Do Moral Emotions Interact with Self-Control and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Rule-Breaking Behavior Committed Together with Friends?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Situational Action Theory
1.2. Individual Propensity to Engage in Rule-Breaking Behavior
1.2.1. Moral Emotions
1.2.2. Self-Control
1.3. Unstructured Socializing as an Situational Factor That Promotes Rule-Breaking Behavior
1.4. Friends’ Influence on Adolescents’ Rule-Breaking Behavior
1.5. The Present Study
- Do moral emotions (a. guilt- and b. shame proneness as well as c. AEMC) interact with self-control in explaining own rule-breaking behavior?
- Do moral emotions (a. guilt- and b. shame proneness as well as c. AEMC) interact with self-control in explaining rule-breaking with friends?
- Do moral emotions (a. guilt- and b. shame proneness as well as c. AEMC) interact with unstructured socializing in explaining own rule-breaking behavior?
- Do moral emotions (a. guilt- and b. shame proneness as well as c. AEMC) interact with unstructured socializing in explaining rule-breaking with friends?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Guilt- and Shame-Proneness
2.2.2. Anticipated Emotions in Moral Conflicts
2.2.3. Self-Control
2.2.4. Unstructured Socializing
2.2.5. Own Rule-Breaking Behavior
2.2.6. Rule-Breaking with Friends
2.3. Data Analytic Strategy
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive and Correlational Analysis
3.2. Interaction between Self-Control and Moral Emotions
3.3. Interaction between Unstructured Socializing and Moral Emotions
4. Discussion
4.1. Interaction between Self-Control and Moral Emotions
4.2. Interaction between Unstructured Socializing and Moral Emotions
5. Limitations and Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Original Data | Combined Score Imputed Data | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables (No. of Items) | M | SD | SE | Min | Max | M | SD | SE | Min | Max |
Guilt (15) | 51.9 (N = 156) | 10.5 | 0.84 | 21 | 75 | 51.8 | 10.7 | 0.83 | 16 | 75 |
Shame (15) | 42.1 (N = 154) | 11.8 | 0.95 | 15 | 75 | 42.2 | 11.7 | 0.90 | 15 | 75 |
AEMC (6) | 22.2 (N = 163) | 4.3 | 0.34 | 6 | 30 | 22.3 | 4.3 | 0.33 | 6 | 30 |
Low self-control (15) | 43.4 (N = 164) | 8.1 | 0.63 | 19 | 62 | 43.5 | 8.0 | 0.62 | 19 | 62 |
Unstructured socializing (4) | 10.7 (N = 165) | 3.8 | 0.29 | 4 | 19 | 10.7 | 3.8 | 0.29 | 4 | 19 |
Own rule-breaking (20) | 30.5 (N = 158) | 8.5 | 0.68 | 20 | 60 | 30.5 | 8.4 | 0.64 | 20 | 60 |
Rule-breaking w. friends (13) | 18.2 (N = 121) | 6.3 | 0.58 | 13 | 39 | 18.6 | 5.6 | 0.43 | 13 | 39 |
References
- Wikström, P.H.; Treiber, K. Situational Theory: The Importance of Interactions and Action Mechanisms in the Explanation of Crime. In The Handbook of Criminological Theory; Piquero, A.R., Ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 415–444. ISBN 978-1-118-51238-8. [Google Scholar]
- Wikström, P.-O.H. Situational Action Theory: A General, Dynamic and Mechanism-Based Theory of Crime and Its Causes. In Handbook on Crime and Deviance; Krohn, M.D., Hendrix, N., Lizotte, A.J., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 259–281. ISBN 978-3-030-20778-6. [Google Scholar]
- Wikström, P.-O.H.; Svensson, R. When Does Self-Control Matter? The Interaction between Morality and Self-Control in Crime Causation. Eur. J. Criminol. 2010, 7, 395–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Svensson, R.; Weerman, F.M.; Pauwels, L.J.R.; Bruinsma, G.J.N.; Bernasco, W. Moral Emotions and Offending: Do Feelings of Anticipated Shame and Guilt Mediate the Effect of Socialization on Offending? Eur. J. Criminol. 2013, 10, 22–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dollinger, B.; Schmidt-Semisch, H. Sozialpädagogik und Kriminologie im Dialog Einführende Perspektiven zum Ereignis “Jugendkriminalität”. In Handbuch Jugendkriminalität; Springer: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2018; ISBN 978-3-531-19952-8. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion, T.J.; Tipsord, J.M. Peer Contagion in Child and Adolescent Social and Emotional Development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 189–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engel, C. How Little Does It Take to Trigger a Peer Effect? An Experiment on Crime as Conditional Rule Violation. J. Res. Crime Delinq. 2023, 60, 455–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallupe, O.; McLevey, J.; Brown, S. Selection and Influence: A Meta-Analysis of the Association between Peer and Personal Offending. J. Quant. Criminol. 2019, 35, 313–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGloin, J.M.; Thomas, K.J. Peer Influence and Delinquency. Annu. Rev. Criminol. 2019, 2, 241–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J.P.; Stuewig, J.; Mashek, D.J. Moral Emotions and Moral Behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 345–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eisenberg, N. Emotion, Regulation, and Moral Development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 665–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindsay-Hartz, J. Contrasting Experiences of Shame and Guilt. Am. Behav. Sci. 1984, 27, 689–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wicker, F.W.; Payne, G.C.; Morgan, R.D. Participant Descriptions of Guilt and Shame. Motiv. Emot. 1983, 7, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maxwell, S.R.; Zepeda, A.; Rzotkiewicz, A. Peers, Social Control, or Guilt? Self-Conscious Emotions as Explanation for Antisocial Behavior. Deviant Behav. 2018, 39, 322–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spruit, A.; Schalkwijk, F.; van Vugt, E.; Stams, G.J. The Relation between Self-Conscious Emotions and Delinquency: A Meta-Analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2016, 28, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walters, G.D. Conscience and Delinquency: A Developmentally Informed Meta-Analysis. Dev. Rev. 2022, 65, 101026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Hooge, I.E.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Zeelenberg, M. Not so Ugly after All: When Shame Acts as a Commitment Device. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 933–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tangney, J.P.; Stuewig, J.; Martinez, A.G. Two Faces of Shame: The Roles of Shame and Guilt in Predicting Recidivism. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 799–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maggi, S.; Zaccaria, V.; Breda, M.; Romani, M.; Aceti, F.; Giacchetti, N.; Ardizzone, I.; Sogos, C. A Narrative Review about Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior in Childhood: The Relationship with Shame and Moral Development. Children 2022, 10, 1556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J.P.; Wagner, P.E.; Hill-Barlow, D.; Marschall, D.E.; Gramzow, R. Relation of Shame and Guilt to Constructive versus Destructive Responses to Anger across the Lifespan. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 797–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christensen, J.F.; Gomila, A. Moral Dilemmas in Cognitive Neuroscience of Moral Decision-Making: A Principled Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1249–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hirschi, T. Self-Control and Crime. In Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications; Baumeister, R.F., Vohs, K.D., Penly Hall, G., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 538–552. ISBN 978-1-57230-991-3. [Google Scholar]
- Vazsonyi, A.T.; Mikuška, J.; Kelley, E.L. It’s Time: A Meta-Analysis on the Self-Control-Deviance Link. J. Crim. Justice 2017, 48, 48–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Svensson, R.; Pauwels, L.; Weerman, F.M. Does the Effect of Self-Control on Adolescent Offending Vary by Level of Morality?: A Test in Three Countries. Crim. Justice Behav. 2010, 37, 732–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerstner, D.; Oberwittler, D. Who’s Hanging out and What’s Happening? A Look at the Interplay between Unstructured Socializing, Crime Propensity and Delinquent Peers Using Social Network Data. Eur. J. Criminol. 2018, 15, 111–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, L.E.; Felson, M. Social Change and Crime Rate Trends: A Routine Activity Approach. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1979, 44, 588–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osgood, D.W.; Wilson, J.K.; O’Malley, P.M.; Bachman, J.G.; Johnston, L.D. Routine Activities and Individual Deviant Behavior. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1996, 61, 635–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weerman, F.M.; Bernasco, W.; Bruinsma, G.J.N.; Pauwels, L.J.R. When Is Spending Time with Peers Related to Delinquency? The Importance of Where, What, and with Whom. Crime Delinq. 2015, 61, 1386–1413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoeben, E.M.; Meldrum, R.C.; Walker, D.; Young, J.T.N. The Role of Peer Delinquency and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Delinquency and Substance Use: A State-of-the-Art Review. J. Crim. Justice 2016, 47, 108–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akers, R.L.; Jennings, W.G. Social Learning Theory of Crime and Deviance. In Handbook in Crime and Deviance; Krohn, M.D., Hendrix, N., Penly Hall, G., Lizotte, A.J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2019; pp. 113–129. [Google Scholar]
- Pratt, T.C.; Cullen, F.T.; Sellers, C.S.; Thomas Winfree, L.; Madensen, T.D.; Daigle, L.E.; Fearn, N.E.; Gau, J.M. The Empirical Status of Social Learning Theory: A Meta-Analysis. Justice Q. 2010, 27, 765–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, J.C.; Brown, R.J.; Tajfel, H. Social Comparison and Group Interest in Ingroup Favouritism. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1979, 9, 187–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hogg, M.A. Social Idenity Theory. In Understanding Peace and Conflict through Social Identity Theory: Contemporary Global Perspectives; McKeown, S., Haji, R., Ferguson, N., Eds.; Peace Psychology Book Series; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 3–17. ISBN 978-3-319-29867-2. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Spiess, M.; Goebel, J. On the Effect of Item Nonresponse on the Estimation of a Two-Panel-Waves Wage Equation. Allg. Stat. Arch. 2005, 89, 63–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, S.D.; Gomez, R.; Gullone, E. The Shame and Guilt Scales of the Test of Self-Conscious Affect-Adolescent (TOSCA-A): Psychometric Properties for Responses from Children, and Measurement Invariance Across Children and Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koglin, U.; Daseking, M. Fragebogen zur Erfassung Moralischer Einstellung im Jugendalter, Carl von Ossietzky University Oldenburg: Oldenburg, Germany, 2019; Unpublished publication.
- Weller, D.; Lagattuta, K.H. Children’s Judgments about Prosocial Decisions and Emotions: Gender of the Helper and Recipient Matters. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 2011–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tischler, T.; Petermann, F.; Herzberg, P.Y.; Daseking, M. Schulbezogene Persönlichkeit und Ressourcen im Alter von 10–16 Jahren. Hemult-Schmidt-University. Unpublished publication, in preperation.
- Achenbach, T.M. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 Profiles; Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont: Burlington, VT, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Döpfner, M.; Plück, J.; Kinnen, C.; Achenbach, T.M. Deutsche Schulalter-Formen der Child Behavior. Elternfragebogen Über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen (CBCL/6-18R), Lehrerfragebogen Über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen (TRF/6-18R), Fragebogen Für Jugendliche (YSR/11-18R); Manual; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Pauwels, L.J.R.; Svensson, R.; Hirtenlehner, H. Testing Situational Action Theory: A Narrative Review of Studies Published between 2006 and 2015. Eur. J. Criminol. 2018, 15, 32–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallupe, O.; Baron, S.W. Morality, Self-Control, Deterrence, and Drug Use: Street Youths and Situational Action Theory. Crime Delinq. 2014, 60, 284–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antonaccio, O.; Tittle, C.R. Morality, Self-Control, and Crime. Criminology 2008, 46, 479–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruinsma, G.J.N.; Pauwels, L.J.R.; Weerman, F.M.; Bernasco, W. Situational Action Theory: Cross-Sectional and Cross-Lagged Tests of Its Core Propositions. Can. J. Criminol. Crim. Justice 2015, 57, 363–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosnjak, M.; Tuten, T.L. Classifying Response Behaviors in Web-Based Surveys. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 2001, 6, JCMC636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Sex | Female: 54% | Male: 46% |
---|---|---|
Age (in years) | M = 14.95, SD = 1.7 Min = 12, Max = 18 | |
School type | Grammar school: 56% | Others: 44% |
Birth country | Germany/Switzerland: 93% | Others: 7% |
Language spoken at home | German: 72% | Others: 28% |
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) Age | 1 | −0.16 (0.039) | −0.18 (0.020) | −0.21 (0.006) | −0.19 (0.014) | 0.12 (0.116) | 0.39 (<0.001) | 0.151 (0.051) | 0.22 (0.004) |
(2) Gender | −0.16 (0.039) | 1 | 0.13 (0.094) | 0.28 (<0.001) | 0.19 (0.014) | −0.09 (0.256) | −0.04 (0.582) | −0.07 (0.362) | 0.005 (0.948) |
(3) Guilt | 1 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
(4) Shame | 0.59 (<0.001) | 1 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
(5) Anticipated emotions in moral conflicts (AEMC) | 0.63 (<0.001) | 0.32 (<0.001) | 1 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
(6) Low self-control | −0.27 (<0.001) | −0.08 (0.324) | −0.24 (0.002) | 1 | - | - | - | - | - |
(7) Unstructured socializing | −0.13 (0.092) | −0.14 (0.080) | −0.11 (0.177) | 0.32 (<0.001) | 1 | - | - | - | - |
(8) Own rule-breaking | −0.23 (0.003) | 0.08 (0.301) | −0.33 (<0.001) | 0.50 (<0.001) | 0.55 (<0.001) | 1 | - | - | - |
(9) Rule-breaking w. friends | −0.14 (0.075) | 0.07 (0.393) | −0.25 (<0.001) | 0.33 (<0.001) | 0.47 (<0.001) | 0.812 (<0.001) | 1 | - | - |
N = 169 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | |
Own rule-breaking | ||||||||||||
Low self-control | 0.45 | 0.31–0.58 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.49 | 0.35–0.62 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.48 | 0.34–0.62 | 0.07 | <0.001 |
Guilt | −0.09 | −0.24–0.06 | 0.07 | 0.270 | −0.09 | −0.24–0.06 | 0.08 | 0.226 | −0.10 | −0.25–0.05 | 0.08 | 0.192 |
Shame | 0.18 | 0.07–0.29 | 0.06 | 0.001 | 0.17 | 0.06–0.28 | 0.06 | 0.003 | 0.19 | 0.07–0.30 | 0.06 | 0.002 |
AEMC | −0.47 | −0.79–−0.16 | 0.16 | 0.003 | −0.42 | −0.73–−0.11 | 0.16 | 0.008 | −0.39 | −0.70–−0.07 | 0.16 | 0.017 |
Low self-control × guilt | – | – | – | – | 0.01 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.344 | 0.01 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.359 |
Low self-control × shame | – | – | – | – | 0.00 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.580 | 0.00 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.598 |
Low self-control × AEMC | – | – | – | – | −0.06 | −0.10–−0.02 | 0.02 | 0.002 | −0.06 | −0.10–−0.02 | 0.02 | 0.003 |
Gender | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | −0.68 | −2.87–1.50 | 1.11 | 0.540 |
Age | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 0.32 | −0.31–0.95 | 0.32 | 0.319 |
R2 (corr. R2) | 0.34 (0.32) | – | – | – | 0.38 (0.36) | – | – | – | 0.39 (0.35) | |||
Rule-breaking with friends | ||||||||||||
Low self-control | 0.20 | 0.09–0.31 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.22 | 0.11–0.33 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.21 | 0.10–0.32 | 0.06 | <0.001 |
Guilt | −0.01 | −0.13–0.10 | 0.06 | 0.826 | −0.02 | −0.13–0.10 | 0.06 | 0.793 | −0.01 | −0.13–0.10 | 0.06 | 0.831 |
Shame | 0.08 | −0.01–0.17 | 0.04 | 0.057 | 0.08 | −0.01–0.17 | 0.05 | 0.082 | 0.09 | −0.01–0.18 | 0.05 | 0.066 |
AEMC | −0.30 | −0.54–−0.05 | 0.12 | 0.017 | −0.27 | −0.52–−0.03 | 0.13 | 0.029 | −0.26 | −0.50–−0.01 | 0.13 | 0.040 |
Low self-control × guilt | – | – | – | – | 0.00 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.613 | 0.00 | −0.01–0.02 | 0.01 | 0.688 |
Low self-control × shame | – | – | – | – | 0.00 | −0.01–0.01 | 0.01 | 0.896 | 0.00 | −0.01–0.01 | 0.01 | 0.839 |
Low self-control × AEMC | – | – | – | – | −0.03 | −0.06–0.00 | 0.02 | 0.053 | −0.03 | −0.06–0.01 | 0.02 | 0.095 |
Gender | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 0.54 | −1.15–2.23 | 0.86 | 0.533 |
Age | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 0.59 | 0.11–1.08 | 0.25 | 0.017 |
R2 (corr. R2) | 0.17 (0.15) | – | – | – | 0.19 (0.16) | – | – | – | 0.22 (0.18) | – | – | – |
N = 169 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | B | 95% CI | SE(B) | p | |
Own rule-breaking | ||||||||||||
Unstructured socializing | 1.21 | 0.95–1.47 | 0.13 | <0.001 | 1.17 | 0.92–1.41 | 0.13 | <0.001 | 1.21 | 0.94–1.48 | 0.14 | <0.001 |
Guilt | −0.16 | −0.29–−0.03 | 0.07 | 0.020 | −0.25 | −0.38–−0.11 | 0.07 | <0.001 | −0.25 | −0.38–−0.12 | 0.07 | <0.001 |
Shame | 0.26 | 0.15–0.37 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.32 | 0.22–0.42 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.33 | 0.23–0.43 | 0.05 | <0.001 |
AEMC | −0.51 | −0.79–−0.22 | 0.15 | <0.001 | −0.42 | −0.69–−0.15 | 0.14 | 0.003 | −0.41 | −0.69–−0.13 | 0.14 | 0.004 |
Unstructured social. × guilt | – | – | – | – | −0.07 | −0.11–−0.04 | 0.02 | <0.001 | −0.07 | −0.10–−0.03 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Unstructured social. × shame | – | – | – | – | 0.07 | 0.04–0.10 | 0.02 | <0.001 | 0.07 | 0.04–0.10 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Unstructured social. × AEMC | – | – | – | – | 0.04 | −0.03–0.10 | 0.03 | 0.274 | 0.04 | −0.03–0.11 | 0.03 | 0.234 |
Gender | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | −1.06 | −2.99–0.87 | 0.99 | 0.283 |
Age | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | −0.26 | −0.86–0.35 | 0.31 | 0.403 |
R2 (corr. R2) | 0.46 (0.44) | – | – | – | 0.53 (0.51) | – | – | – | 0.53 (0.51) | – | – | – |
Rule-breaking with friends | ||||||||||||
Unstructured socializing | 0.70 | 0.51–0.90 | 0.10 | <0.001 | 0.68 | 0.49–0.88 | 0.10 | <0.001 | 0.63 | 0.41–0.84 | 0.11 | <0.001 |
Guilt | −0.04 | −0.15–0.06 | 0.05 | 0.400 | −0.11 | −0.21–−0.01 | 0.05 | 0.043 | −0.11 | −0.21–−0.01 | 0.05 | 0.041 |
Shame | 0.12 | 0.04–0.20 | 0.04 | 0.003 | 0.17 | 0.09–0.25 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 0.18 | 0.09–0.26 | 0.04 | <0.001 |
AEMC | −0.31 | −0.53–−0.08 | 0.11 | 0.007 | −0.24 | −0.45–−0.02 | 0.11 | 0.030 | −0.23 | −0.45–−0.01 | 0.11 | 0.038 |
Unstructured social. × guilt | – | – | – | – | −0.05 | −0.08–−0.03 | 0.01 | <0.001 | −0.06 | −0.09–−0.03 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Unstructured social. × shame | – | – | – | – | 0.05 | 0.03–0.07 | 0.01 | <0.001 | 0.05 | 0.03–0.08 | 0.01 | <0.001 |
Unstructured social. × AEMC | – | – | – | – | 0.05 | −0.01–0.10 | 0.03 | 0.086 | 0.05 | −0.01–0.10 | 0.03 | 0.095 |
Gender | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 0.48 | −1.06–2.01 | 0.78 | 0.541 |
Age | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 0.32 | −0.17–0.80 | 0.25 | 0.195 |
R2 (corr. R2) | 0.31 (0.29) | – | – | – | 0.40 (0.37) | – | – | – | 0.40 (0.37) | – | – | – |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Schön, S.-M.; Daseking, M. Do Moral Emotions Interact with Self-Control and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Rule-Breaking Behavior Committed Together with Friends? Children 2024, 11, 766. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11070766
Schön S-M, Daseking M. Do Moral Emotions Interact with Self-Control and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Rule-Breaking Behavior Committed Together with Friends? Children. 2024; 11(7):766. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11070766
Chicago/Turabian StyleSchön, Sara-Marie, and Monika Daseking. 2024. "Do Moral Emotions Interact with Self-Control and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Rule-Breaking Behavior Committed Together with Friends?" Children 11, no. 7: 766. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11070766
APA StyleSchön, S.-M., & Daseking, M. (2024). Do Moral Emotions Interact with Self-Control and Unstructured Socializing in Explaining Rule-Breaking Behavior Committed Together with Friends? Children, 11(7), 766. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11070766