Next Article in Journal
Histomorphological Changes in Fish Gut in Response to Prebiotics and Probiotics Treatment to Improve Their Health Status: A Review
Next Article in Special Issue
Pig Sedation and Anesthesia for Medical Research
Previous Article in Journal
Comparison between the Effect of Lidocaine or Ropivacaine Hematoma Block and of Different Timings of Administration on Post-Operative Pain in Dogs Undergoing Osteosynthesis of Long-Bone Fractures
Previous Article in Special Issue
Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part II: Embryonal Development of Electroencephalic Neuronal Activity In Ovo as a Prerequisite for Nociception
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part III: Analysis of Movements before and after Application of a Noxious Stimulus

1
Center for Preclinical Research, TUM School of Medicine, Technical University of Munich, 81675 Munich, Bavaria, Germany
2
Chair of Zoology, TUM School of Life Sciences Weihenstephan, Technical University of Munich, 85354 Freising, Bavaria, Germany
3
Clinic for Swine, Center for Clinical Veterinary Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 85764 Oberschleißheim, Bavaria, Germany
4
Clinic for Anesthesiology and Intensive Care, TUM School of Medicine, Technical University of Munich, 81675 Munich, Bavaria, Germany
5
Institute of Pharmacology, Toxicology and Pharmacy, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 80539 Munich, Bavaria, Germany
6
Reproductive Biotechnology, TUM School of Life Sciences Weihenstephan, Technical University of Munich, 85354 Freising, Bavaria, Germany
7
Veterinary Faculty, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 80539 Munich, Bavaria, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(18), 2859; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182859
Submission received: 20 July 2023 / Revised: 28 August 2023 / Accepted: 7 September 2023 / Published: 8 September 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Anaesthesia and Pain Management in Large Animals)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Chicken embryos are currently being increasingly used in various areas of research but are frequently not covered by animal protection legislation. In the food industry, it is often even common practice to kill male embryos because they are of no economic use. In both cases, there is a lack of knowledge about the sentience of these chicken embryos, especially their ability to perceive pain. The purpose of this exploratory study was to investigate whether—and if so, on which developmental day—a chicken embryo shows a behavioral change or a specific response to a noxious stimulus, both of which would be indicative of functioning nociception. Two complementary approaches were used for the evaluation: human observation and computer-assisted evaluation using a markerless pose estimation software (DeepLabCut). Through a combination of both approaches, it became apparent that developmental day 15 was the earliest stage at which a response to the applied stimulus was detectable. This result thus represents a contribution to the future improvement of animal welfare as it suggests that from developmental day 15 a chicken embryo in the egg has the capacity to show a nocifensive reaction.

Abstract

Many potentially noxious interventions are performed on chicken embryos in research and in the poultry industry. It is therefore essential and in the interest of animal welfare to be able to precisely define the point at which a chicken embryo is capable of nociception in ovo. The present part III of a comprehensive study examined the movements of developing chicken embryos with the aim of identifying behavioral responses to a noxious stimulus. For this purpose, a noxious mechanical stimulus and a control stimulus were applied in a randomized order. The recorded movements of the embryos were evaluated using the markerless pose estimation software DeepLabCut and manual observations. After the application of the mechanical stimulus, a significant increase in beak movement was identified in 15- to 18-day-old embryos. In younger embryos, no behavioral changes related to the noxious stimulus were observed. The presented results indicate that noxious mechanical stimuli at the beak base evoke a nocifensive reaction in chicken embryos starting at embryonic day 15.

1. Introduction

The behavior of birds can profoundly differ from the behavior of mammals, especially in terms of indications of pain [1]. For a long time, birds were not believed to feel pain [1]. At present, it is generally accepted that birds are capable of nociception and can feel pain [1,2]. Several studies have established that birds have mechanothermal, mechanical and thermal nociceptors with high stimulus thresholds [2,3]. Furthermore, peripheral and central processing of a potentially noxious stimulus in birds occurs in a similar manner to that in mammals [4]. Raja et al. defined pain as an aversive experience of an individual that includes both sensory perception and emotional aspects [5]. This experience may be caused by a potential or actual lesion of the tissue [5]. Nociception, on the other hand, is described as the detection of a potentially damaging stimulus by primary sensory neurons and its processing in the nervous system [5,6]. The inability to communicate does not exclude the possibility that pain is felt, for example, by animals or neonates [1,5]. Another definition of pain more suitable for assessing pain in animals includes changes in species-specific behavior as a possible consequence of a painful experience [7]. Because pain is a subjective experience, its assessment is difficult in humans and is even more challenging in animals [1,5]. Detection and quantification of pain in animals involves inference from parameters associated with pain in humans [1].
Birds show only subtle behaviors of discomfort or pain due to the disadvantage of showing weakness in a social group or as a prey species in general as well as the potential predominance of the flight reflex [8]. In addition, bird behavior varies greatly among species and individuals, making it necessary to closely examine the typical behavior of the observed individual. This makes it possible to assess deviations in typical behavior as a sign of pain [9]. Although pain-associated behavior is difficult to identify, its major advantage is that it can be observed immediately and noninvasively [3,9]. This makes behavioral observation an essential part of a comprehensive pain assessment in birds.
Behavioral studies have been conducted in a variety of avian species [10]. Many of these studies used chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) and evaluated nociceptive responses to procedures that are assumed to be painful or elicit discomfort [10,11]. The typical behavior of chicken embryos has long attracted scientific interest [10,11]. In the 1960s, the motility of chicken embryos was intensively studied. Movements and motility patterns, along with other aspects, were observed from days 3.5 to 20 of incubation [12,13,14,15]. In contrast, little is known about nociception in the chicken embryo or about nocifensive behavioral responses. According to current understanding, nociception in chicken embryos does not occur before the seventh day of incubation [16,17,18].
The results presented are part of a comprehensive study investigating the developmental day at which chicken embryos are capable of nociception and pain perception. The aim of the present part III of the study was to evaluate the acute behavioral responses of chicken embryos at different developmental stages to a noxious mechanical stimulus. The markerless pose estimation software DeepLabCut (DLC) and manual observations were used to analyze embryonic behavior [19,20,21]. In addition, cardiovascular [22] and electrophysiological [23] parameters were investigated in parts I and II of the comprehensive study.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals and Incubation

Chicken embryos from ED9 to ED18 were analyzed. An overview of the experimental groups is provided in Table 1. Fertilized Lohman Selected Leghorn eggs were obtained from the Technical University of Munich (TUM) Animal Research Centre, Thalhausen. Eggs were disinfected (Röhnfried Desinfektion Pro, Dr. Hesse Tierpharma GmbH & Co., Ltd. KG, Hohenlockstedt, Germany), weighed and stored in a refrigerator at 15 °C until use. The maximum storage time from the day of laying until the start of the incubation was seven days. Before incubation, the eggs were placed at room temperature for 24 h. On the day of incubation, eggs were transferred at 8:30 am into a standard incubator (HEKA Favorit-Olymp 192 Spezial, HEKA-Brutgeräte, Rietberg, Germany) and incubated under the following conditions: 37.8 °C temperature and 55% humidity. The eggs were turned six times a day until fenestration on ED3. The first day of incubation was defined as ED0.
On ED3, eggs were placed horizontally for two minutes, and 5–7 mL of albumin was withdrawn through a small hole at the pointed pole using a cannula. A small window was cut in the top of the eggshell, and 0.5 mL of penicillin-streptomycin (10,000 units penicillin, 10 mg streptomycin/mL, P4333–100 mL, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) was added. Eggs were sealed with plastic film and tape. With the eggs in a horizontal position, the incubation proceeded until the desired embryonic day [24].
At the end of the experiments, the embryos were euthanized by an intravenous injection of pentobarbital-sodium (Narcoren, 16 g/100 mL, Boehringer Ingelheim Vetmedica GmbH, Ingelheim am Rhein, Germany; ED9: 0.05 mL, ED12 to ED15: 0.1 mL and ED16 to ED18: 0.2 mL), followed by decapitation. Afterward, the sexes of the ED12 to ED18 embryos were identified macroscopically by the assessment of the gonads. For the ED9 embryos, sexing was performed with PCR of genomic DNA samples isolated from pectoral and wing muscle. Screening was performed according to an established protocol [25] using primers targeting the Z chromosome [5’ AAGCATAGAAACAATGTGGGAC 3’ (forward) and 5’ AACTCTGTCTGGAAGGACTT 3’ (reverse)] and female-specific primers targeting the W chromosome [5’ CTATGCCTACCACMTTCCTATTTGC 3’ (forward) and 5’ AACTCTGTCTGGAAGGACTT 3’ (reverse)]. The expected lengths of the DNA fragments were 250 bp and 375 bp, respectively, for female embryos and 250 bp for male embryos. An overview of the sex ratio in each ED is shown in Table 1.

2.2. Preparation Process

All experiments were performed between 9:00 am and 7:30 pm by the same two persons to standardize the procedure. To keep the environmental conditions as similar as possible to typical brooding conditions, experiments were conducted in a special heated chamber. The chamber was equipped with a heat mat (ThermoLux Wärmeunterlage, Witte + Sutor GmbH, Murrhardt, Germany), a heat lamp (Wärmestrahlgerät, Taschenlampenwerk ARTAS GmbH, Arnstadt, Germany) and an air humidifier (Series 2000 Luftbefeuchter HU4811/10R1, Philips, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). Humidity was kept at a constant level of 55.5% ± 4.5. Additionally, the eggs were embedded in warm (38.0 °C) Armor Beads (Lab Armor BeadsTM, Sheldon Manufacturing, Cornelius, NC, USA). In this manner, the inner egg temperature was kept at 37.9 °C ± 0.9 during the entire experiment. To observe the entire embryo, the window in the eggshell was enlarged. Next, the chorioallantoic membrane (CAM) was carefully cut open and removed from the field of view. If necessary, blood vessels were ligated to prevent bleeding. However, to the extent possible, ligating or cutting vessels was avoided to prevent disruption of blood circulation. To gain access to the embryo and improve visibility, the amnion was carefully opened. A Desmarres lid retractor (Fuhrmann GmbH, Much, Germany) was carefully placed underneath the beak of the embryo to ensure beak visibility. In the case of ED9 embryos, a small wire loop was used.

2.3. Experimental Setup

All experiments were filmed with a camera (Panasonic LUMIX DC-G110V with a Panasonic Lumix G 30 m lens, Matsushita Electric Industrial Co., Ltd., Osaka, Japan; for ED9 to ED16: HOYA SUPER PRO1 Revo Filter SMC Cir-PL, Kenko Tokina Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) with a frame rate of 50 frames per second.
After preparation, a resting period of three minutes was allotted. Baseline behavior was recorded for two (ED15 to ED18) or three (ED9 to ED14) minutes; subsequently, two stimuli were applied in a randomized order. The stimuli used were a noxious mechanical stimulus (Pinch) using a manual instrument and a light touch (Touch) as a negative control. Both were applied at the base of the beak. For the ED15 to ED18 embryos, a mosquito clamp (Fine Science Tools, Foster City, CA, USA) was used to administer the stimulus. To better monitor the applied force, a mosquito clamp combined with an analgesia meter (Rodent Pincher Analgesia Meter, Bioseb, Vitrolles, France) was used for experiments conducted with ED12 to ED14 embryos. Stimulus 1 (Pinch or Touch) was administered, followed by an observation duration of three minutes. After a second baseline period, stimulus 2 (Touch or Pinch) was administered, followed by another three minutes of observation. Because of their small size, microsurgical anatomical forceps (Fine Science Tools, Foster City, USA) had to be used to administer the stimulus to ED9 embryos. An additional group of ED18 embryos (ED18 w/Lido) was injected with 0.02 mL of lidocaine (Xylocitin® 2%, Mibe GmbH Arzneimittel, Brehna, Germany) in the upper and lower beak region five minutes before the first baseline. Experiments were then performed according to the above protocol.

2.4. Analyses: Hardware, Software and Statistical Analyses

All videos were edited in the same way using the “daVinci Resolve” software (Blackmagic Design Pty. Ltd., Port Melbourne, Australia) before analysis. For each embryo, four single videos were cut referring to the sections of the experimental design: Baseline Pinch, Baseline Touch, Post Pinch and Post Touch. An overview of the experimental procedure is shown in Figure 1.

2.5. DeepLabCut

To track the body parts of the embryo, the Python-based markerless pose estimation software DLC (version 2.2.1.1) [19,21] was used on a computer (MSI MAG Infinite 11TC-1222AT, Intel Core i7–11700F, 16 GB RAM, nVidia GeForce RTX3060, Micro-Star Int’l Co., Ltd., New Taipei City, Taiwan). The neural network was trained for each ED individually with video footage according to the protocol provided by the developers [21]. Manual labeling was always performed by the same person. The training was performed with the default settings and using a ResNet-50-based neural network [26,27]. A test error below 8.5 was obtained for every ED. After the model training was completed, the four experimental videos (Baseline Pinch, Baseline Touch, Post Pinch and Post Touch) were analyzed for each embryo. For each labeled body part, DLC created three outputs for each frame of the video: an x coordinate, a y coordinate and a likelihood value. These values were analyzed with custom-written code using MATLAB (MATLAB Version: 9.12.0.1927505 (R2022a) Update 1, MathWorks). In all cases, a likelihood value cutoff of 0.75 was used.

2.5.1. Visualization of the Data Clusters

In the analysis, the focus was on the following body parts:
  • Beak;
  • Head;
  • Limbs;
  • Stationary points on the egg, the Desmarres lid retractor, and the wire loop (for ED9) were used as reference controls.
As a first step, the labeled data clusters for each analyzed body part were visualized in the x–y coordinate space. This enabled refinement of the dataset through identification of outliers or mislabeled body parts. The videos were then checked for errors, and if any real outlier was found in a frame, its value was manually excluded.

Distance between the Upper and Lower Beak

The distance between the upper and lower boundaries of the beak was calculated in terms of the Euclidian distance d between two points:
d = x u x l 2 + y u y l 2
where x u is the x coordinate of the upper beak label, x l is the x coordinate of the lower beak label, y u is the y coordinate of the upper beak label and y l is the y coordinate of the lower beak label. The Euclidian distance was calculated (in pixels) for every frame of the video.

Angle between the Upper and Lower Beak

The angle between the upper and lower beak was computed by calculating the angle α between two lines, P 0 to P 1 and P 0 to P 2 , where P 0 is the fulcrum between the beak parts, P 1 is the upper beak point and P 2 is the lower beak point. The angle was then calculated as follows:
α = a t a n 2 n o r m d e t n 2 ; n 1 , d o t n 2 , n 1
where a t a n 2 is the four-quadrant inverse tangent, d e t is the matrix determinant, d o t is the dot product, and n 2 , n 1 are the Euclidean normalized vectors for P 0 to P 1 and P 0 to P 2 , respectively. The angle between the upper and lower beak was calculated for all frames of the video in radians and then converted to degrees.

Movement

The movement of the body parts of interest was calculated in terms of the Euclidean distance d between identical labels across consecutive frames:
d = x f 1 x f 2 2 + y f 1 y f 2 2
where x f 1 is the x coordinate in frame 1, x f 2 is the x coordinate in frame 2, y f 1 is the y coordinate in frame 1 and y f 2 is the y coordinate in frame 2. The distances were calculated for all consecutive frames. From ED12 to ED18, movements of the medial eye corner, elbow and metatarsus were analyzed. For the body movements on ED9, the tarsus (instead of the metatarsus) was used to assess leg movement, as the tissue of the metatarsus was translucent and prone to errors in tracking.

2.5.2. Analysis

To simplify the analyses, 30 s intervals were evaluated. For each parameter, i.e., Beak Distance, Beak Angle, Movement Eye Corner, Movement Elbow and Movement Metatarsus, the sum of the 1500 frame values of the interval was calculated. In Post Stimulus, this resulted in four intervals: 0–30, 30–60, 60–90 and 90–120 s. The beginning of the first poststimulus interval was defined as the moment from which the clamp was no longer in contact with the beak. The median of the four 30-s intervals prior to the stimulus was considered the baseline. Missing values, which arose after the exclusion of low likelihood values, were manually imputed. For each missing value series, the median was determined for half of the adjacent data and used in place of the missing value. If more than 5% of the data in an interval were missing, the interval was excluded from the analysis. Due to a lack of visibility, one ED14 embryo and one ED18 embryo were completely excluded from the DLC analysis. A precise overview of the number of datasets ultimately included in the analysis is provided in Table S1.
Due to the presence of repeated measures, generalized linear mixed effects models with the individual embryo as a random effect were chosen for analysis. Due to the violation of numerous model assumptions (normality of residual distribution, heteroscedasticity of residuals, heterogeneity of variances between groups and presence of outliers), only robust linear mixed-effects models were applied for all analyses (R package-robustlmm). All contrasts (differences) between particular groups were assessed after model-fitting by the estimated marginal means (R package–emmeans) with Tukey’s p value correction for multiple comparisons. The results with a p value < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Data analysis was performed using R 4.2.1 (23 June 2022). Detailed information about the statistical analyses, including the corresponding effect size for each reported p value, can be found in Table S2.

2.6. Manual Observation

The same video footage as used in the DLC analyses was used for manual observations. Since preliminary observations and data from the DLC analyses indicated that changes in beak position were frequent after Pinch, manual observations focused on beak movements. Four different patterns of beak movements were identified from the video material:
  • Beak Shift—a small horizontal shift of the upper and lower beaks against each other;
  • Mandibulation—a small vertical opening of the beak, often executed several times, and reminiscent of a chewing movement;
  • Beak Opening—single, swift, vertical opening of the beak;
  • Wide Beak Opening—single, wide, vertical opening of the beak, accompanied by a characteristic tongue movement.
In an analogous approach to the one described above, the baseline and poststimulus observations were divided into intervals of 30 s. For manual observations, the 30 s before the stimulus were used as a baseline. For each interval, the occurrences of the described beak movements were counted.

3. Results

3.1. Beak Movements in Response to a Noxious Stimulus

To analyze the movements of chicken embryos, the markerless pose estimation software DLC was used. The angle (Beak Angle) and distance (Beak Distance) between the upper and lower beak were calculated to reflect the opening of the beak as a potential response to a noxious mechanical stimulus applied at the base of the beak. The mechanical stimulation of the beak led to a change in the beak position at embryonic day (ED) 9 and ED12; thus, evaluation with DLC was distorted and could not be interpreted. At ED13 and ED14, Beak Distance did not differ between any time intervals during the two minutes after the control touch stimulus (hereafter, Post Touch) and the time intervals during the two minutes after the noxious pinch stimulus (hereafter, Post Pinch) (Figure S1). At ED15, significant increases in Beak Distance as a response to Pinch were detected (Figure 2). Additionally, in ED15 embryos, beak movements Post Pinch increased significantly over the first 120 s compared to Baseline Pinch and over the first 90 s compared to Post Touch. On ED16, ED17 and ED18, a significant increase in Beak Distance was observed over all time intervals Post Pinch compared to Baseline Pinch and Post Touch. The greatest increase in Beak Distance occurred during the first 30 s of Post Pinch. The group of ED18 embryos that received an injection of the local anesthetic lidocaine (ED18 w/Lido) did not exhibit reduced beak movements compared to same-age embryos that did not receive the local anesthetic (Figure S2). Beak Distance was still significantly increased in ED18 w/Lido in the first 30 s of Post Pinch (p < 0.0001).
Beak Angle results are displayed in the Supplementary Information (Figure S3). Briefly, Beak Angle showed a similar pattern of changes as Beak Distance. Additionally, significant increases in Beak Angle during Post Pinch were observed from ED15 onward.

3.2. Head Movements in Response to a Noxious Stimulus

The medial eye corner was tracked to analyze the head movements of chicken embryos. Changes were particularly observed on ED13 and ED16 to ED18 in the first 30 s of Post Pinch. On these days, the embryos showed a significant increase in head movements after Pinch compared to after Touch (ED13: p = 0.0254; ED16: p = 0.0381; ED17: p = 0.026; ED18: p < 0.0001) and during Baseline Pinch (ED13: p = 0.0256; ED16: p = 0.0001; ED17: p < 0.0001; ED18: p < 0.0001). At ED12, head movements increased significantly at 30–60 s after Pinch compared to those 30–60 s after Touch (p = 0.0372). At ED14, head movements also increased significantly in the first 30 s after the stimulus compared to those in the corresponding baseline period. These movements were observed after both stimuli (Pinch: p = 0.0153; Touch: p = 0.0069). In addition, a significant difference between head movements in response to Pinch and those in response to Touch was observed at 30–60 s after the stimulus (p = 0.0175). Head movements were significantly reduced in ED18 w/Lido embryos in the first 30 s of Post Pinch compared to those of ED18 embryos in the same period (p < 0.0001). Head movements on ED15 to ED18 are displayed in Figure 3, while data on ED9, ED12 to ED14 and ED18 w/Lido embryos are provided in the Supplementary Information (Figures S4 and S5).

3.3. Limb Movements in Response to a Noxious Stimulus

To track limb movements, the movements of the Elbow, Metatarsus and Tarsus (ED9) were analyzed. Significant differences in limb movements between Baseline Pinch and Post Pinch and between Post Pinch and Post Touch were observed only on ED18 (Figures S6 and S7). An increase in elbow movements was observed between Baseline Pinch and Post Pinch (p = 0.0023) as well as between Post Pinch and Post Touch (p = 0.0096) during the first 30 s after the stimulus. Regarding metatarsus movements, ED18 embryos showed a significant increase between Baseline Pinch and Post Pinch (p < 0.0001) as well as between Post Pinch and Post Touch (p = 0.0002) during the first 30 s after the stimulus. For ED18 w/Lido embryos, no significant differences in limb movements were observed between Baseline and the first 30 s of Post Stimulus. There was also no significant difference between the ED18 embryos and the ED18 w/Lido embryos. Other significant changes in limb movements were observed at specific time intervals during development.

3.4. Characterization of Beak Movements in Response to a Noxious Stimulus

In particular, DLC analysis identified changes in beak movement during Post Pinch in embryos from ED15 to ED18. To characterize beak movements in further detail, manual observations were performed. The focus of the manual observations was on four behaviors: Beak Shift, Mandibulation, Beak Opening and Wide Beak Opening. An overview of the percentage of animals that exhibited each behavior at specific time intervals is shown in Table 2. In addition, the counts of each behavior are shown in Figures S8–S11.
Beak Opening was rarely displayed during Baseline and was observed in only 10.0% of animals from ED9 to ED18. Beak Opening was particularly rare on ED9 and ED12 to ED14. Before ED12, a maximum of 10.0% of animals exhibited this behavior within a single time interval; up to ED14, a maximum of 20.0% of animals exhibited this behavior within a single time interval. Starting from ED15, an increasing frequency (31.3%) of Beak Opening was observed after the application of the noxious stimulus. At ED16, 87.5% of embryos showed Beak Opening in the first 30 s of Post Pinch. Additionally, 50.0% of ED17 embryos and 62.5% of ED18 embryos showed this behavioral response to Pinch. During these days, at least twice as many embryos showed Beak Opening during Post Pinch as those during Post Touch.
Wide Beak Opening, characterized by visible tongue movement, was observed only sporadically during Baseline on all developmental days. This behavior was observed in only one animal each on ED13, ED14 and in ED18 w/Lido embryos during baseline. Moreover, this specific beak movement was not observed during Post Pinch and Post Touch for ED9 to ED13 embryos and was observed only once during Post Pinch on ED14. On ED15 and ED16, this behavior was increasingly observed. A total of 18.8% (ED15) and 25.0% (ED16) of embryos exhibited Wide Beak Opening in the first 30 s of Post Pinch. A total of 81.3% and 87.5% of embryos on ED17 and ED18, respectively, showed more Wide Beak Opening in the first 30 s of Post Pinch. However, this behavior was never observed during Post Touch or corresponding baseline periods at these ages.
Beak Shift was observed from ED12 onward, but it did not appear to be associated with Pinch. Mandibulation was also observed across all embryonic days. Changes were observed in Mandibulation at all times in Post Pinch and Post Touch and regularly during both baseline periods.
Since Beak Opening and Wide Beak Opening were the most noticeable Post Pinch responses, the focus of comparisons with the additional control group that received local anesthetic (ED18 w/Lido) was on these two movements, as the application of lidocaine reduced these behaviors. In the ED18 w/Lido group, 40.0% of the embryos reacted with Wide Beak Opening to the noxious mechanical stimulus; in the ED18 embryos without a lidocaine injection, 87.5% exhibited this behavior. Beak Opening was observed in 20.0% of the ED18 w/Lido animals and 62.5% of the untreated ED18 embryos. Neither Mandibulation nor Beak Shift appeared to be associated with a specific reaction in any time interval, similar to embryos without lidocaine treatment. In other words, no noticeable increase or decrease in these behaviors was observed after a stimulus.

4. Discussion

In this exploratory study, we investigated the movements of chicken embryos in response to a noxious stimulus at different developmental stages. We used DeepLabCut, a Python-based markerless pose estimation software, as well as manual observations to determine their responses.
Recently, the use of artificial intelligence and deep learning systems in behavioral studies has increased, and the availability of free software such as DLC allows such techniques to be used by researchers with less sophisticated programming experience [28,29,30]. In our study, we trained a model to provide satisfactory accuracy of tracking individual body parts on each embryonic day. One of the major advantages of using the markerless pose estimation software DLC is that it enables unbiased analysis. Calculations of distances are not based on subjective perception by an observer and are therefore quantifiable and reliable. Therefore, deep learning systems in general and DLC in particular offer a means of detecting and classifying behaviors that may not be detectible to the naked eye. However, the DLC analysis did not allow us to distinguish between types of beak movements. Thus, for better differentiation of beak movements, we added manual observation of these movements and identified four different patterns.
Pain behavior in general is influenced by a variety of factors specific to the stimulus or the affected animal. For example, noxious agents can differ in duration (acute or chronic), source (somatic or visceral) and severity (mild to severe), each of which may provoke a different reaction [9,10,31]. Since behavioral responses vary extensively depending on the species and stimulus, any description is valid only for the specifically described case and cannot be transferred to another species without re-evaluation [10]. In our study, we applied an acute mechanical stimulus to the beak base of chicken embryos. The beak of chickens is known to be equipped with nociceptors [32] and therefore represents a pain-sensitive area [11]. The beak has also been reported as the region in chicken embryos where the earliest response to stimuli is observed [33]. Chumak observed reflex movements in the form of flexions of the head on day 7 of incubation in response to pinpricks in the beak region, describing reflexes provoked by external stimuli (isolated movements of the head or wing) and spontaneous voluntary movements (involving generalized head, trunk and limb movements) [33].
Nociceptive reflexes have evolved as protective mechanisms [34]. A noxious stimulus is transmitted via peripheral nociceptors to the spinal cord and transmitted to motor neurons, resulting in muscle contraction and thus the nociceptive reflex [34,35,36]. Chumak reported more specific responses, including increased defensive movements, in chicken embryos at ED14/15 but characterized these responses as reflexive [33]. Hamburger and Oppenheim reported that coordinated movements appear around ED17 [14]. Since our study was based solely on observations of movements by chicken embryos, a conclusion regarding whether the observed movements are reflexes or coordinated movements cannot be drawn.
We analyzed the movements of chicken embryos in response to a noxious stimulus applied to the beak from ED9 to ED18. Consistent with the assumption that a response to a stimulus is expected at the site of stimulus application, as was shown for well-innervated regions such as the beak [10], our DLC data for Beak Angle and Beak Distance showed the most noticeable changes after the stimulus. Both parameters, Beak Angle and Beak Distance, quantified beak movements. A significant increase in beak movements was detected immediately after Pinch from ED15 to ED18. As the increase in beak movements during Post Pinch was significant compared to those during Baseline Pinch and Post Touch, we assumed that the increase in beak movements was a reaction to the noxious stimulus and was not a random movement of the chicken embryos.
Further differentiation of the movements through manual observation revealed that Beak Opening (starting on ED16) and Wide Beak Opening (starting on ED17) were recurring movements in response to the noxious stimulus. Individual, slow beak openings have been described in connection with the penetration of the air sac membrane shortly before hatching, at the end of day 18 [14]. This description, however, does not match the rapid and clustered movements that we observed following the stimulus. Since these beak openings do not appear to be part of the typical behavior of chick embryos and markedly occurred only after a noxious stimulus, they may represent a nocifensive response by the embryo. Whether this can be interpreted as the presence of pain sensation remains unclear because an experience of pain presupposes consciousness [37], and no indications can be made about this in the context of this part of the study.
Hamburger and Oppenheim also described a behavior that they called beak clapping, which involves rapid opening and closing of the beak in sequences that occurred at irregular intervals [14]. The description and random occurrence of this behavior matches Mandibulation in our study. Likewise, the movement was randomly observed across time intervals and had no clear connection to any of the stimuli. However, a similar behavior was observed in adult chickens as a response to low atmospheric pressure stunning before slaughter [38]. In this case, the mandibulation was discussed as a possible sign of reduced welfare or a physiological reaction to hypoxia [38]. As in the other studies, the embryos in our study underwent stress from the opening of the egg, preparation and stimuli. Therefore, it is possible that Mandibulation is also a sign of stress in chicken embryos.
Application of the local anesthetic lidocaine did not yield a significant reduction in the beak movements of chicken embryos on ED18 according to the DLC analysis. However, in the manual observations, the application of lidocaine reduced the percentage of embryos that responded to stimuli with Wide Beak Opening and Beak Opening by approximately half. Furthermore, local anesthetics are known to be effective in birds [39,40,41] and can be used in chickens, e.g., for spinal anesthesia [42] or brachial plexus blockade [43]. However, there are no reliable empirical data regarding the mode of action of local anesthetics in chicken embryos. Additionally, we emphasize that only a small number of embryos were examined; thus, the results must be interpreted with caution. The inability of local anesthesia to reduce beak movements could also stem from the injection of lidocaine, which itself constitutes a noxious stimulus. In addition, numbness in the beak due to local anesthesia could have led to behavioral changes [44]. This is supported by the fact that head movements were significantly reduced by applying lidocaine to ED18 embryos.
Overall, stress could not be completely eliminated within the experimental setup; thus, its potential influence on behavior must be considered. The fenestrated egg does not represent a completely typical environment for the embryo because of the increased exposure to environmental influences, such as light. Additionally, the invasiveness of the preparation itself can induce stress, which is known to alter the behavior of birds [9]. We attempted to reduce external influences by standardizing the temperature and humidity during the experiments and adjusting them to match the typical incubation conditions as closely as possible. However, since direct access to the embryo was necessary for stimulation and the embryo had to be visible to assess responses, some stressors were unavoidable.
We were also interested in whether limb movements changed after the noxious stimulus; however, we did not detect any overarching pattern until ED17. Occasional significant differences in limb movements during Post Pinch compared to those during Baseline Pinch or Post Touch were inconsistent over several EDs or time intervals and are therefore likely due to random movements, which have been described previously in the literature [12,13,14,45,46,47,48,49]. Hamburger and Oppenheim stated that before ED15, the observed leg motility was not connected to any sensory input but appeared randomly due to autonomous cell discharges [15]. Wu et al. counted unilateral and bilateral simultaneous limb movements and found a maximum of movements between ED10 and ED13 for the former and two maxima on ED13 and ED17 for the latter [50]. In the present study, we detected a significant increase in elbow and metatarsal movements during the first 30 s of Post Pinch compared to those during the first 30 s of Baseline Pinch and Post Touch on only ED18, suggesting that these movements may represent an actual response to the noxious stimulus.

5. Conclusions

We observed the movements of chicken embryos from ED9 to ED18 before and after noxious stimulation. During Post Pinch, the observed movement changes in ED15 to ED18 embryos were most likely a response to the noxious mechanical stimulus and can therefore be interpreted as nocifensive behavior. The results of our current movement analysis in combination with the corresponding results of the cardiovascular changes [22] and the evaluation of the onset of physiological neuronal signals [23] in chicken embryos during this developmental period provide valuable information that enhances our understanding of the development of nociception and pain perception in chickens.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13182859/s1, Figure S1: Beak Distance ED13/ED14; Figure S2: Beak Distance Lidocaine; Figure S3: Beak Angle; Figure S4: Movement Eye corner ED9/ED12-14; Figure S5: Movement Eye corner Lidocaine; Figure S6: Movement Metatarsus respectively Tarsus (ED9); Figure S7: Movement Elbow; Figure S8: Beak Shift; Figure S9: Mandibulation; Figure S10: Beak Opening; Figure S11: Wide Beak Opening; Table S1: Overview on the number of datasets from DLC included in the final analysis; Table S2: Statistical metrics. All MATLAB analysis code used in this study is available in a public GitHub repository: https://github.com/ondracej/dlcAnalysisEmbryo (accessed on 6 September 2023).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.B., J.W., A.M.S., J.R., T.F., B.S.; methodology, C.B., J.W., A.M.S., J.R., S.C.S., L.W., S.K., M.A., T.F., B.S., H.P.; software, J.M.O.; validation, C.B., J.W., A.M.S.; formal analysis, J.M.O., Y.Z.; investigation, S.C.S., L.W., A.M.S., J.W., J.R.; resources, C.B., B.S.; data curation, S.C.S., J.W., A.M.S., J.M.O.; writing—original draft preparation, S.C.S.; writing—review and editing, S.C.S., J.W., A.M.S., J.R., J.M.O., S.K., M.A., T.F., B.S., C.B.; visualization, S.C.S., J.W., A.M.S.; supervision, C.B., T.F., B.S., H.P.; project administration, C.B.; funding acquisition, C.B., T.F., B.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the German Federal Ministry of Food and Agriculture (BMEL, https://www.bmel.de, accessed on 6 September 2023) based on a decision of the Parliament of the Federal Republic of Germany, granted by the Federal Office for Agriculture and Food (BLE, https://www.ble.de, accessed on 6 September 2023, grant number: 2821HS005).

Institutional Review Board Statement

According to Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and the German Animal Welfare Law, no ethical approval was required for the use of chicken embryos in the experiments. The experiments were performed in an AAALAC-certified animal facility. All experiments and the euthanasia of embryos were conducted in strict accordance with the institutional Guidelines for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and under general animal welfare principles.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the scientific advisory board including Michael Erhard, Wolf Erhardt, Harald Luksch, Heidrun Potschka, Hans Straka and Britta Wirrer. In addition, the authors thank Marie-Louise Schmid, Johannes Fischer and Hicham Sid for their helpful support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Livingston, A. Pain and analgesia in domestic animals. In Comparative and Veterinary Pharmacology, 1st ed.; Cunningham, F., Elliott, J., Lees, P., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 159–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gentle, M.J. Pain in birds. Anim. Welf. 1992, 1, 235–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Paul-Murphy, J.R.; Hawkins, M. Bird-specific considerations: Recognizing pain behavior in pet birds. In Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management, 3rd ed.; Gaynor, J.S., Muir, W.W., Eds.; Elsevier Mosby: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2014; pp. 536–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Douglas, J.M.; Guzman, D.S.-M.; Paul-Murphy, J.R. Pain in birds: The anatomical and physiological basis. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2018, 21, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Raja, S.N.; Carr, D.B.; Cohen, M.; Finnerup, N.B.; Flor, H.; Gibson, S.; Keefe, F.J.; Mogil, J.S.; Ringkamp, M.; Sluka, K.A.; et al. The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: Concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain 2020, 161, 1976–1982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Julius, D.; Basbaum, A.I. Molecular mechanisms of nociception. Nature 2001, 413, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Zimmermann, M. Ethical considerations in relation to pain in animal experimentation. Acta Physiol. Scandinavica. Suppl. 1986, 554, 221–233. [Google Scholar]
  8. Korbel, R.; Lierz, M. Vögel. In Anästhesie und Analgesie beim Klein- und Heimtier mit Exoten, Labortieren, Vögeln, Reptilien, Amphibien und Fischen, 2nd ed.; Erhardt, W., Baumgartner, C., Haberstroh, J., Eds.; Schattauer GmbH: Stuttgart, Germany, 2012; pp. 790–834. [Google Scholar]
  9. Mikoni, N.A.; Guzman, D.S.-M.; Paul-Murphy, J. Pain recognition and assessment in birds. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2023, 26, 65–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Mikoni, N.A.; Guzman, D.S.-M.; Fausak, E.; Paul-Murphy, J. Recognition and assessment of pain-related behaviors in avian species: An integrative review. J. Avian Med. Surg. 2022, 36, 153–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gentle, M.J. Pain issues in poultry. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 135, 252–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hamburger, V.; Balaban, M. Observations and experiments on spontaneous rhythmical behavior in the chick embryo. Dev. Biol. 1963, 6, 533–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hamburger, V.; Balaban, M.; Oppenheim, R.; Wenger, E. Periodic motility of normal and spinal chick embryos between 8 and 17 days of incubation. J. Exp. Zool. 1965, 159, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hamburger, V.; Oppenheim, R. Prehatching motility and hatching behavior in the chick. J. Exp. Zool. 1967, 166, 171–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hamburger, V.; Wenger, E.; Oppenheim, R. Motility in the chick embryo in the absence of sensory input. J. Exp. Zool. 1966, 162, 133–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Peters, J.J.; Vonderahe, A.R.; Powers, T.H. The functional chronology in developing chick nervous system. J. Exp. Zool. 1956, 133, 505–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Eide, A.L.; Glover, J.C. Developmental dynamics of functionally specific primary sensory afferent projections in the chicken embryo. Anat. Embryol. 1997, 195, 237–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Eide, A.L.; Glover, J.C. Development of the longitudinal projection patterns of lumbar primary sensory afferents in the chicken embryo. J. Comp. Neurol. 1995, 353, 247–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Mathis, A.; Mamidanna, P.; Cury, K.M.; Abe, T.; Murthy, V.N.; Mathis, M.W.; Bethge, M. DeepLabCut: Markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2018, 21, 1281–1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Mathis, A.; Warren, R. On the inference speed and video-compression robustness of DeepLabCut. bioRxiv 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nath, T.; Mathis, A.; Chen, A.C.; Patel, A.; Bethge, M.; Mathis, M.W. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nat. Protoc. 2019, 14, 2152–2176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Weiss, L.; Saller, A.M.; Werner, J.; Süß, S.C.; Reiser, J.; Kollmansperger, S.; Anders, M.; Potschka, H.; Fenzl, T.; Schusser, B.; et al. Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part I: Analysis of Cardiovascular Responses to a Mechanical Noxious Stimulus. Animals 2023, 13, 2710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kollmansperger, S.; Anders, M.; Werner, J.; Saller, A.M.; Weiss, L.; Süß, S.C.; Reiser, J.; Schneider, G.; Schusser, B.; Baumgartner, C.; et al. Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part II: Embryonal Development of Electroencephalic Neuronal Activity In Ovo as a Prerequisite for Nociception. Animals 2023, 13, 2839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Spurlin, J., III; Lwigale, P. A technique to increase accessibility to late-stage chick embryos for in ovo manipulations. Dev. Dyn. 2013, 242, 148–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Itoh, Y.; Suzuki, M.; Ogawa, A.; Munechika, I.; Murata, K.; Mizuno, S. Identification of the sex of a wide range of carinatae birds by PCR using primer sets selected from chicken EE0.6 and its related sequences. J. Hered. 2001, 92, 315–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Insafutdinov, E.; Pishchulin, L.; Andres, B.; Andriluka, M.; Schiele, B. DeeperCut: A deeper, stronger, and faster multi-person pose estimation model. In Proceedings of the ECCV (European Conference on Computer Vision), Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 11–14 October 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. He, K.; Zhang, X.; Ren, S.; Sun, J. Deep residual learning for image recognition. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, Las Vegas, NV, USA, 27–30 June 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mathis, M.W.; Mathis, A. Deep learning tools for the measurement of animal behavior in neuroscience. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2020, 60, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. von Ziegler, L.; Sturman, O.; Bohacek, J. Big behavior: Challenges and opportunities in a new era of deep behavior profiling. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021, 46, 33–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Hardin, A.; Schlupp, I. Using machine learning and DeepLabCut in animal behavior. Acta Ethol. 2022, 25, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Henke, J.; Tacke, S.; Erhardt, W. Analgesie. In Anästhesie und Analgesie beim Klein- und Heimtier mit Exoten, Labortieren, Vögeln, Reptilien, Amphibien und Fischen, 2nd ed.; Erhardt, W., Baumgartner, C., Haberstroh, J., Eds.; Schattauer GmbH: Stuttgart, Germany, 2012; pp. 383–431. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kuenzel, W.J. Neurobiological basis of sensory perception: Welfare implications of beak trimming. Poult. Sci. 2007, 86, 1273–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chumak, V.I. Dinamika reflektomykh reaktsii i vklyuchenie retseptornykh apparatov u embriona kuritsy (Dynamics of reflex reactions and initiation of receptor systems in the chick embryo). In Voprosy Fiziologii i Patologii Tsentral’noi Nervnoi Sistemy Cheloveka i Zhivotnykli v Ontogeneze; Sbornik: Moscow, Russia, 1961; pp. 63–68. [Google Scholar]
  34. Antognini, J.F.; Carstens, E. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia and its components. Br. J. Anaesth. 2002, 89, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Diener, M.; Gerstberger, R. Zentrales Nervensystem. In Physiologie der Haustiere, 5th ed.; von Engelhardt, W., Breves, G., Diener, M., Gäbel, G., Eds.; Enke: Stuttgart, Germany, 2015; pp. 131–145. [Google Scholar]
  36. Willis, W.D.; Westlund, K.N. Neuroanatomy of the pain system and of the pathways that modulate pain. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1997, 14, 2–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Diesch, T.J.; Mellor, D.J.; Johnson, C.B.; Lentle, R.G. Responsiveness to painful stimuli in anaesthetised newborn and young animals of varying neurological maturity (wallaby joeys, rat pups and lambs). In Proceedings of the AATEX (World Congress on Alternatives & Animal Use in the Life Sciences), Tokyo, Japan, 21–25 August 2007. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mackie, N.; McKeegan, D.E.F. Behavioural responses of broiler chickens during low atmospheric pressure stunning. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 174, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Glatz, P.C.; Murphy, L.B.; Preston, A.P. Analgesic therapy of beak-trimmed chickens. Aust. Vet. J. 1992, 69, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hocking, P.M.; Gentle, M.J.; Bernard, R.; Dunn, L.N. Evaluation of a protocol for determining the effectiveness of pretreatment with local analgesics for reducing experimentally induced articular pain in domestic fowl. Res. Vet. Sci. 1997, 63, 263–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Paul-Murphy, J.; Ludders, J.W. Avian analgesia. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2001, 4, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Khamisabadi, A.; Kazemi-Darabadi, S.; Akbari, G. Comparison of anesthetic efficacy of lidocaine and bupivacaine in spinal anesthesia in chickens. J. Avian Med. Surg. 2021, 35, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Figueiredo, J.P.; Cruz, M.L.; Mendes, G.M.; Marucio, R.L.; Riccó, C.H.; Campagnol, D. Assessment of brachial plexus blockade in chickens by an axillary approach. Vet. Anaesth. Analg. 2008, 35, 511–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Binshtok, A.M.; Bean, B.P.; Woolf, C.J. Inhibition of nociceptors by TRPV1-mediated entry of impermeant sodium channel blockers. Nature 2007, 449, 607–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Provine, R.R.; Sharma, S.; Sandel, T.; Hamburger, V. Electrical activity in the spinal cord of the chick embryo, in situ. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1970, 65, 508–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Provine, R.R. Development of between-limb movement synchronization in the chick embryo. Dev. Psychobiol. 1980, 13, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Sharma, S.; Provine, R.R.; Hamburger, V.; Sandel, T. Unit activity in the isolated spinal cord of chick embryo, in situ. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1970, 66, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Bekoff, A. Ontogeny of leg motor output in the chick embryo: A neural analysis. Brain Res. 1976, 106, 271–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bradley, N.S.; Solanki, D.; Zhao, D. Limb movements during embryonic development in the chick: Evidence for a continuum in limb motor control antecedent to locomotion. J. Neurophysiol. 2005, 94, 4401–4411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wu, K.-C.; Streicher, J.; Lee, M.; Hall, B.; Müller, G. Role of motility in embryonic development I: Embryo movements and amnion contractions in the chick and the influence of illumination. J. Exp. Zool. 2001, 291, 186–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the experimental procedures. (a) Recordings of the embryo were collected in ovo, and video data were transferred to a computer for editing. The body parts of chicken embryos tracked by DLC are labeled in the schema. (b) The neural network was trained, and the video material was analyzed according to the timeline. (c) The video material was manually analyzed according to the timeline. (Created with BioRender.com, accessed on 6 September 2023).
Figure 1. Flowchart of the experimental procedures. (a) Recordings of the embryo were collected in ovo, and video data were transferred to a computer for editing. The body parts of chicken embryos tracked by DLC are labeled in the schema. (b) The neural network was trained, and the video material was analyzed according to the timeline. (c) The video material was manually analyzed according to the timeline. (Created with BioRender.com, accessed on 6 September 2023).
Animals 13 02859 g001
Figure 2. Beak Distance. This variable was defined as the distance between the upper and lower beak of embryos. It was measured at (a) ED15 (n = 16), (b) ED16 (n = 16), (c) ED17 (n = 16) and (d) ED18 (n = 15) before and after application of a control (Touch) or noxious stimulus (Pinch). The total distance in pixels across 30 s intervals (1500 frames) was evaluated. Plots show the estimated mean ±95% confidence intervals at the following 30 s intervals from Baseline (BL) to post-stimulation, with stimulation occurring at 0 s: −30–0, 0–30, 30–60, 60–90 and 90–120 s. Robust linear mixed effects were applied for all analyses. All contrasts (differences) between particular groups were assessed after model-fitting by the estimated marginal means with Tukey’s p value correction for multiple comparisons. Touch: blue; Pinch: red. * Significant difference between Pinch and Touch; ♦ Significant difference from baseline. p values shown.
Figure 2. Beak Distance. This variable was defined as the distance between the upper and lower beak of embryos. It was measured at (a) ED15 (n = 16), (b) ED16 (n = 16), (c) ED17 (n = 16) and (d) ED18 (n = 15) before and after application of a control (Touch) or noxious stimulus (Pinch). The total distance in pixels across 30 s intervals (1500 frames) was evaluated. Plots show the estimated mean ±95% confidence intervals at the following 30 s intervals from Baseline (BL) to post-stimulation, with stimulation occurring at 0 s: −30–0, 0–30, 30–60, 60–90 and 90–120 s. Robust linear mixed effects were applied for all analyses. All contrasts (differences) between particular groups were assessed after model-fitting by the estimated marginal means with Tukey’s p value correction for multiple comparisons. Touch: blue; Pinch: red. * Significant difference between Pinch and Touch; ♦ Significant difference from baseline. p values shown.
Animals 13 02859 g002
Figure 3. Eye Corner Movement. This variable was used to detect head movements of embryos at (a) ED15 (n = 16), (b) ED16 (n = 16), (c) ED17 (n = 16) and (d) ED18 (n = 15) before and after application of two stimuli (Touch and Pinch). The total distance in pixels across 30 s intervals (1500 frames) was evaluated. Plots show the estimated mean ±95% confidence intervals at the following 30 s intervals from Baseline (BL) to post-stimulation, with stimulation occurring at 0 s: −30–0, 0–30, 30–60, 60–90 and 90–120 s. Robust linear mixed effects were applied for all analyses. All contrasts (differences) between particular groups were assessed after model-fitting by the estimated marginal means with Tukey’s p value correction for multiple comparisons. Touch: blue; Pinch: red. * Significant difference between Pinch and Touch; ♦ Significant difference from baseline. p values shown.
Figure 3. Eye Corner Movement. This variable was used to detect head movements of embryos at (a) ED15 (n = 16), (b) ED16 (n = 16), (c) ED17 (n = 16) and (d) ED18 (n = 15) before and after application of two stimuli (Touch and Pinch). The total distance in pixels across 30 s intervals (1500 frames) was evaluated. Plots show the estimated mean ±95% confidence intervals at the following 30 s intervals from Baseline (BL) to post-stimulation, with stimulation occurring at 0 s: −30–0, 0–30, 30–60, 60–90 and 90–120 s. Robust linear mixed effects were applied for all analyses. All contrasts (differences) between particular groups were assessed after model-fitting by the estimated marginal means with Tukey’s p value correction for multiple comparisons. Touch: blue; Pinch: red. * Significant difference between Pinch and Touch; ♦ Significant difference from baseline. p values shown.
Animals 13 02859 g003
Table 1. Number of chicken embryos. Overview of the number of chicken embryos analyzed on each embryonic day and the sex distribution.
Table 1. Number of chicken embryos. Overview of the number of chicken embryos analyzed on each embryonic day and the sex distribution.
ED9ED12ED13ED14ED15ED16ED17ED18ED18 w/
Lido
Amount of
embryos (n)
10101016161616165
Sex
male/female
5/53/75/59/77/97/87/97/92/3
Table 2. Percentage of chicken embryos showing beak movements. Overview of the percentage of chicken embryos that showed beak movements (Beak Shift, Mandibulation, Beak Opening or Wide Beak Opening) during the 30 s before (Baseline) and 30 s after (Post) the stimulus.
Table 2. Percentage of chicken embryos showing beak movements. Overview of the percentage of chicken embryos that showed beak movements (Beak Shift, Mandibulation, Beak Opening or Wide Beak Opening) during the 30 s before (Baseline) and 30 s after (Post) the stimulus.
ED9
n = 10
ED12
n = 10
ED13
n = 10
ED14
n = 16
ED15
n = 16
ED16
n = 16
ED17
n = 16
ED18
n = 16
ED18
w/Lido
n = 5
Amount of embryos [%]TouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinchTouchPinch
Beak ShiftBaseline0.00.00.00.010.030.018.825.031.30.018.825.025.031.325.06.340.040.0
Post0.00.030.030.020.020.018.831.331.325.018.818.825.018.831.36.320.060.0
MandibulationBaseline20.030.040.030.010.050.012.512.537.512.562.525.043.837.531.337.580.080.0
Post30.020.050.060.050.040.056.356.362.581.368.893.862.587.568.887.580.060.0
Beak
Opening
Baseline10.00.010.010.010.010.06.30.00.06.30.06.36.36.30.00.00.020.0
Post0.00.000.010.010.020.00.012.512.531.331.387.518.850.018.862.50.020.0
Wide Beak
Opening
Baseline0.00.00.00.010.00.00.06.30.00.00.00.00.00.00.00.020.00.0
Post0.00.00.00.00.00.00.06.312.518.86.325.00.081.30.087.50.040.0
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Süß, S.C.; Werner, J.; Saller, A.M.; Weiss, L.; Reiser, J.; Ondracek, J.M.; Zablotski, Y.; Kollmansperger, S.; Anders, M.; Potschka, H.; et al. Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part III: Analysis of Movements before and after Application of a Noxious Stimulus. Animals 2023, 13, 2859. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182859

AMA Style

Süß SC, Werner J, Saller AM, Weiss L, Reiser J, Ondracek JM, Zablotski Y, Kollmansperger S, Anders M, Potschka H, et al. Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part III: Analysis of Movements before and after Application of a Noxious Stimulus. Animals. 2023; 13(18):2859. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182859

Chicago/Turabian Style

Süß, Stephanie C., Julia Werner, Anna M. Saller, Larissa Weiss, Judith Reiser, Janie M. Ondracek, Yury Zablotski, Sandra Kollmansperger, Malte Anders, Heidrun Potschka, and et al. 2023. "Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part III: Analysis of Movements before and after Application of a Noxious Stimulus" Animals 13, no. 18: 2859. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182859

APA Style

Süß, S. C., Werner, J., Saller, A. M., Weiss, L., Reiser, J., Ondracek, J. M., Zablotski, Y., Kollmansperger, S., Anders, M., Potschka, H., Schusser, B., Fenzl, T., & Baumgartner, C. (2023). Nociception in Chicken Embryos, Part III: Analysis of Movements before and after Application of a Noxious Stimulus. Animals, 13(18), 2859. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182859

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop