth Literacy and a Healthy Lifestyle: A Cross-Sectional Study of Spanish Primary Healthcare Patients
Previous Article in Special Issue
Patients’ Clinical and Psychological Status in Different COVID-19 Waves in Italy: A Quanti-Qualitative Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Machine Learning Approach Unveils the Relationships between Sickness Behavior and Interoception after Vaccination: Suggestions for Psychometric Indices of Higher Vulnerability

1
Department of Surgical, Medical and Molecular Pathology and Critical Care Medicine, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
2
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
3
Department of Translational Research and of New Surgical and Medical Technologies, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2023, 11(22), 2981; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare11222981
Submission received: 25 September 2023 / Revised: 10 November 2023 / Accepted: 15 November 2023 / Published: 18 November 2023

Abstract

:
Objective: Prior research has suggested a possible connection between vaccination and manifestations of Sickness Behavior; however, a need remains to first delve deeper into this association and second examine how Interoceptive Awareness and emotional factors may modulate individuals’ perceptions of their health status post vaccination. Method: An online retrospective cross-sectional survey of 647 individuals who received a COVID-19 vaccination was conducted. Together with vaccination side effects, socio-demographic characteristics, health status, level of concern about vaccination, and Interoceptive Awareness were collected at the baseline level. Mood, sleep, and Sickness Behavior were assessed at baseline and after vaccination. Data were analyzed using inferential statistics and machine learning techniques. Results: After vaccination, there was a significant increase in Sickness Behavior levels (mean (±SD) SicknessQ T0 = 1.57 (±2.72), mean (±SD) SicknessQ T1 = 5.54 (±5.51); p-value = 0.001; ES = 0.77). A Machine Learning analysis revealed specific patterns of individual dispositions (sex and age), baseline emotional characteristics (levels of depression, anxiety, stress, and concern about adverse reactions), as well as some components of Interoceptive Awareness (Noticing, Body Listening, and Attention Regulation), as predictors of high levels of Sickness Behavior, both in terms of overall scores (JRIP: 72.65% accuracy, AUC = 0.692, d = 0.709; F1 = 0.726) and individual items (JRIP: 75.77% accuracy, AUC = 0.694; d = 0.717; F1 = 0.754). Conclusions: Our results provide new insight into post-immune reactions by highlighting the contribution of Interoceptive Awareness in modulating the severity of Sickness Behavior. This sheds light on the role of awareness of bodily sensations in modulating perceptions of health status, helping to identify the characteristics that make individuals more prone to feeling sick.

1. Introduction

Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has affected over seven hundred million people worldwide, causing almost seven million deaths (https://covid19.who.int, accessed on 25 September 2023, at 15.16 p.m.) since it was declared a pandemic by the World Health Organization (WHO) on 11 March 2020. The development of vaccines has been conducted at an unprecedented pace to alleviate the resulting burden and complications [1].
Although COVID-19 vaccines have significantly mitigated attack rates, hospitalizations, and deaths [2], immunization-related reactogenicity [3] is a very common response: soon after vaccination, people can show physical manifestations of the associated inflammatory response that may both cause local (pain, redness, and swelling of the injection site) and systemic effects (fever, fatigue, headache, and flu-like symptoms).
These symptoms are known to be associated with the Sickness Behavior (SB) condition, a reflex of the immune activation specifically linked to the activity of pro-inflammatory cytokines (Interleukin-1 β, Interleukin-6, and Tumor Necrosis Factor-α) triggered by an infectious agent or an injury [4]. Consequently, the cytokines, via the vagus nerve and along with the activation of gastrointestinal and cardiovascular reflexes, transmit the altered state of the body to the brain, giving rise to the profound behavioral changes that characterize SB [5,6]: fever, fatigue, lethargy, loss of appetite, social withdrawal, malaise, loss of libido, hyperalgesia, sleep disturbances, and cognitive weakness.
Furthermore, SB includes an emotional component as sick individuals usually experience increased levels of anxiety and a dampening of their mood to the point of mimicking depressive behavior [7], suggesting that it may also lead to pathogenic behaviors.
However, the way individuals experience this condition varies. Responses to sickness conditions can differ greatly: while some of us can feel poorly with mild infections, others can feel well even when more serious conditions and significant physical symptoms are present. Thus, the extent of SB appears to be influenced by several factors, including individual (e.g., age, sex, and gender identity) and cultural aspects [8].
Herein, we hypothesize that in addition to the previously mentioned factors, interoceptive awareness (IA), which is how each of us feels and perceives internal bodily sensations and processes, as well as the attention we pay to changes in our bodies [9], may have an impact on the severity of SB.
Indeed, through the interplay between a specific network of cortical brain regions (including the insula and the anterior cingulate cortex), and peripheral signals originating from the vagus nerve or related to molecular changes (e.g., hormones, cytokines), IA plays a crucial role in maintaining homeostasis and in influencing emotional experience [10,11].
Therefore, it simultaneously enables the monitoring of bodily states and the appropriate response to them, as well as tuning bodily sensations to specific emotions, helping people to recognize and understand their own emotional states [7].
Taking advantage of the COVID-19 vaccination campaign, we collected data through an online survey to retrospectively investigate whether vaccine-induced SB could be modulated by Interoceptive Awareness alongside emotional disposition perceived before vaccination (at baseline).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Online Survey

A cross-sectional online survey, aimed at exploring the potential association between individual characteristics and vaccination side effects, was carried out retrospectively between March and June 2021. The survey was conducted anonymously, with a guarantee of strict confidentiality regarding all participant information. Notably, no data pertaining to the IP addresses used for survey participation were collected.
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Pisa. In order to take part to the study, all participants provided written informed consent (presented in the first part of the survey and mandatory to proceed with the questionnaire), declared themselves to be native Italian speakers, and were over the age of 18 years. In order to send the questionnaire responses, participants had to completely fill it out.
Each participant received the first dose of the same type of vaccine, and the online survey was made available in a flexible period ranging from one month to three months after the injection.
The survey consisted of three main sections. In the first one (Table S1, Supplementary Materials), socio-demographic characteristics were self-reported by participants who answered about their sex, age, civil status, education, level of physical activity, and past or current pathologies (including past COVID-19). Next, they were asked whether the vaccine had produced side effects and if so, to specify which ones through multiple choice in a list of possibilities including both local (e.g., pain, warmth, itching or bruising in the site of injection) and systemic (e.g., feeling tired or generally feeling unwell, feeling sick, decreased appetite, joint pain or muscle pain, fatigue) adverse reactions. Also, the level of concern and precautions before vaccination were investigated.
The second section assessed the individual IA using the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA, Cronbach’s α ranges from 0.66 to 0.82 across the subscales) composed of eight components: Noticing, Not—Distracting, Not—Worrying, Attention Regulation, Emotional Awareness, Self—Regulation, Body Listening, and Trusting [12].
The third section pertained to sickness symptoms, quality of sleep, and the mood of participants, referring retrospectively to both before and the week following the first dose of the COVID-19 vaccination. The Sickness Questionnaire (SicknessQ, Cronbach’s α of 0.86) was used to detect the presence and the severity of SB [13]. The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale—Short Form (DASS-21, Cronbach’s α of 0.90) was used to assess negative emotional states of depression (DASS-21 depression, Cronbach’s α of 0.82), anxiety (DASS-21 anxiety, Cronbach’s α of 0.74), and stress (DASS-21 stress, Cronbach’s α of 0.85) [14]. Quality of sleep was assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI, Cronbach’s α of 0.835), composed of seven components: subjective quality of sleep, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficacy, sleep disorders, use of hypnotic drugs, and disorders during the day [15].

2.2. Data Pre-Processing

The online survey was implemented using Google Forms. Raw data were then preprocessed in order to be suitable for statistical analysis. In particular, the subjects’ health status responses, as they were collected as free text, were managed, and reported diseases were classified into the five classes of diseases according to the International Classification of Diseases 10th revision (ICD-10) [16], with the addition of the category “other diseases” for health conditions not included in the manual. The question about COVID-19 positivity was collected as multiple choice. As a result, the data were converted into categorical variables and analyzed at a dichotomous level: the presence or absence of diseases belonging to each category.
The adverse reactions to the vaccine were originally collected by asking for the presence/absence of each possible one within a prepared list and then clustering the responses into three categorical variables: fever > 38 °C, local symptoms, and systemic symptoms. Consequently, they were analyzed dichotomously for the presence or absence of symptoms within each cluster. Responses related to the concern about an adverse reaction to the vaccine were collected using a 10-point Likert scale and analyzed as continuous variables.

2.3. Data Analysis

A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, and the mean and standard deviation were reported for the following variables, referring to the baseline and post COVID-19 vaccination (T0 and T1, respectively): SicknessQ, DASS-21 Total Score, DASS-21 components (DASS-21 Depression, DASS-21 Anxiety, DASS-21 Stress), and PSQI Total Score.
Additionally, for all variables that achieved statistical significance (except SicknessQ), a repeated-measures ANCOVA was carried out, incorporating the changes from the post COVID-19 vaccination and baseline (delta) values of SicknessQ as covariates. This allowed us to investigate whether the pre–post effect could be explained by the variation in Sickness Behavior.
For each variable, the effect size (d) was derived [17] and its magnitude was interpreted according to Cohen (1998) [18]: 0.0–0.1 (no effect), 0.2–0.4 (small effect), 0.5–0.7 (intermediate effect), and 0.8–≥1 (large effect). The statistical significance level was set at 5%. In order to investigate the independent variables that could possibly correlate with a high level of SB after COVID-19 vaccination, we divided the participants (n = 647) into two subgroups: participants with a lower level of Sickness Behavior (L-SB), as measured by the SicknessQ at T1, and participants with a higher level of SB (H-SB). For the L-BS group, we retained participants within the 33rd percentile of SicknessQ scores at T1 that ranged from 0 to 2 (n = 252) in our sample. Analogously, for the H-SB group, we retained participants above the 66th percentile of the SicknessQ scores at T1 that ranged from 7 to 28 (n = 227) in our sample. The data analysis was carried out using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and Weka 3.9.
With the aim of assessing the features contributing to the classification of participants with lower and higher levels of post-vaccination SB, we applied Machine Learning (ML) techniques and relevant algorithms. In particular, the classification was based on a propositional rule learner, Repeated Incremental Pruning to Produce Error Reduction [19], implemented in Weka software (JRIP). JRIP uses incremental error pruning to produce error reduction, followed by a complex optimization step; it was selected in this study since it has the advantage of performing classification by selecting a subset of features and providing intelligible decision rules based on this selection. Thus, the classification performance of JRIP was compared to the most common classifiers in order to ensure an appropriate approach. For the comparison, the following classifiers were used: Naïve Bayes [20], Logistic Regression [21], Simple Logistic [22], Support Vector Machine [23], and Random Forest [24]. The classifiers used were run using the default parameters of the Weka software.
In order to have a lower rate of bias and avoid overfitting, we used the k-fold cross validation technique, which randomly divides the dataset into k folds of equal size, where the value for k was set to 10 (k = 10). The quality of classification was estimated by means of the AUC-ROC (AUC) and F1-score (F1) metrics: F1 can have a lowest value of 0 and a maximum value of 1. The highest value indicates perfect precision (correct positive predictions relative to all positive predictions) and recall (correct positive predictions relative to all actual positives), and the lowest possible value is 0. With regards to the AUC, when the value is 1, the classifier is able to accurately distinguish between every positive and every negative class point. In contrast, if the AUC had been 0, the classifier would have predicted all positives as negatives and all negatives as positives.
The classification analysis was carried out in consecutive steps that gradually increased the level of specificity of the potential predictors. All ML models were estimated using the following data: (1) five sociodemographic features; and (2) thirteen items of health status information, including physical side effects after vaccination. Additionally, based on the level of analysis, we progressed from studying potential predictors among the questionnaire’s total scores to evaluating specific questionnaire subscores and items contributing to scores that were identified as predictors at the previous analysis level.
For the classification, we only used the DASS-21, PSQI, and MAIA questionnaire scores/subscores, as well as the corresponding items related to the baseline before vaccination, discarding any information from the week following vaccination.

3. Results

A total of six hundred and forty-seven Italian-speaking participants (384 men and 263 women; mean age, 49.87 ± 10.56 (mean ± SD), range 20–70 years) completed the online survey. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are reported in Table 1, while Table 2 reports the adverse effects to vaccination (Table S2, Supplementary Materials, report trait characteristics).
A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, and the results are shown in Table 3. The difference of the mean between the paired variables at baseline and after COVID-19 vaccination was statistically significant for the following measures: SicknessQ, DASS-21, and all subcomponents. A large effect size was shown for the SicknessQ scores (T0 vs. T1; d = 0.77) (SicknessQ boxplots are reported in the Supplementary Materials Figure S1), and the smallest effect was found for the DASS-21 stress score (T0 vs. T1; d = 0.22). Including SicknessQ as a covariate in the repeated-measures ANCOVA for the significant variables allowed us to remove the contribution of SicknessQ to the pre–post changes. Of note, the observed changes in anxiety appear to be completely explained by SicknessQ since the main effect disappears when controlling for this variable (Table 4).

Machine-Learning-Based Classification Accuracies of High and Low Levels of Sickness Behavior Referred after COVID-19 Vaccination

In order to investigate the independent variables that possibly predict a high level of SB after COVID-19 vaccination, as measured by the SicknessQ in the week after the vaccination, the participants’ data were divided into two subgroups: those with a lower level of SB (L-SB) and those with a higher level of SB (H-SB). Sociodemographic information, health status and DASS-21, and the PSQI and MAIA questionnaire scores were used as independent variables for the predictions made using different ML methods.
In the first preliminary round, all baseline variables were used, including the SicknessQ scores. Regardless of the classifier, the classification accuracy in distinguishing the L-SB and H-SB classes was around 77% (Supplementary Materials File S1, Tables S3 and S4). In order to exclude possible trait effects on the individual levels of SicknessQ, the classification was then repeated after excluding SicknessQ at baseline from the ML analysis. During the classification process, the variables exhibiting optimal efficiency in classifying the participants within the L-SB and H-SB groups were identified through JRIP’s rules and their cut-offs. JRIP yielded an accuracy of 72.65% (decision matrix (AUC = 0.692, d = 0.709; F1 = 0.726), Supplementary Materials Table S5) by identifying the following eight rules:
(1)
If DASS-21 Stress T0 ≥ 4 and Fever > 38 °C = Positive, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(2)
If Systemic Symptoms = Positive and MAIA Noticing ≥ 1.75 and Sex = Female and Age ≤ 50, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(3)
If DASS-21 Depression T0 ≥ 1 and Systemic Symptoms = Positive, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(4)
If Systemic Symptoms = Positive and MAIA Attention Regulation ≤ 2 and MAIA Noticing ≥ 2.5, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(5)
If Systemic Symptoms = Positive and DASS-21 Anxiety T0 ≥ 1, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(6)
If Systemic Symptoms = Positive and PSQI Sleep Latency T0 ≥ 1 and MAIA Body Listening ≤ 2.333333 and MAIA Body Listening ≥ 2, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(7)
If Systemic Symptoms = Positive and Concerns about adverse reactions ≥ 5 and MAIA Attention Regulation ≤ 2.714286 and Local symptoms = Absent, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(8)
If the previous seven rules are not applicable, then the individual is classified as an L-SB subject.
In addition to the JRIP rules, the classification was also performed using five other common classifiers: Naïve Bayes, Logistic Regression, Simple Logistic, Support Vector Machine, and Random Forest. Their classification accuracy was estimated using the k-fold cross validation technique (k = 10), yielding a performance slightly higher than JRIP (Supplementary Materials Table S6).
The most accurate classifier in classifying L-SB individuals (199/252; 79%) and H-SB ones (175/227; 77%) was Simple Logistic (AUC = 0.819, d = 1.289; F1 = 0.78).
As the final step, the classification was repeated, focusing the analysis on the variables that JRIP indicated as exhibiting optimal efficiency in classifying the individuals within the two classes. Taking into account the eight JRIP rules, the variables were (1) DASS-21 subcomponents, Stress, Depression, and Anxiety; (2) fever > 38 °C; (3) systemic symptoms; (4) local symptoms; (5) concerns about adverse reactions; (6) MAIA Noticing, Attention Regulation, and Body Listening; (7) sex; (8) age ≤ 50; (9) PSQI Sleep latency. In this particular instance, with the goal of identifying more specific aspects capable of predicting the SB level after vaccination, the classification was performed using as independent variables the individual items contributing to all the aforementioned variables. Accordingly, we ran the classifiers using the following as inputs: (1) age; (2) sex; (3) fever > 38 °C; (4) systemic symptoms; (5) local symptoms; (6) concerns about adverse reaction; (7) MAIA items from 1 to 4 (MAIA—Noticing); MAIA items from 11 to 17 (MAIA—Attention Regulation), MAIA items from 27 to 29 (MAIA—Body Listening); (8) PSQI items 2 and 5 (sleep latency); and (9) all items of the DASS-21 (DASS-21 Stress, Depression, and Anxiety). In this instance, JRIP yielded an accuracy of 75.37% with a simplified set of five rules:
(1)
If DASS-21 Item 11 (“I found myself getting agitated”) T0 ≥ 1 and MAIA item 2 (“I notice when I am uncomfortable in my body”) ≥ 2 and DASS-21 Item 13 (“I felt down-hearted and blue”) T0 ≥ 1, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(2)
If systemic symptoms are present and DASS-21 Item 12 (“I found it difficult to relax”) T0 ≥ 1, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(3)
If systemic symptoms are present and Sex = Female and Age ≤ 50, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(4)
If systemic symptoms are present and concerns about adverse reaction ≥ 4 and MAIA Item 11 (“I can pay attention to my breath without being distracted by things happening around me”) ≤ 2, then the subject is classified as H-SB;
(5)
If the previous four rules are not applicable, then the individual is classified as an L-SB subject.
The confusion matrix (AUC = 0.694, d = 0.717; F1 = 0.754) showed a correct classification of 180 out of 227 H-SB subjects (accuracy: 79%) and 181 out of 252 L-SB individuals (accuracy: 72%). The accuracies demonstrated by the five main classifiers previously used, using 10-fold cross validation, did not differ significantly compared to the previous one (Supplementary Materials Table S7).

4. Discussion

This study aimed to determine the association between COVID-19 vaccination and SB as well as identify personal characteristics that predict higher SB levels, with a focus on IA. The results revealed a significant increase in SB following vaccine inoculation, accompanied by heightened levels of depression, anxiety, and stress, which are typical symptoms of SB [4]. Indeed, the phenomenon of Sickness Behavior explained part of the exacerbated emotional distress exhibited by patients following vaccination, specifically contributing to a comprehensive understanding of heightened anxiety levels. This insight points out that Sickness Behavior is not necessarily directly associated with a pathological condition. Despite the statistically significant increases in depression, anxiety, and stress levels observed, these elevations do not reach a clinically relevant magnitude. Therefore, it is more reasonable to consider them typical emotional correlates of the body’s response to the immune challenge.
A hierarchical ML analysis identified specific combinations of characteristics associated with higher SB levels, including female sex, younger age, the presence of systemic symptoms, and specific baseline levels of depression, anxiety, stress, sleep latency, and concern about adverse reaction. Furthermore, high SB levels were associated with the absence of local symptoms. These data suggest that the inability to locate the area of pain or discomfort could favor the emergence of a global condition of malaise.
As hypothesized, the role of IA emerged. Three out of eight MAIA components, Noticing, Body Listening, and Attention Regulation, in combination with other factors, were found to contribute to the classifying individuals into the high-SB group. This sheds new light on the role that the awareness of bodily signals may have in modulating perception of health status. Interoception, which involves predicting and controlling bodily signals, plays a vital role in maintaining homeostasis and providing a flexible allostatic response to complex demands [11]. Based on our results, it appears that becoming aware of body discomfort (Noticing and Body Listening) and the ability to sustain and control attention on it at specific levels (Attention Regulation) may increase the possibility of experiencing more severe SB. In particular, the Noticing component refers to abilities to observe with awareness and experience interoceptive stimuli through mind–body listening, regardless of the state of anxiety [12]; we might contend that noticing body discomfort (becoming aware) may increase the possibility of experiencing more severe SB.
The Body Listening component measures the ability to listen to the body, to observe mindfully, and the ability to not have emotional awareness difficulties [12]. It appears that individuals exhibiting systemic symptoms, a prolonged sleep latency (score > 1), together with a specific level of bodily listening (between 2 and 2.3) may be more vulnerable to the perception of high levels of SB.
The Attention Regulation component measures the ability to sustain and control attention on bodily sensations, which is required for accepting them. It enables one to maintain emotional clarity while remaining connected to emotional and sensory experiences [12]. On the contrary, the lack of the ability to mindfully observe bodily sensations could make subjects less able to regulate their own sensations and consequently be more reactive to immune challenges. This effect could be amplified by the co-presence of a higher level of the Noticing component (Rule 4).
In this scenario, possible strategies emerge; an increasing body of evidence indicates that meditation-based practices, such as Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction, can have positive effects on attention control, emotional regulation, and self-awareness by acting specific neural circuits involved in interoceptive processes [25,26]. These could alleviate the distress of perceiving a high level of SB.

5. Limitations

This study has some limitations. Firstly, we encountered challenges in comprehending the reasons behind the altered sleep quality of our sample prior to vaccine administration, as indicated by the results of the PSQI questionnaire. Secondly, although similar, the two groups of patients classified as L-SB and H-SB are not entirely balanced. Thirdly, the methodological approach may be susceptible to recall bias: the memory of the previous emotional state could be influenced by the emotional state experienced at the time of completing the questionnaire. Additionally, it is worth noting that the participants underwent the vaccination from one to three months before responding to the online survey. This can particularly impact the accuracy of pre-vaccination experiences. Fourth, despite identifying some predictors of SB severity, we currently lack an explanation for the underlying mechanisms and their interaction with interoceptive variables.

6. Conclusions

Our findings suggest that SB can be partially attributed to an individual’s emotional disposition at baseline and some components of IA.
It is important to emphasize that SB should not be misconstrued as a negative indicator or evidence that vaccines are harmful. Rather, it reflects a normal response of the body to vaccination. Indeed, from a molecular standpoint, SB can be attributed to the effects of inflammatory mediators that, via the vagus nerve, facilitate communication between the immune response and the CNS following vaccine administration [3]. This physiological response is a part of the body’s natural defense mechanisms and should not be misinterpreted as a vaccine-related issue.
Nevertheless, insights gained from the ML analysis indicate that the integration and regulation of bodily sensations have the potential to alter perceptions of one’s health status. This could potentially aid in identifying characteristics that may be associated with a higher risk of experiencing post-immune challenge malaise, offering a basis for providing tailored support and interventions.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/healthcare11222981/s1, Figure S1: SicknessQ Boxplots; Table S1: first section of the online survey; Table S2: trait characteristics; Supplementary File S1; Table S3: confusion matrix referring to the first preliminary round; Table S4: common classifiers referring to the first preliminary round; Table S5: confusion matrix referring to the second round; Table S6: common classifiers referring to the second round; Table S7: confusion matrix referring to the last round.

Author Contributions

G.A., A.B. and A.G. conceived and planned the experiment; G.A., G.O. and D.M. participated in all phases of the work; D.M. and M.M. performed statistical analyses: G.O. performed machine learning analyses; V.C. and M.T. contributed to data collection, data interpretation, and reviewing the work. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Pisa (protocol code 14/2021 and date of approval 28 May 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Orrù, G.; Conversano, C.; Malloggi, E.; Francesconi, F.; Ciacchini, R.; Gemignani, A. Neurological complications of COVID-19 and possible neuroinvasion pathways: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Moghadas, S.M.; Vilches, T.N.; Zhang, K.; Wells, C.R.; Shoukat, A.; Singer, B.H.; Meyers, L.A.; Neuzil, K.M.; Langley, J.M.; Fitzpatrick, M.C.; et al. The Impact of Vaccination on Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Outbreaks in the United States. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2021, 73, 2257–2264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hervé, C.; Laupèze, B.; Del Giudice, G.; Didierlaurent, A.M.; Tavares Da Silva, F. The how’s and what’s of vaccine reactogenicity. NPJ Vaccines 2019, 4, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Dantzer, R. Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: A neuroimmune response to activation of innate immunity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2004, 500, 399–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Dantzer, R. Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depression. Neurol. Clin. 2006, 24, 441–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Shattuck, E.C.; Muehlenbein, M.P. Human sickness behavior: Ultimate and proximate explanations. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2015, 157, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Pinna, T.; Edwards, D.J. A Systematic Review of Associations between Interoception, Vagal Tone, and Emotional Regulation: Potential Applications for Mental Health, Wellbeing, Psychological Flexibility, and Chronic Conditions. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Lasselin, J. Back to the future of psychoneuroimmunology: Studying inflammation-induced sickness behavior. Brain Behav. Immun.-Health 2021, 18, 100379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Nord, C.L.; Garfinkel, S.N. Interoceptive pathways to understand and treat mental health conditions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 499–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Savitz, J.; Harrison, N.A. Interoception and Inflammation in Psychiatric Disorders. Biological psychiatry. Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2018, 3, 514–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Sennesh, E.; Theriault, J.; Brooks, D.; van de Meent, J.W.; Barrett, L.F.; Quigley, K.S. Interoception as modeling, allostasis as control. Biol. Psychol. 2022, 167, 108242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Mehling, W.E.; Price, C.; Daubenmier, J.J.; Acree, M.; Bartmess, E.; Stewart, A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Andreasson, A.; Wicksell, R.K.; Lodin, K.; Karshikoff, B.; Axelsson, J.; Lekander, M. A global measure of sickness behaviour: Development of the Sickness Questionnaire. J. Health Psychol. 2018, 23, 1452–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Bottesi, G.; Ghisi, M.; Altoè, G.; Conforti, E.; Melli, G.; Sica, C. The Italian version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21: Factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr. Psychiatry 2015, 60, 170–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Curcio, G.; Tempesta, D.; Scarlata, S.; Marzano, C.; Moroni, F.; Rossini, P.M.; Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L. Validity of the Italian version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Neurol. Sci. 2013, 34, 511–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. WHO. The Icd-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fritz, C.O.; Morris, P.E.; Richler, J.J. Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences; Lawrence Erbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen, W.W. Fast effective rule induction. In Machine Learning Proceedings 1995; Morgan Kaufmann: Burlington, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 115–123. [Google Scholar]
  20. John, G.H.; Langley, P. Estimating continuous distributions in bayesian classifiers. In Proceedings of the Eleventh Conference on Uncertainty in Artificial Intelligence, Montreal, QU, Canada, 18–20 August 1995; Morgan Kaufmann Publishers Inc.: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1995; pp. 338–345. [Google Scholar]
  21. Le Cessie, S.; Van Houwelingen, J.C. Ridge estimators in logistic regression. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C (Appl. Stat.) 1992, 41, 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Sumner, M.; Frank, E.; Hall, M. Speeding up logistic model tree induction. In European Conference on Principles of Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2005; pp. 675–683. [Google Scholar]
  23. Platt, J. Sequential Minimal Optimization: A Fast Algorithm for Training Support Vector Machines; Microsoft: Redmond, DC, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  24. Breiman, L. Random forests. Mach. Learn. 2001, 45, 5–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, W.G.; Schloesser, D.; Arensdorf, A.M.; Simmons, J.M.; Cui, C.; Valentino, R.; Gnadt, J.W.; Nielsen, L.; Hillaire-Clarke, C.S.; Spruance, V.; et al. The Emerging Science of Interoception: Sensing, Integrating, Interpreting, and Regulating Signals within the Self. Trends Neurosci. 2021, 44, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Farb, N.; Daubenmier, J.; Price, C.J.; Gard, T.; Kerr, C.; Dunn, B.D.; Klein, A.C.; Paulus, M.P.; Mehling, W.E. Interoception, contemplative practice, and health. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants (n = 647).
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants (n = 647).
Demographic Characteristics
Sex (male)59.35% (n = 384)
Age49.87 (±10.568)
Physical activity practitioners72.64% (n = 470)
Civil Status
Unmarried/nubile29.67% (n = 192)
Married60.43% (n = 391)
Separated8.65% (n = 56)
Widower1.23% (n = 8)
Main Diseases During Lifespan
Class II (neoplasm)3.40% (n = 22)
Class IV (endocrine, nutritional, and metabolic diseases)7.88% (n = 51)
Class IX (diseases of the circulatory System)5.56% (n = 36)
Class X (diseases of the respiratory System)2.47% (n = 16)
Class XI (diseases of the digestive System1.39% (n = 9)
Past COVID-19 positivity2.93% (n = 19)
Presence of an immunocompromised state0.15% (n = 1)
Other diseases5.1% (n = 33)
Note: frequencies expressed in %; mean (±standard deviation).
Table 2. Adverse effects to vaccination (n = 647).
Table 2. Adverse effects to vaccination (n = 647).
No Reaction13.60% (n = 88)
Fever (>38 °C)36.93% (n = 239)
Local Symptoms
Swelling or redness where the injection is given6.49% (n = 42)
Tenderness, pain, warmth, itching, or bruising where the injection was given40.05% (n = 270)
Systemic Symptoms
Feeling tired (fatigue) or generally feeling unwell55.17% (n = 357)
Chills or a feeling of fever40.80% (n = 264)
Tenderness40.95% (n = 265)
Joint pain or muscle pain40.49% (n = 262)
Headache39.10% (n = 253)
Feeling sick (nausea)10.04% (n = 65)
Malaise (vomiting or diarrhea)3.86% (n = 25)
Drowsiness or feelings of dizziness9.42% (n = 61)
Decreased appetite5.40% (n = 35)
Enlarged lymph nodes2.00% (n = 13)
Excessive sweating, itching, or rash2.16% (n = 14)
Table 3. Repeated-measures ANOVA analysis for state variables referring to baseline and post COVID-19 vaccination (T0 and T1, respectively).
Table 3. Repeated-measures ANOVA analysis for state variables referring to baseline and post COVID-19 vaccination (T0 and T1, respectively).
State VariablesMeanStandard
Deviation
p-ValueObserved PowerEffect
Size
SicknessQ T01.57(±2.72)0.0011.000.77
SicknessQ T15.54(±5.51)
DASS-21 Depression T01.06(±2.56)0.0011.000.49
DASS-21 Depression T11.32(±2.74)
DASS-21 Anxiety T00.65(±1.64)0.0011.000.48
DASS-21 Anxiety T11.11(±2.22)
DASS-21 Stress T02.12(±3.27)0.0010.8720.22
DASS-21 Stress T12.34(±3.51)
DASS-21 Total Score T03.83(±6.61)0.0011.000.49
DASS-21 Total Score T14.77(±7.53)
PSQI Total Score T06.03(±3.09)0.4090.1310.017
PSQI Total Score T16.08(±3.25)
Table 4. Repeated-measures ANCOVA for state variables achieving statistical significance when comparing baseline and post-COVID-19 vaccination results.
Table 4. Repeated-measures ANCOVA for state variables achieving statistical significance when comparing baseline and post-COVID-19 vaccination results.
State VariablesFp-ValueObserved Power
DASS-21 Depression10.2280.0010.891
DASS-21 Depression * Delta SB162.1840.0001.00
DASS-21 Anxiety0.3620.5480.092
DASS-21 Anxiety * Delta SB149.8470.0001.00
DASS-21 Stress6.1530.0130.697
DASS-21 Stress * Delta SB62.7320.0001.00
DASS-21 Total Score6.4920.0110.720
DASS-21 Total Score * Delta SB166.0870.0001.00
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Alfì, G.; Orrù, G.; Menicucci, D.; Miccoli, M.; Casigliani, V.; Totaro, M.; Baggiani, A.; Gemignani, A. A Machine Learning Approach Unveils the Relationships between Sickness Behavior and Interoception after Vaccination: Suggestions for Psychometric Indices of Higher Vulnerability. Healthcare 2023, 11, 2981. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare11222981

AMA Style

Alfì G, Orrù G, Menicucci D, Miccoli M, Casigliani V, Totaro M, Baggiani A, Gemignani A. A Machine Learning Approach Unveils the Relationships between Sickness Behavior and Interoception after Vaccination: Suggestions for Psychometric Indices of Higher Vulnerability. Healthcare. 2023; 11(22):2981. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare11222981

Chicago/Turabian Style

Alfì, Gaspare, Graziella Orrù, Danilo Menicucci, Mario Miccoli, Virginia Casigliani, Michele Totaro, Angelo Baggiani, and Angelo Gemignani. 2023. "A Machine Learning Approach Unveils the Relationships between Sickness Behavior and Interoception after Vaccination: Suggestions for Psychometric Indices of Higher Vulnerability" Healthcare 11, no. 22: 2981. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare11222981

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop