Next Article in Journal
The Dynamics of Fire Activity in the Brazilian Pantanal: A Log-Gaussian Cox Process-Based Structural Decomposition
Next Article in Special Issue
CP2DIMG: An Innovative Research Program Aimed at Preparing Firefighters and Police Officers to Manage Emotions and Stress in Operational Contexts
Previous Article in Journal
Characteristics of Carbon Monoxide and Ethylene Generation in Mine’s Closed Fire Zone and Their Influence on Methane Explosion Limits
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Modelling Context Effects in Exit Choice for Building Evacuations

Department of Architecture and Civil Engineering, City University of Hong Kong, Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon, Hong Kong SAR, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Fire 2024, 7(5), 169; https://doi.org/10.3390/fire7050169
Submission received: 28 March 2024 / Revised: 30 April 2024 / Accepted: 15 May 2024 / Published: 17 May 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Fire Safety and Emergency Evacuation)

Abstract

:
Understanding exit choice behaviour is essential for optimising safety management strategies in building evacuations. Previous research focused on contextual attributes, such as spatial information, influencing exit choice, often using utility models based on monotonic functions of attributes. However, during emergencies, evacuees typically make rapid, less calculated decisions. The choice of context can significantly impact the evaluation of attributes, leading to preference reversals within the same choice set but under varying context conditions. This cognitive psychological phenomenon, known as context effects, encompasses the compromise effect, the similarity effect, and the attraction effect. While researchers have long recognised the pivotal role of context effects in human decision making, their incorporation into computer-aided evacuation management remains limited. To address this gap, we introduce context effects (CE) in a social force (SF) model, CE-SF. Evaluating CE-SF’s performance against the UF-SF model, which considers only the utility function (UF), we find that CE-SF better replicates exit choice behaviour across urgency levels, highlighting its potential to enhance evacuation strategies. Notably, our study identifies three distinct context effects during evacuations, emphasising their importance in advancing safety measures.

1. Introduction

Understanding how crowds can safely and efficiently evacuate in emergencies is crucial for public safety. The urban population is growing faster than ever before, and mass gatherings are becoming more regular [1], thus making the probability of overcrowding, crowd surges, crowd collapses, and stampedes much higher [2]. The South Korean Halloween night tragedy on 29 October 2022, the worst stampede disaster in the recent year, which caused 156 deaths and 170 injuries [2], highlights the significance of crowd management and evacuation optimisation strategies.
Optimisation via behavioural modification in route/exit choice is one of the important approaches to improve evacuation efficiency [3]. Various numerical and experimental studies have investigated how to optimise evacuation by modifying the route/exit choice strategies. For instance, Wang and Cao [4] used a revised social force model to investigate the efficacy of diverse evacuation strategies, including walking along walls and following the average moving direction or positional cues, across differing visibility levels, densities, and exit widths. Observations revealed the varying effectiveness of these strategies across different conditions; for instance, following the average movement direction or position proved more efficient under high densities, while wall walking exhibited greater efficacy under low densities. Similarly, Zhou et al. [5] utilised the social force model to compare five evacuation strategies incorporating distance, density, and capacity considerations. Optimal performance across these strategies diverged under different conditions, as evaluated from the perspectives of evacuation time, the channel utilisation rate, and evacuation efficiency. For instance, strategies integrating density and capacity factors excelled in minimising evacuation time, whereas distance-based strategies exhibited a superior evacuation efficiency. Additionally, Ma et al. [6] proposed a dynamic exit choice model to assess the evacuation efficiency of varied multiple exit layouts, establishing alterations in exit locations and the implementation of two parallel exits as the most efficient layout for optimising evacuation time. Furthermore, Feng et al. [7] conducted virtual reality experiments to evaluate the impact of additional evacuation information—namely exit signs, directional signage, and cues provided by fellow evacuees—on exit choice performance, noting significant influences and observing asymmetrical exit choices, particularly in interactions with other evacuees. Moreover, Zhang et al. [8] highlighted the substantial influence of crowd flow on human wayfinding decisions and performance, emphasising the importance of comprehending how individuals navigate route and exit choices in optimising evacuation strategies. Real-world observations by Helbing and Molnár [9] underscored the frequent oversight or inefficient utilisation of exits during emergency situations, further emphasising the criticality of understanding decision-making processes in route and exit selection to optimise evacuation strategies.
In the context of exit choice, decision makers engage in a multifaceted evaluation of various choice attributes, encompassing factors such as the distance to exits, fire conditions, and the presence of emergency illumination [10]. Consequently, their decisions hinge upon a convergence of factors, incorporating external considerations like the architectural configuration of the structure [11] and the information available to them during evacuation [7,12]. Additionally, internal factors, such as individual risk attitude [13] and demographic characteristics [14,15], play a pivotal role in shaping exit choices. Moreover, the dynamics of social interaction, as evidenced by leader–follower behaviour [16,17], introduce an additional stratum of complexity to this decision-making process.
Despite potential evacuee familiarity with a building’s layout, presuming exhaustive knowledge of factors influencing exit choice is unrealistic [18]. In reality, individuals often grapple with integrating information across multiple attributes to make strategic decisions accurately. This challenge in attribute weighting can lead to conflicting preferences among decision makers. Additionally, the seminal contributions of Daniel Kahneman, a renowned psychologist awarded the Nobel Prize in 2002 for his insights into the psychology of judgment and decision making, have shed light on the prevalence of heuristics and biases in human decision processes [19,20]. His research underscores the non-uniform rationality of human decisions. Furthermore, Klüpfel et al. [21] have highlighted the inherent difficulty evacuees face in making meticulously calculated strategic decisions during emergencies. Moreover, Gao et al. [13] have demonstrated that exit choice decision making is subject to rank-dependent and reference-dependent preferences, further elucidating the intricate nature of this process in evacuation contexts.
The selection of context holds significant sway over attribute evaluation, thereby profoundly influencing decision makers’ preferences [22]. Notably, the occurrence of preference reversals can arise within the same choice set under varying contextual conditions, including distinct framing or representations of identical choice sets [23]. Empirical investigations across a range of decision tasks consistently revealed the profound impact of context effects on the decision-making processes [24]. This phenomenon manifests across diverse domains, including, but not limited to: (a) decision making within game scenarios with penalties for incorrect responses [25], (b) assessments of suitable candidates for scholarship awards [26], (c) product-selection processes and subsequent in-store purchasing decisions by consumers [27], and (d) resolutions of perceptual dilemmas [28]. Moreover, our prior investigation [29] furnishes compelling evidence that context effects play a pivotal role in exit choice behaviour during evacuations.
Numerous scholars specialising in evacuation modelling have conducted extensive inquiries into the impact of contextual variables, such as exit proximity and prevailing fire conditions, on exit choice [30,31]. Nonetheless, it is worth noting that, to the best of our knowledge, relatively few modelling simulations have accounted for the psychological responses of evacuees to visual cues and available information during building evacuations. This study seeks to address this gap by integrating context effects into the development of a social force model. By incorporating psychological reactions into the visual context, our model aims to provide a more comprehensive representation of the complex decision-making dynamics inherent to evacuation scenarios.
The subsequent sections of this paper are structured as follows. Section 2 provides an overview of context effects, a brief explanation of utility function, and a general introduction to the social force model. In Section 3, a comprehensive account of the experiment and simulation methodologies is delineated. Section 4 is dedicated to presenting simulation results, followed by a comparative analysis with empirical data. Section 5 engages in a thorough discussion of the findings. Lastly, Section 6 offers a summary of the discoveries and provides concluding remarks, encapsulating the core insights derived from this study.

2. Related Works

2.1. Context Effects

Human decision making frequently diverges from strategic and rational principles, primarily due to the human brain’s struggle to provide accurate and precise measurements for available options. As Kahneman [19] observed: people are lazy controllers who avoid effortful thinking when possible. This phenomenon aligns with the principle of least effort, whereby individuals inherently seek paths of minimal cognitive exertion. Numerous studies [19,32,33,34,35] have observed that people make a decision based on cognition, heuristics, and biases. The phenomenon of context effects constitutes a notable aspect of cognitive psychology, elucidating systematic alterations in decision-making behaviour stemming from individuals’ perception of choice sets [35]. The context effects include the compromise effect, similarity effect, and attraction effect [24,35].
The compromise effect [36] posits that individuals tend to perceive a particular option more favourably when they view it as a compromise among available choices, rather than considering it as an extreme or outlier selection [37]. For example, Figure 1a shows the distances and congestion of two exits in front of an evacuee. Exit A is located in close proximity to the evacuee but is heavily congested, while Exit C is farther away but less crowded. In general, the probabilities of the evacuee choosing either Exit A or Exit C as their target are similar. Then, we introduce a third option, Exit B, which differs significantly from both Exit A and Exit C. In accordance with the compromise effect, the presence of Exit B results in a reduction in the probability of the evacuee choosing either Exit A or Exit C as their preferred exit, while the likelihood of choosing Exit B increases. This shift in behaviour reflects a preference for Exit B as the compromise option, akin to a customer rating a restaurant’s service from a range of five options: very poor, poor, fair, good, and very good. In such cases, most customers tend to avoid extreme ratings (i.e., very poor and very good) in favour of selecting a compromise option, a behaviour elucidated in previous literature [36].
The similarity effect [38] denotes a phenomenon where decision makers tend to change their choices away from options that closely resemble existing alternatives and lean toward options that exhibit dissimilarity, particularly when the similar option is neither clearly superior nor inferior [39]. This effect can manifest in exit choice scenarios, as shown in Figure 1b. In the absence of Exit B, the probabilities of evacuees choosing Exit A and Exit C as their preferred exits are comparable. However, with the introduction of Exit B, which closely resembles Exit A, the probability of evacuees choosing Exit A and Exit B diminish, while the probability of choosing Exit C, which significantly differs from both Exit A and Exit B, increases. This change in choice behaviour illustrates the presence of the similarity effect, wherein decision makers are inclined to opt for options that are dissimilar to those already under consideration. A parallel example in a shopping context reinforces this concept: consider encountering three apples in a fruit shop, two of which closely resemble each other in terms of size, smell, and colour, and both are smaller but cheaper than the third apple. Due to the similarity effect, customers are more likely to choose the third apple due to its uniqueness and dissimilarity to the other two, even though it is comparatively larger and more expensive.
The attraction effect [40] describes a phenomenon where the likelihood of choosing a superior option is heightened when an option that is similar but inferior is introduced into the choice set [24,37]. This effect is illustrated in Figure 1c within the context of exit choice during an evacuation. Exit A, while in close proximity to an evacuee, is congested, whereas Exit C, though farther away, offers less congestion. When Exit A and Exit C are the sole exit options, the probabilities of choosing either exit are comparable. However, with the introduction of Exit B, which is marginally less favourable than Exit A, evacuees tend to be drawn toward similar exit alternatives. As a result, a majority of evacuees opt for the superior exit, Exit A. This effect can also be demonstrated by a shopping scenario. Consider three toaster options: a USD 3 toaster (toaster A) with two slots wide enough for standard white bread, a USD 9 toaster (toaster C) with six slots of the same size, and a USD 3 toaster (toaster B) with two slots that are too narrow for standard white bread. In this scenario, customers tend to evaluate the two similar toasters (A and B) and often choose toaster A due to its superior attributes. Consequently, toaster C becomes less attractive to customers due to the presence of the attraction effect.
Decades of research have observed these effects and explored how context influences preferences in decision making involving multiple attributes and alternatives, spanning from traveller choice dynamics [41] to consumer decision-making processes [42], from the intricacies of risky decision making [43] to the complexities of market behaviour [44], and from the discernment of preferences by human decision makers [45] to the remarkable decision-making abilities exhibited by non-human entities such as honeybees [46] and even slime mould [47]. These findings highlight the necessity for any serious theoretical model to capture the context effects in the decision-making process [48,49].

2.2. Utility Function

Modelling exit choice behaviour is generally based on the framework of utility function [50,51,52,53], which depends on the linear weighting attributes of available exits [15,30]. The exit with the maximum utility is chosen as the target exit. To account for the evacuees’ behavioural uncertainty, the utility U i k t of option k for evacuee i at time t is expressed as [14,15,31]:
U i k t = V i k t + ε i k t
where V i k t denotes a deterministic component given by Equation (2) and ε i k t is a random residual parameter:
V i k t = m = 0 M β m k X i m k t
Here, there are M factors influencing exit choice. X i m k t is the expected value of the mth factor affecting the choice for option k perceived by evacuee i at time t. β m k is the weight parameter representing the evacuees’ preferences related to the factor m.

2.3. Social Force Model

Social Force (SF) model is one of the most widely used evacuation simulation models with the significant merits of simulating continuous movement [9,54] and describing the realistic self-organisation phenomena of crowd behaviour, e.g., arching and clogging [9], lane formation [55], “faster is slower” [56], and stop-and-go waves [57]. The SF model can be used to describe the diversity of pedestrians, e.g., disabilities [58] and wicked pedestrians [59]. The SF model is based on Newton’s Second Law, which describes the force generated by evacuees and their surroundings observed in crowd evacuations [54,57]. The force expression is given by Equation (3), which can be divided into three parts: the self-driving force of evacuee i, f i 0 (Unit: N); interaction force between evacuee i and j, f i j (Unit: N); and interaction force between evacuee i and walls W, f i W (Unit: N):
m i d v i ( t ) d t = f i 0 + j ( i ) f i j + W f i W
where evacuee i of mass m i (Unit: kg) changes his or her position with the velocity v i ( t ) = d r i d t (Unit: m/s). The expression of self-driving force is as follows:
f i 0 = m i v i 0 ( t ) e i 0 ( t ) v i ( t ) τ i
where evacuee i likes to move with a desired velocity v i 0 ( t ) (Unit: m/s) in the expected direction e i 0 ( t ) at time t, and τ i is the acceleration time from the current velocity to the desired velocity.
In rooms with multiple exits, evacuees need to make a decision on their direction of movement, i.e., e i 0 ( t ) . Several studies have explored exit choice strategies within the SF model framework. Xie et al. [60], Hou et al. [61], and Song et al. [62] delineated exit choice strategies for evacuees, categorising them based on their roles. Xie et al. [60] and Hou et al. [61] distinguished between leaders and followers, while Song et al. [62] classified them as authority figures and normal evacuees. Leaders and authority figures were typically directed towards either the nearest exit or the exit with lower pedestrian density, while followers or normal evacuees tended to follow the guidance of their leaders or authority figures. Zheng et al. [63] proposed an improved SF model to determine the exit direction by probability with consideration of spatial distance and occupant density. They defined transition rules to address the issue of unrealistic evacuee trajectories in previous methods. Fu et al. [64] proposed a static and dynamic exit choice model to calculate the probability of exits being selected, and evacuees either choose the exit with the highest probability or keep the original direction. In this study, the proposed CE-SF model is used to determine the desired moving direction, i.e., e i 0 ( t ) . This approach upgrades the SF model from pre-defining an exit for each occupant to evacuees autonomously choosing an exit based on their visual context.

3. Methodology

3.1. Experimental Data

We use the experimental data collected from a field observation reported by Haghani et al. [16] for model calibration and validation. The controlled experiment was conducted in a rectangular room equipped with two entrances (each 1 m wide) and three exits (each 0.5 m wide), as depicted in Figure 2. Participants entered the room through Entrance I and II and made decisions on exiting through Exit A, B, or C. Two types of scenarios were implemented: low- and high-urgency scenarios. In the low-urgency scenarios, 33 participants waited at Entrance I and 27 participants waited at Entrance II, instructed to leave the room without competition. Conversely, in the high-urgency scenario, 27 participants waited at Entrance I and 25 participants waited at Entrance II, tasked with escaping from the room as fast as possible. The experiment was recorded using a camera to capture participant’s trajectories and evacuation times. Additional details of the experimental setup can be found in the reference [16]. We used PeTrack [65,66] software (version v0.9) to extract participants’ trajectories, enabling us to analyse their exit choice. Trajectories were computed at a 25 frames per second rate, i.e., 0.04 s/frame. At each frame, the coordinates of the participants’ position were recorded and then used to calculate the distance to exits by comparing their coordinates with those of each available exit.
The distance d i k t (Unit: m) from evacuee i to exit k at time t is represented by the dashed straight line in Figure 2. The decision on exit choice at each time step is determined by the change in distance from t 1 to t. The choice is made based on the exit with the shorter distance and the maximum absolute distance. A semi-circular area centred on the exit position, with a radius from the evacuee, is defined as the exit area [63,67], illustrated by the red, blue, and yellow area in Figure 2. Evacuee i competes with those in the exit area who choose that exit, so the number n i k t of evacuees choosing exit k within the exit area is considered as a factor in making the exit choice, calculated at each time step.

3.2. Model Description

In this section, we introduce the ‘UF-SF model’, integrating the previously discussed utility function (UF) with the social force (SF) model, to consider the impact of specific attributes on exit choice in the experiment. Additionally, building upon this original model, we propose the context-effects-implemented social force (CE-SF) model in this study to simulate real exit choice behaviour. Both the UF-SF and CE-SF models are employed to determine the desired moving direction, with subsequent movement being driven by the SF model.

3.2.1. UF-SF Model

Li et al. [68] determined three factors governing exit choice through a VR experiment and a field study, namely: (i) distance to exits, (ii) density in front of exits, and (iii) moving speed at exits. Lovreglio et al. [69] also determined three factors contributing to exit choice: (i) the number of people using exits, (ii) the distance of an evacuee to exits, and (iii) the presence of smoke. Cai et al. [50] developed an exit choice model in which they defined the total utility of each exit, which is the linear combination of the exit’s defined static and dynamic utilities, and found that the exit with the largest total utility is chosen. The static utility is determined by the width of the exits. The dynamic utility is defined as the linear combination of two terms: (i) the distance of an evacuee to exits and (ii) the evacuees’ density at exits. The path distance and the level of congestion at exits markedly influence exit choice [64]. Based on the above factors and Equations (1) and (2), utility U is given by
U i k t = β d d i k t d i , max t + β n n i k t n i , max t
where d i k t (Unit: m) is the Euclidean distance of evacuee i from exit k at time t, see the straight dashed line in Figure 2; n i k t (Unit: ped) refers to the number of evacuees in proximity to the exit k, see people at the semi-circular region in Figure 2; and d i , max (Unit: m) is the distance of evacuee i from the farthest exit, i.e., d i , max = max k = A , B , C ( d i k t ) . Similarly, n i , max = max k = A , B , C ( n i k t ) (Unit: ped), and β d and β n are coefficient parameters of the weighting distance and exit efficiency, respectively. As the distance from the exit increases and the number of people selecting that exit rises, the utility of that exit should decrease, and thus both β d and β n are negative.

3.2.2. CE-SF Model

When a room has only one exit, no exit choice needs to be made. If there are two exits, the exit with the higher U is chosen. When there are three or more exits, the exits with the three highest U are chosen, and the final exit choice is made after accounting for context effects. According to the references [49,70,71], the coexistence of the three context effects is very rare. Therefore, a demarcation system was proposed in this study to identify the most dominant context effects for a given exit-choice scenario.
The compromise effect requires the three exits to be dissimilar to each other; the similarity effect requires two exits to be similar and the third exit to be dissimilar to the other two; and the attraction effect requires two exits to be dissimilar to each other, while the third exit should exhibit similarity but be inferior to one of the others. Therefore, the measurement of option similarity and inferiority is our primary cue for identifying the three context effects.
Two attributes, i.e., distance to exit k, d k (Unit: m) and the number of evacuees near exit k, n k (Unit: ped), are considered separately in our study as the different units and ranges. Two threshold values d sim (Unit: m) and n sim (Unit: ped) are defined to demarcate similarity and dissimilarity. That is, Exit k 1 and k 2 are similar if Equation (6) is satisfied; otherwise, they are dissimilar. Similarly, two threshold values d inf (Unit: m) and n inf (Unit: ped) are defined to demarcate inferiority and non-inferiority. Exit k 2 is inferior to Exit k 1 if Equation (7) is satisfied; otherwise, it is non-inferior. Here, 0 < d inf < d sim 0 < n inf < n sim .
| d k 2 d k 1 | d sim | n k 2 n k 1 | n sim
d k 2 d k 1 d inf n k 2 n k 1 n inf
Figure 3 illustrates how context effects influence exit choice. The size marking on the rectangular area of the left of Figure 3 is determined by Equations (6) and (7). An option within the light-coloured area is similar to the centre reference point, an option within the darker area at the bottom left corner is superior to the reference point, and an option within the darkest area at the top right is inferior to the reference point. The compromise effect occurs when the three exits are dissimilar from each other, and the compromise option, Exit B in Figure 3a, where the size of each attribute is the middle, i.e., Exit B in Equation (8), is chosen. The similarity effect occurs when the two exits are similar to each other, and the dissimilar option, see Exit C in Figure 3b, where the size of one attribute is small, and the other is large, i.e., Exit C in Equation (9), is chosen. The attraction effect occurs when the two exits are similar to each other, and one of the similar options, which is superior to the other, such as Exit A being superior to Exit B in Figure 3c, is chosen:
d A < d B < d C n A > n B > n C
d A < d B < d C n A > n B > n C or d A > d B > d C n A < n B < n C

3.2.3. Model Framework

In the CE-SF model, context effects are integrated as a new module to improve the traditional UF-SF model. The simulation process of these two models is illustrated in Figure 4 through a flow chart. First, all attributes are computed, and the U values for all available exits are determined using Equation (5). Next, the three exits with the three highest U are selected. In the UF-SF model, the target direction is oriented towards the exit with the maximum U, while in the CE-SF model, the evacuees’ visual context on exits determines the target direction. The context module is highlighted in the yellow rectangle in Figure 4. After identifying the three exits with the highest U, Equation (6) is employed to assess the similarity between the two exits. If all three exits are similar to each other, evacuees follow the moving direction of their last step. Conversely, if all three exits are dissimilar to each other and there exists a compromised exit (determined by Equation (8)), the compromise effect is applied, and the compromised exit becomes the desired target. If the dissimilar exit is not significantly superior to the two similar exits (determined by Equation (9)), and if an exit is superior to its similar exit (determined by Equation (6)), then the attraction effect is applied, and the superior exit is chosen. Otherwise, the similarity effect is applied, and the dissimilar exit is chosen. Lastly, in both UF-SF and CE-SF models, the position of evacuees is updated by the SF model after making a decision on exit choice. The simulation continues until all evacuees leave the room. The SF model parameters in this study adhere to those originally proposed by Helbing et al. [54]: k = 1.2 × 10 5 kg / s 2 , κ = 2.4 × 10 5 kg / ( m · s ) , τ i = 0.5 s , A i = 2 × 10 3 N , B i = 0.08 N . All evacuees are assumed to have a body radius of 0.2 m and a weight of 80 kg. The desired moving velocity is set as 1.2 m/s for low urgency and 2.5 m/s for high urgency [16,54].

4. Results

4.1. Sensitivity Analysis of the UF-SF Model

Two sensitivity parameters, namely β d and β n , are used to control evacuees’ preference for exit choice. β d and β n weight the influence of path distance and exit congestion, respectively. In this study, β d and β n vary from −0.3 to −10, and the combination for simulation scenarios is shown in Table 1. In scenarios U1 and U2, the effect of exit congestion is greater, while in scenarios U4 and U5, the effect of path distance is greater; in the middle scenario U3, the effect is the same for both. To compare the simulation results with the corresponding experiment results, the difference in the total evacuation time between the simulated T SIM (Unit: s) and the corresponding experimental T EXP (Unit: s) is termed the e r r o r ( T ) , given by Equation (10):
e r r o r ( T ) = | T SIM T EXP | T EXP
Table 1 shows that all the e r r o r ( T ) of UF-SF simulations are under 15%. However, the simulated moving trajectories significantly disagreed with the experiment, see Figure 5. Figure 5a,g show that at least three individuals entered the room through Entrance I and chose Exit C to leave in the experiment, as indicated by the arrow of the moving direction depicted in Figure 5a,g. In contrast, there was no one from Entrance I leaving from Exit C in the UF-SF simulations. Moreover, the number of evacuees leaving from exits over time is shown in Figure 6. The flow of people leaving from Exit A and C in the UF-SF simulation is faster than that in the experiment (see the slopes of lines in Figure 6a,c,d,f), while it is reversed for the flow of people leaving from Exit B (see Figure 6b,e).
Although the evacuation time of the UF-SF simulation is consistent with the experiment, the moving trajectories and the exit choice are significantly different from the experiment. Therefore, to simulate the real exit choice behaviour, in this study, the context effects are implemented into an SF model, i.e., the CE-SF model. Considering the relatively good performance of e r r o r ( T ) , moving trajectories, and the flow of people leaving exits in the UF-SF simulations, the parameters in scenario U2, i.e., β d = 0.3 , β n = 1 , are adopted in the CE-SF model.

4.2. Simulation Performance of the CE-SF Model

4.2.1. Sensitivity Analysis of the CE-SF Model

Two pairs of threshold parameters d sim , n sim and d inf , n inf are used to determine the similarity and the inferiority of two available exits, which further determine whether the context effects can be applied. Specifically, d sim and n sim are the smallest detectable difference of d k and n k between two exits. The Just Noticeable Difference (JND), which signifies the visibility threshold, refers to the smallest detectable difference between two stimuli [72]. According to Weber’s Law, the JND is directly proportional to the magnitude of the stimulus ( ϕ ), expressed mathematically as Δ ϕ = k ϕ , where k is Weber fraction, a constant [73]. Research indicates that Weber fractions for perceived 3D lengths vary between 25 and 30% [74], and the median Weber fraction for the visual ratio was found to be 32.6% [75]. Therefore, in this study, the range of d sim and n sim is derived from the JND and Weber fraction k and is estimated as 30%. Given that the maximum distance to an exit is 11.8 m and the number of evacuees near the exit does not exceed 20 ped, consequently, d sim 3.5 m, n sim 6 ped. d sim ranges from 2 to 3 m, and n sim ranges from 3 to 6 ped during simulations. Additionally, d inf and n inf are introduced to delineate inferiority and non-inferiority, as shown in Figure 3; that is, 0 < d inf < d sim 0 < n inf < n sim . Thus, the range of d inf is set from 1 to 1.5 m, and n inf is 2 ped. Table 2 shows the value of these parameters adopted in this study and the nine simulation scenarios, i.e., C1–C9 in the CE-SF model.
The percentage of evacuees entering the room from Entrance I and II and leaving from Exit A, B, and C is presented in Figure 7. Whether under low urgency (Figure 7a) or high urgency (Figure 7b), around 10% of evacuees entered from Entrance I and left from Exit B or C in the experiment (EXP). In contrast, in the UF-SF model, no evacuees who entered the room from Entrance I left from Exit C under low or high urgency. In the CE-SF model, the percentages of evacuees entering from Entrance I and leaving from Exit B and C fluctuate around 10%, except for scenario C9. To further compare the simulation and experimental results, the difference in the percentage of evacuees leaving the exit between the simulated P SIM and experimental P EXP is determined by Equation (11):
e r r o r ( P ) = i = A , B , C j = I , II | P SIM i , j P EXP i , j |
Figure 8 illustrates the simulation error of evacuation time, e r r o r ( T ) , and the percentage of evacuees leaving from exits, e r r o r ( P ) . The e r r o r ( T ) under low urgency, as shown in Figure 8a, in the CE-SF model, except for C7, fluctuates at the e r r o r ( T ) of the UF-SF model, i.e., 0.1. The e r r o r ( T ) under high urgency in the CE-SF model is larger than that in the UF-SF model. However, under both levels of urgency, the e r r o r ( P ) (see Figure 8b) of the CE-SF model is significantly smaller than that of the UF-SF model. To further investigate the differences between the UF-SF and CE-SF models, the scenario of C1 is selected for additional analysis, due to its commendable performance in both e r r o r ( T ) and e r r o r ( P ) .

4.2.2. Different Percentages of Evacuees Affected by Context Effect

To further investigate the influence of context effects on our proposed exit choice model, some evacuees were designated to adhere to the CE-SF model while others followed the UF-SF model. Three simulation scenarios were conducted, where different percentages of evacuees (i.e., 100%, 80%, and 60%) adhered to the CE-SF model, denoted as CE-SF_100%, CE-SF_80%, and CE-SF_60%, respectively, while the remaining evacuees followed the UF-SF model, which adopted the parameters of C1.
Figure 9 presents a comparison of movement trajectories between the experimental data and simulations. In both low- and high-urgency scenarios of the experiment, evacuees were observed entering the room from Entrance I and exiting from Exit C, as depicted by the arrow of the moving direction in Figure 9a,g. Notably, this phenomenon was effectively simulated by the CE-SF model across various scenarios, irrespective of the percentage of evacuees influenced by the context effects (see Figure 9d–f for low urgency and Figure 9j–l for high urgency). However, this observed behaviour was notably absent in simulations conducted using the SF model with the shortest path (Shortest) or the UF-SF model, as evidenced in Figure 9b,c,h,i. Additionally, the experimental observations revealed instances of exit choice-changing behaviour, particularly notable in scenarios characterised by high urgency (see Figure 9g). In contrast, simulations utilising the Shortest or UF-SF models exhibited rare instances of exit choice alteration (see Figure 9b,c,h,i). Impressively, our proposed CE-SF model successfully replicated this observed decision-changing behaviour across various scenarios, regardless of the percentage of evacuees influenced by the context effects. Notably, the scenario where 80% of evacuees adhered to the CE-SF model demonstrated the highest fidelity in simulating exit choice-changing behaviour. The CE-SF model exhibits remarkable flexibility in modelling both exit choice behaviour and exit choice-changing behaviour, which has been observed in numerous empirical studies [76,77,78,79].
Moreover, Figure 10 shows that the number of evacuees leaving from exits over time in the three scenarios of the CE-SF model is much more consistent with the experimental data compared to that from the Shortest and UF-SF models. Although the errors in evacuation time for the UF-SF and Shortest are smaller than those in the CE-SF model (still less than 0.3), as shown in Figure 11a, the errors in the percentage of evacuees leaving from exits in the UF-SF and Shortest models are much larger than those in the CE-SF model, as depicted in Figure 11b.

4.3. Evidenceof Context Effects in Experimental Data

The coordinates of each participant in the room for each frame were obtained using PeTrack software (version v0.9) from the experimental video (see Section 3.1 for more details), facilitating the extraction of exit k attributes, i.e., d k and n k . Subsequently, Equations (6) and (7) were employed to determine whether exit choice is influenced by the context effects or not. The parameter pairs in Equations (6) and (7) are given by the scenario of C1: d sim = 2 m, n sim = 3 ped, d inf = 1.5 m, and n inf = 2 ped. Finally, the cumulative occurrences of occurrences in the context effects are computed and shown in Figure 12. All three context effects—the compromise effect, similarity effect, and attraction effect—were observed in the experiment under both low and high urgency levels. Notably, the similarity effect occurred more frequently than the compromise and attraction effects.

5. Discussion

The simulation results in the UF-SF model (see Section 4.1) reveal that the traditional utility function model, relying solely on a monotonic function of the attribute value, fails to accurately mimic real exit choice behaviour. Previous studies [24,37,80] have demonstrated that the simple scalability property, fundamental to most utility models, inadequately accounts for the three context effects commonly observed in multialternative and multiattribute decision-making tasks. Moreover, significant evidence of context effects emerged in evacuation experiments under varying urgency levels (see Section 4.3). This evidence elucidates why our CE-SF model outperforms the original exit choice model, i.e., the UF-SF model, in replicating exit choice behaviour. However, errors persist in movement trajectories and evacuation time during CE-SF simulations. These errors can be mitigated by reducing the percentage of evacuees affected by context effects to 80% (see Section 4.2.2). As illustrated in the previous studies [80,81], the context effects change the probabilities of the same option across different choice sets. Nonetheless, the maximum or an increase in the probability of an option does not guarantee the final choice. Thus, it is reasonable to assume that not all individuals adhere to context effects when making decisions.

6. Conclusions and Limitations

Exit choice behaviour under varying levels of urgency was investigated by integrating context effects into a social force model (CE-SF). The capability of the CE-SF model was validated by comparing evacuation time, exit utilities, and movement trajectories with those of a utility-function-based SF model (UF-SF). A sensitivity analysis of the UF-SF model revealed that while the simulated evacuation time aligned well with experimental data across urgency levels, the trajectories and exit utilities exhibited significant discrepancies from the experiment, irrespective of variations in the exit attribute parameters. Despite CE-SF displaying a slightly inferior performance in evacuation time compared to UF-SF, it surpassed UF-SF in terms of trajectory accuracy and exit utilities regardless of the parameter values. The simulation results of CE-SF with different percentages of evacuees influenced by context effects indicated that adjusting the percentage to 80% could enhance trajectory accuracy, albeit leading to a slight increase in the evacuation time error. Furthermore, we provided substantial evidence of three context effects during real evacuation decision making, irrespective of urgency levels.
However, our study has limitations that need addressing in future research. Firstly, we solely explore variations in participant urgency within the same experimental layout. Despite each evacuee facing the same choice set of exits A, B, and C, the properties of each exit (e.g., distance and congestion) differ at each time step. In other words, in the continuous simulations, the evacuees are faced with a different choice set due to the different exit properties at each time step.
Secondly, our study only considers three exits. The definition of context effects necessitates a choice set with three or more options. While most studies [24,35,37,49] on context effects examined a choice set of three options, some researchers [82,83] have observed context effects in choice sets with more than three options. Additionally, a recent study [84] suggested that comparisons between multiple options could be made in pairs before finally being considered together. Another approach to addressing the issue of having more than three exits could involve considering only the three most preferred exits for each evacuee, as it is unlikely for an evacuee to choose exits with low preference levels. Therefore, our findings can be applicable to evacuation scenarios with more than three exits.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, D.G. and E.W.M.L.; methodology, D.G.; software, D.G.; validation, D.G., X.L. and Q.C.; formal analysis, D.G. and H.Q.; investigation, X.L., Q.C. and H.Q.; writing—original draft preparation, D.G.; writing—review and editing, D.G.; supervision, E.W.M.L.; funding acquisition, E.W.M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Research Grants Council of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region China (project no. CityU 11208119) and by a grant from CityU (project no. SRG-Fd 7005895).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Soomaroo, L.; Murray, V. Disasters at mass gatherings: Lessons from history. Plos Curr. 2012, 4, RRN1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Sharma, A.; McCloskey, B.; Hui, D.S.; Rambia, A.; Zumla, A.; Traore, T.; Shafi, S.; El-Kafrawy, S.A.; Azhar, E.I.; Zumla, A.; et al. Global mass gathering events and deaths due to crowd surge, stampedes, crush and physical injuries-lessons from the Seoul Halloween and other disasters. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 2023, 52, 102524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Haghani, M. Optimising crowd evacuations: Mathematical, architectural and behavioural approaches. Saf. Sci. 2020, 128, 104745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wang, P.; Cao, S. Simulation of pedestrian evacuation strategies under limited visibility. Phys. Lett. A 2019, 383, 825–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Zhou, J.; Guo, Y.; Dong, S.; Zhang, M.; Mao, T. Simulation of pedestrian evacuation route choice using social force model in large-scale public space: Comparison of five evacuation strategies. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0221872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ma, G.; Wang, Y.; Jiang, S. Optimization of building exit layout: Combining exit decisions of evacuees. Adv. Civ. Eng. 2021, 2021, 6622661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Feng, Y.; Duives, D.C.; Hoogendoorn, S.P. Using virtual reality to study pedestrian exit choice behaviour during evacuations. Saf. Sci. 2021, 137, 105158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Zhang, M.; Xu, R.; Siu, M.F.F.; Luo, X. Human decision change in crowd evacuation: A virtual reality-based study. J. Build. Eng. 2023, 68, 106041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Helbing, D.; Molnar, P. Social force model for pedestrian dynamics. Phys. Rev. E 1995, 51, 4282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lovreglio, R.; Fonzone, A.; Dell’Olio, L.; Borri, D. A study of herding behaviour in exit choice during emergencies based on random utility theory. Saf. Sci. 2016, 82, 421–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhao, H.; Gao, Z. Reserve capacity and exit choosing in pedestrian evacuation dynamics. J. Phys. A Math. Theor. 2010, 43, 105001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kinateder, M.; Warren, W.H. Social influence on evacuation behavior in real and virtual environments. Front. Robot. AI 2016, 3, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gao, D.L.; Xie, W.; Lee, E.W.M. Individual-level exit choice behaviour under uncertain risk. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2022, 604, 127873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Song, X.B.; Lovreglio, R. Investigating personalized exit choice behavior in fire accidents using the hierarchical Bayes estimator of the random coefficient logit model. Anal. Methods Accid. Res. 2021, 29, 100140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lovreglio, R.; Fonzone, A.; Dell’Olio, L. A mixed logit model for predicting exit choice during building evacuations. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2016, 92, 59–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Haghani, M.; Sarvi, M.; Shahhoseini, Z. Evacuation behaviour of crowds under high and low levels of urgency: Experiments of reaction time, exit choice and exit-choice adaptation. Saf. Sci. 2020, 126, 104679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Haghani, M.; Sarvi, M. Following the crowd or avoiding it? Empirical investigation of imitative behaviour in emergency escape of human crowds. Anim. Behav. 2017, 124, 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lovreglio, R. Modelling Decision-Making in Fire Evacuation Based on Random Utility Theory. Ph.D. Thesis, Polytechnic University of Bari, Bari, Italy, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kahneman, D.; Sibony, O.; Sunstein, C.R. Noise: A Flaw in Human Judgment; Hachette: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  20. Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 1992, 5, 297–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Klüpfel, H.; Schreckenberg, M.; Meyer-König, T. Models for crowd movement and egress simulation. In Traffic and Granular Flow’03; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2005; pp. 357–372. [Google Scholar]
  22. Thinking, C.H.O. Cambridge Handbook Of Thinking And Reasoning Ebook; Psychology Press: London, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  23. Gao, S.; Li, N. Preference reversal and the evolution of cooperation. Appl. Math. Comput. 2023, 438, 127567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Trueblood, J.S.; Brown, S.D.; Heathcote, A.; Busemeyer, J.R. Not just for consumers: Context effects are fundamental to decision making. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 901–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Spektor, M.S.; Kellen, D.; Hotaling, J.M. When the good looks bad: An experimental exploration of the repulsion effect. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 1309–1320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. O’Curry, Y.P.S.; Pitts, R. The attraction effect and political choice in two elections. J. Consum. Psychol. 1995, 4, 85–101. [Google Scholar]
  27. Doyle, J.R.; O’Connor, D.J.; Reynolds, G.M.; Bottomley, P.A. The robustness of the asymmetrically dominated effect: Buying frames, phantom alternatives, and in-store purchases. Psychol. Mark. 1999, 16, 225–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Trueblood, J.S.; Pettibone, J.C. The phantom decoy effect in perceptual decision making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2017, 30, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gao, D.; Lee, E.W.M.; Lee, Y.Y. The influence of context effects on exit choice behavior during building evacuation combining virtual reality and discrete choice modeling. Adv. Eng. Inform. 2023, 57, 102072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Cao, S.; Fu, L.; Song, W. Exit selection and pedestrian movement in a room with two exits under fire emergency. Appl. Math. Comput. 2018, 332, 136–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Guo, R.-Y.; Huang, H.-J. Logit-based exit choice model of evacuation in rooms with internal obstacles and multiple exits. Chin. Phys. B 2010, 19, 030501. [Google Scholar]
  32. Muñoz, M.A.; Pineda, S.; Morales, J.M. A bilevel framework for decision-making under uncertainty with contextual information. Omega 2022, 108, 102575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Pirrone, A.; Reina, A.; Stafford, T.; Marshall, J.A.; Gobet, F. Magnitude-sensitivity: Rethinking decision-making. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 66–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cheng, Y.; Liu, W.; Yuan, X.; Jiang, Y. Following Other People’s Footsteps: A Contextual-Attraction Effect Induced by Biological Motion. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 33, 1522–1531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Spektor, M.S.; Bhatia, S.; Gluth, S. The elusiveness of context effects in decision making. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2021, 25, 843–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Simonson, I. Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects. J. Consum. Res. 1989, 16, 158–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Trueblood, J.S.; Brown, S.D.; Heathcote, A. The multiattribute linear ballistic accumulator model of context effects in multialternative choice. Psychol. Rev. 2014, 121, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Tversky, A. Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychol. Rev. 1972, 79, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Zalta, E.N.; Nodelman, U.; Allen, C.; Anderson, R.L. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; The Metaphysics Research Lab: Stanford, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  40. Huber, J.; Payne, J.W.; Puto, C. Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. J. Consum. Res. 1982, 9, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kim, J.; Park, J.; Lee, J.; Kim, S.; Gonzalez-Jimenez, H.; Lee, J.; Choi, Y.K.; Lee, J.C.; Jang, S.; Franklin, D.; et al. COVID-19 and extremeness aversion: The role of safety seeking in travel decision making. J. Travel Res. 2022, 61, 837–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Milberg, S.J.; Silva, M.; Celedon, P.; Sinn, F. Synthesis of attraction effect research: Practical market implications? Eur. J. Mark. 2014, 48, 1413–1430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Castillo, G. The attraction effect and its explanations. Games Econ. Behav. 2020, 119, 123–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Wu, C.; Cosguner, K. Profiting from the decoy effect: A case study of an online diamond retailer. Mark. Sci. 2020, 39, 974–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Arad, A.; Bachi, B.; Maltz, A. On the relevance of irrelevant strategies. Exp. Econ. 2023, 26, 1142–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Shafir, S.; Waite, T.A.; Smith, B.H. Context-dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2002, 51, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Latty, T.; Beekman, M. Irrational decision-making in an amoeboid organism: Transitivity and context-dependent preferences. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 278, 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Spektor, M.S.; Kellen, D.; Klauer, K.C. The repulsion effect in preferential choice and its relation to perceptual choice. Cognition 2022, 225, 105164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Turner, B.M.; Schley, D.R.; Muller, C.; Tsetsos, K. Competing theories of multialternative, multiattribute preferential choice. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 125, 329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Cai, Z.; Zhou, R.; Cui, Y.; Wang, Y.; Jiang, J. Influencing factors for exit selection in subway station evacuation. Tunn. Undergr. Space Technol. 2022, 125, 104498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wang, L.; Zheng, J.H.; Zhang, X.S.; Zhang, J.L.; Wang, Q.Z.; Zhang, Q. Pedestrians’ behavior in emergency evacuation: Modeling and simulation. Chin. Phys. B 2016, 25, 118901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Veeraswamy, A. Computational Modelling of Agent Based Path Planning and the Representation of Human Wayfinding Behaviour within Egress Models. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Greenwich, London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kuffner, J.J., Jr. Goal-directed navigation for animated characters using real-time path planning and control. In International Workshop on Capture Techniques for Virtual Environments; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1998; pp. 171–186. [Google Scholar]
  54. Helbing, D.; Farkas, I.; Vicsek, T. Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 2000, 407, 487–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Xie, W.; Lee, E.W.M.; Lee, Y.Y. Self-organisation phenomena in pedestrian counter flows and its modelling. Saf. Sci. 2022, 155, 105875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Helbing, D.; Johansson, A. Pedestrian, crowd, and evacuation dynamics. arXiv 2013, arXiv:1309.1609. [Google Scholar]
  57. Chen, X.; Treiber, M.; Kanagaraj, V.; Li, H. Social force models for pedestrian traffic–state of the art. Transp. Rev. 2018, 38, 625–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Fu, L.; Qin, H.; He, Y.; Shi, Y. Application of the social force modelling method to evacuation dynamics involving pedestrians with disabilities. Appl. Math. Comput. 2024, 460, 128297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kang, Z.; Zhang, L.; Li, K. An improved social force model for pedestrian dynamics in shipwrecks. Appl. Math. Comput. 2019, 348, 355–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Xie, W.; Gao, D.; Lee, E.W. Detecting undeclared-leader-follower structure in pedestrian evacuation using transfer entropy. IEEE Trans. Intell. Transp. Syst. 2022, 23, 17644–17653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hou, L.; Liu, J.G.; Pan, X.; Wang, B.H. A social force evacuation model with the leadership effect. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2014, 400, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Song, X.; Zhang, Z.; Peng, G.; Shi, G. Effect of authority figures for pedestrian evacuation at metro stations. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2017, 465, 599–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Zheng, X.; Li, H.Y.; Meng, L.Y.; Xu, X.Y.; Chen, X. Improved social force model based on exit selection for microscopic pedestrian simulation in subway station. J. Cent. South Univ. 2015, 22, 4490–4497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Fu, Y.; Shi, W.; Zeng, Y.; Zhang, H.; Liu, X.; Liu, Y. Simulation study on pedestrian evacuation optimization in a multi-exit building. J. Physics. Conf. Ser. 2021, 1780, 012024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Boltes, M.; Seyfried, A.; Steffen, B.; Schadschneider, A. Automatic extraction of pedestrian trajectories from video recordings. In Pedestrian and Evacuation Dynamics 2008; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 43–54. [Google Scholar]
  66. Boltes, M.; Seyfried, A. Collecting pedestrian trajectories. Neurocomputing 2013, 100, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Yuan, W.; Tan, K.H. An evacuation model using cellular automata. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2007, 384, 549–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Li, H.; Zhang, J.; Xia, L.; Song, W.; Bode, N.W. Comparing the route-choice behavior of pedestrians around obstacles in a virtual experiment and a field study. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2019, 107, 120–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Lovreglio, R.; Dillies, E.; Kuligowski, E.; Rahouti, A.; Haghani, M. Exit choice in built environment evacuation combining immersive virtual reality and discrete choice modelling. Autom. Constr. 2022, 141, 104452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Liew, S.X.; Howe, P.D.; Little, D.R. The appropriacy of averaging in the study of context effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016, 23, 1639–1646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Trueblood, J.S.; Brown, S.D.; Heathcote, A. The fragile nature of contextual preference reversals: Reply to Tsetsos, Chater, and Usher (2015). Psychol. Rev. 2015, 122, 848–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Luce, R.D.; Edwards, W. The derivation of subjective scales from just noticeable differences. Psychol. Rev. 1958, 65, 222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Norwich, K.H. On the theory of Weber fractions. Percept. Psychophys. 1987, 42, 286–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Norman, J.F.; Todd, J.T.; Perotti, V.J.; Tittle, J.S. The visual perception of three-dimensional length. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1996, 22, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Gomez, D.M.; Dartnell, P. Psychophysical Distance between Visually-Presented Pairs of Ratios. Available online: https://osf.io/y4zmn/download/?format=pdf (accessed on 14 May 2024).
  76. Bode, N.W.; Kemloh Wagoum, A.U.; Codling, E.A. Human responses to multiple sources of directional information in virtual crowd evacuations. J. R. Soc. Interface 2014, 11, 20130904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Bode, N.W.; Kemloh Wagoum, A.U.; Codling, E.A. Information use by humans during dynamic route choice in virtual crowd evacuations. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2015, 2, 140410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Haghani, M.; Sarvi, M. Simulating dynamics of adaptive exit-choice changing in crowd evacuations: Model implementation and behavioural interpretations. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2019, 103, 56–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Liao, W.; Kemloh Wagoum, A.U.; Bode, N.W. Route choice in pedestrians: Determinants for initial choices and revising decisions. J. R. Soc. Interface 2017, 14, 20160684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Wollschlaeger, L.M.; Diederich, A. Similarity, attraction, and compromise effects: Original findings, recent empirical observations, and computational cognitive process models. Am. J. Psychol. 2020, 133, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Spektor, M.S.; Gluth, S.; Fontanesi, L.; Rieskamp, J. How similarity between choice options affects decisions from experience: The accentuation-of-differences model. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 126, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Pinger, P.; Ruhmer-Krell, I.; Schumacher, H. The compromise effect in action: Lessons from a restaurant’s menu. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2016, 128, 14–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Kuncel, N.R.; Dahlke, J.A. Decoy effects improve diversity hiring. Pers. Assess. Decis. 2020, 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Lee, M.D.; Ke, M.Y. Framing effects and preference reversals in crowd-sourced ranked opinions. Decision 2022, 9, 153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Illustration of (a) compromise effect; (b) similarity effect; and (c) attraction effect in the context of exit choice during evacuations. The arrow points to the exit more likely to be selected under three distinct context effects, with varying Exit B conditions, while the choice set {Exit A, Exit C} remains constant.
Figure 1. Illustration of (a) compromise effect; (b) similarity effect; and (c) attraction effect in the context of exit choice during evacuations. The arrow points to the exit more likely to be selected under three distinct context effects, with varying Exit B conditions, while the choice set {Exit A, Exit C} remains constant.
Fire 07 00169 g001
Figure 2. Schematic of the experimental and modelling setup. To estimate the number of people in proximity to each exit, we use the exit area [63,67], which is defined as a semi-circular region centred on the exit and having a radius equal to the distance between the evacuee and the exit. For Exit A, B, and C, the exit areas are indicated by red, blue, and yellow semi-circular regions, respectively.
Figure 2. Schematic of the experimental and modelling setup. To estimate the number of people in proximity to each exit, we use the exit area [63,67], which is defined as a semi-circular region centred on the exit and having a radius equal to the distance between the evacuee and the exit. For Exit A, B, and C, the exit areas are indicated by red, blue, and yellow semi-circular regions, respectively.
Fire 07 00169 g002
Figure 3. Demonstration of the (a) compromise effect for choosing the compromise option, i.e., Exit B; (b) similarity effect for choosing the dissimilar option, i.e., Exit C; and (c) attractive effect for choosing the superior option, i.e., Exit A. The target exit is highlighted in yellow. For each rectangular area, the light-coloured area around the exit point is the similar range; the darker area at the bottom left corner is the superior range; the darkest-coloured area at the top right corner is the inferior range.
Figure 3. Demonstration of the (a) compromise effect for choosing the compromise option, i.e., Exit B; (b) similarity effect for choosing the dissimilar option, i.e., Exit C; and (c) attractive effect for choosing the superior option, i.e., Exit A. The target exit is highlighted in yellow. For each rectangular area, the light-coloured area around the exit point is the similar range; the darker area at the bottom left corner is the superior range; the darkest-coloured area at the top right corner is the inferior range.
Fire 07 00169 g003
Figure 4. Flow chart of the simulation process.
Figure 4. Flow chart of the simulation process.
Fire 07 00169 g004
Figure 5. Evacuees’ movement trajectories in the experiment (EXP) under low urgency (a) and high urgency (g), and simulated in UF-SF model by different parameters (see U1–U5 in Table 1 under low urgency (bf) and high urgency (hl)).
Figure 5. Evacuees’ movement trajectories in the experiment (EXP) under low urgency (a) and high urgency (g), and simulated in UF-SF model by different parameters (see U1–U5 in Table 1 under low urgency (bf) and high urgency (hl)).
Fire 07 00169 g005
Figure 6. The number of evacuees leaving from Exit A, i.e., (a,d); Exit B, i.e., (b,e); and Exit C, i.e., (c,f) under low urgency (ac) and high urgency (df) in the different scenarios of UF-SF model, i.e., U1–U5.
Figure 6. The number of evacuees leaving from Exit A, i.e., (a,d); Exit B, i.e., (b,e); and Exit C, i.e., (c,f) under low urgency (ac) and high urgency (df) in the different scenarios of UF-SF model, i.e., U1–U5.
Fire 07 00169 g006
Figure 7. Percentage of evacuees choosing the exit is depicted for (a) low-urgency and (b) high-urgency scenarios. The columns without and with white diagonal lines represent the percentage of evacuees entering the room from Entrance I and II, respectively. The black dashed line indicates the dividing line between the percentage of evacuees entering from Entrance I and II in the experiment.
Figure 7. Percentage of evacuees choosing the exit is depicted for (a) low-urgency and (b) high-urgency scenarios. The columns without and with white diagonal lines represent the percentage of evacuees entering the room from Entrance I and II, respectively. The black dashed line indicates the dividing line between the percentage of evacuees entering from Entrance I and II in the experiment.
Fire 07 00169 g007
Figure 8. The simulation error of (a) total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) and (b) percentage of evacuees leaving from exits e r r o r ( P ) in the different scenarios of CE-SF model, i.e., C1–C9. The scenario of C1 in the yellow oval is selected for further study.
Figure 8. The simulation error of (a) total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) and (b) percentage of evacuees leaving from exits e r r o r ( P ) in the different scenarios of CE-SF model, i.e., C1–C9. The scenario of C1 in the yellow oval is selected for further study.
Fire 07 00169 g008
Figure 9. The movement trajectories of evacuees are depicted in the following scenarios: (a,g) experiment (EXP); (b,h) SF model using the shortest path (Shortest); (c,i) UF-SF model; and (df,jl) CE-SF model with varying percentages of context effect under low urgency (af) and high urgency (gl).
Figure 9. The movement trajectories of evacuees are depicted in the following scenarios: (a,g) experiment (EXP); (b,h) SF model using the shortest path (Shortest); (c,i) UF-SF model; and (df,jl) CE-SF model with varying percentages of context effect under low urgency (af) and high urgency (gl).
Fire 07 00169 g009
Figure 10. The number of evacuees leaving from Exit A, i.e., (a,d); Exit B, i.e., (b,e); and Exit C, i.e., (c,f) under low urgency (ac) and high urgency (df) in the CE-SF model with different percentages of context effects.
Figure 10. The number of evacuees leaving from Exit A, i.e., (a,d); Exit B, i.e., (b,e); and Exit C, i.e., (c,f) under low urgency (ac) and high urgency (df) in the CE-SF model with different percentages of context effects.
Fire 07 00169 g010
Figure 11. The simulation error of (a) total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) and (b) percentage of evacuees leaving from exits e r r o r ( P ) in the different scenarios of the CE-SF model, i.e., 100%–60%.
Figure 11. The simulation error of (a) total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) and (b) percentage of evacuees leaving from exits e r r o r ( P ) in the different scenarios of the CE-SF model, i.e., 100%–60%.
Fire 07 00169 g011
Figure 12. Cumulative number of evacuees affected by context effects in experimental data [16] under (a) low urgency and (b) high urgency.
Figure 12. Cumulative number of evacuees affected by context effects in experimental data [16] under (a) low urgency and (b) high urgency.
Fire 07 00169 g012
Table 1. Simulation scenarios and parameters used in the UF-SF model and the simulation error of total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) . The bolded line is the UF-SF parameters used for further study in the next section.
Table 1. Simulation scenarios and parameters used in the UF-SF model and the simulation error of total evacuation time e r r o r ( T ) . The bolded line is the UF-SF parameters used for further study in the next section.
No.UF-SF Model error ( T )
β d β n Low UrgencyHigh Urgency
U1−0.3−101.6%3.8%
U2−0.3−19.4%3.8%
U3−1−111.1%10.0%
U4−10−0.32.2%2.2%
U5−1−0.39.6%13.2%
Table 2. Simulation scenarios and parameters used in the CE-SF model.
Table 2. Simulation scenarios and parameters used in the CE-SF model.
No. d sim (m) n sim (ped) d inf (m) n inf (ped)
C1231.52
C2241.52
C3261.52
C42312
C52412
C62612
C7331.52
C8341.52
C9361.52
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gao, D.; Liang, X.; Chen, Q.; Qiu, H.; Lee, E.W.M. Modelling Context Effects in Exit Choice for Building Evacuations. Fire 2024, 7, 169. https://doi.org/10.3390/fire7050169

AMA Style

Gao D, Liang X, Chen Q, Qiu H, Lee EWM. Modelling Context Effects in Exit Choice for Building Evacuations. Fire. 2024; 7(5):169. https://doi.org/10.3390/fire7050169

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gao, Dongli, Xuanwen Liang, Qian Chen, Hongpeng Qiu, and Eric Wai Ming Lee. 2024. "Modelling Context Effects in Exit Choice for Building Evacuations" Fire 7, no. 5: 169. https://doi.org/10.3390/fire7050169

APA Style

Gao, D., Liang, X., Chen, Q., Qiu, H., & Lee, E. W. M. (2024). Modelling Context Effects in Exit Choice for Building Evacuations. Fire, 7(5), 169. https://doi.org/10.3390/fire7050169

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop