Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Differences in Perceived Stress and Depression among Weight (Dis)Satisfied Midwestern College Students during COVID-19
Previous Article in Journal
Population-Based Model of the Fraction of Incidental COVID-19 Hospitalizations during the Omicron BA.1 Wave in the United States
Previous Article in Special Issue
Evusheld Prophylaxis Improves Social Interactions, Anxiety, Depression, Agoraphobia, and Quality of Life in Blood Cancer Patients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Decision-Making during the Pandemic: Compassion, Trust, and the Altruistic Paradox

Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
COVID 2023, 3(5), 744-756; https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3050055
Submission received: 7 April 2023 / Revised: 2 May 2023 / Accepted: 4 May 2023 / Published: 15 May 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue COVID and Post-COVID: The Psychological and Social Impact of COVID-19)

Abstract

:
The occurrence of breakthrough infections with SARS-CoV-2 in vaccinated individuals argues against abandoning mitigation efforts such as social distancing. Some public health messages, however, promote vaccination by increasing psychological distress, which interferes with social distancing. Prosocial messages present an alternative approach that may avoid this problem. Accordingly, the present study examined the relation of pandemic mitigation with scores on prosocial personality traits (i.e., altruism, sympathy, and trust) and vaccination intentions. Regression analyses indicated that while vaccination intentions increased significantly with an increase in trust, distancing increased significantly with increases in altruism and sympathy. Because older adults are much more vulnerable to COVID-19 than younger adults, these findings reveal an altruistic paradox, in which older adults, perhaps the most altruistic portion of the population, may be dependent on the altruistic behavior of younger adults, who may be the least altruistic portion. The challenge for public health messaging will be to motivate younger adults to take the consequences of their mitigation decisions for others into account.

1. Introduction

During the COVID-19 pandemic, the degree of psychological distress has been reported to have reached historically high levels in the US and elsewhere [1,2] although the extent to which the pandemic is the cause of the current level of distress is unclear [2,3]. Regardless of whether or not the COVID-19 pandemic is the primary cause of the current level of psychological distress, the distress is real, and it has clearly come at a bad time. Although psychological distress appears to motivate people to get vaccinated, it also interferes with mitigation behaviors, particularly social distancing [4]. It has been hypothesized that this interference occurs because social deprivation is a major cause of the current distress, and behaviors like social distancing and mask wearing may actually increase people’s feelings of social deprivation [5]. Thus, the more distressed (and socially deprived) a person is, the less likely they are to engage in behaviors that would increase their social deprivation and exacerbate their distress.
This is especially unfortunate because the occurrence of breakthrough infections with COVID-19 in vaccinated and even boosted individuals suggests that it may be a bad time to abandon mitigation efforts such as social distancing, regardless of how aversive those behaviors are. Still, if mitigation behaviors increase distress, people’s decision not to engage in behaviors that increase their distress may have some benefits. It has long been known that psychological distress can impair immune function (e.g., [6,7]). Recently, distress was reported to predict the likelihood of hospitalization with SARS-CoV-2 infection [8], and even more recently, distress levels have been reported to predict post-COVID conditions often known as “long-COVID” [9,10].
Previously, we called attention to the public health conundrum posed by the fact that while distress increases the likelihood of vaccination against SARS-CoV-2, it simultaneously decreases the likelihood of mitigation behaviors [4]. The effects of distress, and social deprivation, in particular, on the function of the immune system only deepen this conundrum, particularly as it affects the public health messaging strategy. Traditional “fear messages” are intended to increase distress while motivating specific health behaviors (e.g., vaccination) that can alleviate that distress. In the present situation, however, increasing distress can clearly have unintended negative health consequences in addition to the intended positive ones. The goal of the present study was to explore the possibility of an alternative, prosocial messaging strategy that could promote effective health behaviors without increasing psychological distress.
Studies suggest that both mitigation and vaccination may be motivated by appeals highlighting their benefits to others [11,12,13]. It is unclear, however, whether this approach works better than appeals to personal benefits [14], whether it works better for some decisions than others (e.g., decisions regarding vaccination compared with mitigation), and whether it works better for some people than others. Older adults, for example, are well known to be more altruistic than younger adults [15]. Are they therefore also more likely to have engaged in or to plan actions that decrease the possibility of infection because this also decreases the possibility of infecting others?
The questions addressed by the present study concern pandemic-related distress, specific personality traits consistent with prosocial attitudes and behaviors, and health behaviors recommended by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), such as social distancing and the likelihood of getting vaccinated in the future (e.g., with a “booster”). Previous research on mitigation and vaccination suggests that when it comes to devising a messaging strategy to encourage healthy behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic, one size will not fit all [1,4,5]—different demographic groups may be responsive to different messages, and different target behaviors may also be responsive to different messages. Compared to young adults, for example, older adults’ likelihood of social distancing is much less sensitive to their level of psychological distress. Regardless of age, social distancing decreases with the level of distress, whereas more distressed individuals are more likely to report having been vaccinated [4].
Although our research during the pandemic underscores the important role of distress in health decision-making [3,4,5], such decisions are complex, because distress can have differential implications for how messaging campaigns might need to be designed in order to manage different behaviors. Moreover, distress and pandemic-related stressors may not be the only important motivating factors. Indeed, given our social nature and the importance of social outcomes, it would be surprising if concern for others was not also important in the decision-making process. If true, then future messaging campaigns would have another avenue for motivating appropriate health behaviors and for engaging in broader social outcomes that are important to well-being. Therefore, the present study explored potentially relevant individual and age differences in personality and other psychological factors that might provide a basis for positive, prosocial appeals.
More specifically, the present study assessed a diverse sample of online participants on three facets of the trait of agreeableness (altruism, sympathy, and trust) from the NEO [16], facets that appear to be directly relevant to healthy behaviors recommended by the CDC. Our working assumption was that individuals who scored highly on a particular personality scale would be more likely to respond positively to appeals consistent with that trait than individuals who scored low on the trait. We hypothesized that those who scored high on altruism and sympathy, for example, would be more responsive to appeals emphasizing a particular behavior’s potential benefits to others than those who scored low on these scales.
We tested our hypothesis by examining the association between individuals’ altruism, sympathy, and trust scores with their vaccination intentions, because the decreased likelihood of getting infected following vaccination necessarily results in a decreased likelihood that one would be infectious—a clear benefit to others. An issue was whether individuals with higher altruism and sympathy scores would report that they were more likely to get vaccinated (or boosted) against the shot. Following the same logic, would they also be more likely to engage in mitigation behaviors because, like vaccination, mitigation behaviors also decrease the likelihood of becoming infectious, thereby benefiting others? Additionally, do individuals with higher trust scores report that they are more likely to get a booster shot, consistent with their having greater trust in claims by doctors and others regarding the efficacy of COVID vaccines?
To address these questions, the present study gathered evidence concerning the relations among personality scores and pandemic-related attitudes and behaviors, evidence that we believe is consistent with the relevance of prosocial personality traits and the potential power of prosocial messaging. Thus, this study broadens the scope of our past work [3,4,5] by investigating personality variables specific to trust and concern for others, while also replicating our previous work on distress, to provide a fuller picture of what might motivate people to engage in recommended behaviors during the pandemic and in the post-COVID period.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited online from 27 February to 3 March 2022. After indicating their consent, 806 MTurk workers, aged 18 to 84 years, completed the survey and received $2.00 for their participation, which took 16.4 min on average. The submitted surveys were screened for age, participation in a previous study [4], and a valid IP address associated with internet providers in the United States of America. They were also screened for survey completion times shorter than that which a fast, expert reader would require just to read the questions [17]. Fifty surveys were excluded from analysis: 7 based on invalid IP addresses, 10 based on participation in our previous study, 5 based on invalid responses, and 28 based on a completion time of less than 5 min. Analyses were conducted on the remaining 756 surveys (Table 1).
The median income for each age group was in the second quartile of the US population in 2021 (i.e., between $31 K and $65 K). Across the full sample, participants’ racial/ethnic breakdown was 83.6% White, 12.3% Black, 2.1% Asian, and 1.9% other races, while 19.4% identified as Hispanic/Latinx. Although the table divides the sample into age groups, subsequent analyses were based on age as a continuous measure.

2.2. Procedure

The survey consisted of four parts, although all of the items measured were not analyzed in the present study. The survey began with questions about CDC-recommended mitigation behaviors. Specifically, the mitigation measures included two social distancing behaviors and one hand hygiene measure: “how many times were you in close proximity (i.e., closer than 6 feet) to someone you do not live with?” (Prox); how many times were you “in an enclosed public space?” (Public); and (3) “how often did you clean your hands with hand sanitizer or soap and water while in a public place or right after returning home?” (Hand). For each mitigation behavior, participants were asked about its frequency “on average this week” and “before the pandemic began”. Although the survey also asked about wearing a mask, these data were not analyzed because of their dependence on local mandates specific to businesses and communities.
The second part of the survey consisted of the 14-item Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, or HADS [18], questions about participants’ degree of concern about the possible effects of the pandemic on themselves and others, and questions regarding participants’ current levels of loneliness and social deprivation (e.g., “How often do you feel isolated from others?”) [19]. Participants then were asked about personal connections with COVID-19 cases (e.g., number of acquaintances hospitalized) and their opinions regarding the safety and efficacy of vaccines against COVID-19 (e.g., “Do you believe vaccination against COVID-19 is safe?”).
The third part of the survey asked questions about participants’ overall health and the frequency of their attendance at religious gatherings as well as other demographic items (e.g., current employment status and household income, level of formal education, age, gender, ethnicity, and race). Participants also answered the questions on the Kessler Screening Scale for Psychological Distress (K6).
The fourth part of the survey asked about participants’ vaccination status, although problems with the Qualtrics implementation made responses unanalyzable. Importantly, participants were also asked about their vaccination intentions, specifically if, regardless of their current status, they would get vaccinated if it was recommended to them. Participants also were asked to select up to three statements regarding what they missed about life before the COVID-19 pandemic. Following this, participants were asked to respond to ten questions each for altruism, sympathy, and trust, three facets of the agreeableness scale of the IPIP-NEO (International Personality Item Pool—Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness) personality test. (The specific items used from this standardized test are available at osf.io/jmykd/, posted on 1 May 2023). Finally, participants were asked about their political affiliation, who they voted for in the 2020 United States Presidential Election, and the zip code for their current residence.

3. Results

3.1. Mitigation

Participants’ survey responses in February–March 2022 indicated that both the frequency of close proximity interactions with nonhousehold members and the frequency of visits to enclosed public spaces were significantly lower during the pandemic than they were before the pandemic (Table 2). In contrast, although washing and/or disinfecting one’s hands increased in frequency, it may be noted that while the median frequencies of both social distancing measures (close proximity encounters and visits to enclosed public spaces) decreased, the median frequency of washing/disinfecting one’s hands (hand hygiene) did not change (Figure 1). Because of the skewed distributions of the mitigation measures (all three comparisons failed the Shapiro–Wilk test), changes from before the pandemic to the time of the survey (during the pandemic) were converted to binary variables (1 = change in the CDC-recommended direction) and examined with a logistic regression using JASP software to assess potential predictors of the likelihood of CDC-recommended changes in mitigation behaviors (i.e., decreases for social distancing measures, and increases for hand hygiene). It should be noted that, while the majority of participants engaged in social distancing (decreases in weekly frequency), the percentage who increased their hand hygiene did not differ significantly from chance, and as there was no evidence of mitigation, no further analyses of this measure were conducted (Table 2).
Linear regression analyses conducted with SigmaPlot software revealed that while distress (i.e., anxiety and depression) decreased with age, replicating the results of Myerson et al. [4,5], compassion (i.e., altruism and sympathy) increased as did trust (Figure 2): rs = −0.321, 0.480, and 0.145, respectively. While in previous studies, the attention has been on age differences in distress, and the association of age with compassion has not been examined, the present results suggest that the latter relation is stronger. For the linear regression functions depicted in the figure, age accounted for approximately twice as much of the variance in compassion compared with the variance in distress accounted for by age (R2s = 0.231 and 0.103, respectively).
The primary goal of the present study was to examine the roles played by age and individual differences in psychological distress (anxiety and depression scores) and three facets of the trait of agreeableness (altruism, sympathy, and trust) on mitigation and vaccination decisions (Table 3). To minimize multicollinearity, variables that were both strongly correlated and conceptually related were represented by their mean Z scores in subsequent analyses (Table 4). Throughout, the variable representing the combination of anxiety and depression is termed distress and the variable representing the combination of altruism and sympathy is labelled compassion.
Linear regression analyses revealed that while distress decreased with age, replicating the results of Myerson et al. [4,5], compassion increased, as did trust (Figure 2): rs = −0.321, 0.480, and 0.145, respectively (Figure 2). While in previous studies, attention has been on age differences in distress, and the association of age with compassion has not been examined, the present results suggest that the latter relation is stronger. For the linear regression functions depicted in the figure, age accounted for approximately twice as much of the variance in compassion compared with the variance in distress accounted for by age (R2s = 0.231 and 0.103, respectively).
The results of logistic regression analyses conducted to test the differential distress hypothesis are presented in Table 5. As predicted by the differential distress hypothesis, greater distress was associated with a lower likelihood of having engaged in social distancing (i.e., the distress coefficients were negative in the models predicting both the Prox and Publ measures).
The results of further logistic regression analyses conducted to assess the contributions of compassion and trust to the likelihood of mitigation behaviors are presented in Table 6. When both compassion and trust were added to the regression model, only compassion was a significant predictor of either of the two social distancing measures.
When added to the regression models in Table 6, neither party affiliation nor vote in the 2020 US presidential election were significant predictors of either of the social distancing mitigation behaviors (Prox and Publ). To assess the roles of the demographic measures health, income, and education, these measures were added to reduced versions of the models consisting of only the significant predictors. For example, to assess the role of health in predicting decreases in close proximity interactions (Prox), the independent variables consisted of only health and distress. The six resulting tests (3 demographic measures times 2 mitigation behaviors) revealed that while all previous predictors remained significant when the demographic measures were tested, only one demographic measure was significant: Poorer self-rated health was significantly associated with a lower likelihood of the recommended decrease in visits to enclosed public spaces (odds ratio = 0.781, Z = −2.922, p = 0.003).

3.2. Vaccination Intentions

The analysis of participants’ self-rated vaccination intentions was roughly analogous to the preceding analysis of their mitigation behaviors. That is, analysis began with the prediction of intentions from the independent variables distress and age and then individual differences in compassion were added first, followed by trust. Finally, as with the analyses of the mitigation behaviors, the roles of political affiliations and preferences were analyzed, after which the roles of basic demographic variables were examined. Models with party affiliation as a binary predictor (e.g., democrat vs. not a democrat; R2 = 0.092) accounted for approximately half as much variance as corresponding models with preferred presidential candidates as a predictor (e.g., Biden vs. not Biden; R2 = 0.185). Because of the interest in the influence of Donald J. Trump on vaccinations, results are reported here for a binary variable comparing those who voted for Donald J. Trump with those who did not, although because most participants (93.5%) reported that they voted for either Trump or Biden, analyses yielded comparable results, albeit opposite in sign, when Biden voters were compared with non-Biden voters. Notably, Biden voters (553) outnumbered Trump voters (197) in the present sample.
The first regression model in the planned sequence of analyses included just distress and age as independent variables and was not a significant predictor of vaccination intentions (i.e., the variable likely); the second model, which added compassion, also failed to reach significance (both R2s = 0.008, both ps > 0.05). Including Trust as an additional independent variable, however, resulted in a model that was a significant predictor of vaccination intentions, and the results for this model as well as for the model that examined political preferences are presented in Table 7. In contrast to what was observed in analyses of models predicting mitigation behaviors, in which compassion was a significant predictor and trust was not, the opposite was true for models predicting vaccination intentions. In such models, trust was a significant predictor and compassion was not (compare Table 6 with Table 7). Moreover, although political preference was not a predictor of mitigation, it was a significant predictor of vaccination intentions.
Further analysis suggested a mechanism that may account for the role played by trust in vaccination intentions. Notably, trust was significantly correlated with a variable (Vacc) indicating how strongly participants rated the safety and efficacy of vaccines against COVID-19 (rho = 0.316, p < 0.001); it may be noted that as with some other measures used in this study, safety and efficacy scores were combined and analyzed as mean Z-scores (Vacc) to minimize multicollinearity because of the strong correlation between them (r = 0.690). Notably, a regression model consisting of Vacc in addition to Trust and the binary variable TrmpYN (1 = voted for Donald J. Trump, 0 = did not vote or voted for someone else) accounted for 36.7% of the variance in likely, raising the possibility that much of the variance accounted for by trust could be explained by its contribution to belief in vaccination as a preventive measure. Nonetheless, in that model (Table 8), trust was shown to make a significant and unique contribution to vaccination intentions.

4. Discussion

The present study had two major goals: first, to assess the replicability of the association of psychological distress with mitigation and vaccination that led to formulation of the differential distress hypothesis [4], and second, to examine the association of specific personality traits with mitigation and vaccination decisions. With respect to the first goal, it should be noted that our initial findings regarding mitigation were based on data collected in April 2020, at the very beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic [5]. In the present sample, surveyed nearly two years later, the majority of the participants were engaged in social distancing. In fact, the percentage (67.3%) of those in the sample who reported they had decreased the frequency of their close proximity interactions with nonhousehold members since before the pandemic was very similar to the corresponding percentage (61.0%) of the sample studied more than two years before. Moreover, not only was the overall level of psychological distress in the present study similar to that measured in the two previous studies, but so were the levels of the two components of distress measure. For example, the mean anxiety and depression scores on the HADS for participants in the present sample were 8.97 and 6.63; the corresponding means for the initial sample were 8.03 and 6.33. The stability of these measures is a positive sign suggesting that online surveys such as that used in the present study potentially provide reliable information concerning psychological phenomena in a continuously changing pandemic environment.

4.1. Mitigation

The differential distress hypothesis specifically predicts that social distancing will decrease with the level of psychological distress. The negative association between the level of distress and social distancing, initially observed in 2020 [5] and first replicated in a follow-up study conducted a year later [4], was replicated again in the present study. More specifically, not only were decreases in the frequency of close proximity interactions with nonhousehold members significantly associated with the level of psychological distress, as shown in both of the previous studies in this series (i.e., [4,5]), but as predicted by the differential distress hypothesis, so too were decreases in the frequency of visits to enclosed public spaces, another social distancing behavior.
As Myerson et al. [4] pointed out, the observed negative association of psychological distress with mitigation behaviors poses potential problems for the use of traditional fear appeals to promote mitigation behaviors. Such messages typically highlight current stressors (e.g., social deprivation and risks of infection in the present situation) and indicate that specific health behaviors could change the situation. According to the differential distress hypothesis, there are two problems with this approach. First, the messages call attention to current stressors, and while this may have some positive effects (e.g., increasing the likelihood of vaccination), it may simultaneously have serious negative effects (e.g., decreasing mitigation behaviors like social distancing). This might not be a problem if, for example, vaccines are always completely effective, but they are not (e.g., breakthrough infections), and the deliberately increased distress may interfere with the very mitigation behaviors that could decrease the likelihood of subsequent infections. Second, as Myerson et al. found, older adults are not only less sensitive to pandemic stressors, but even when their distress levels were high, their vaccination decisions are much less affected than those of younger adults with equivalent levels of distress [4].
The use of prosocial health messages could represent an alternative approach that, in contrast, would build specifically on the strengths of older adults. This is because it could appeal to people’s altruism and sympathy for others, both of which increase significantly with age and are obviously relevant to people’s mitigation and vaccination decisions. (trust increases significantly with age as well, but to a lesser degree.)
When compassion and trust were both included in logistic regression models of social distancing behaviors along with age and distress, compassion was a significant predictor, but age, distress, and trust were not. Notably, when political preferences, as reflected in participants’ votes in the 2020 presidential election, were added to a model that also included age, distress, compassion, and trust (e.g., as independent variables, such preferences were not significant. This finding may have important implications because, as discussed in the following section, it is one of several significant differences between the present results for mitigation and vaccination, suggesting that different factors are involved in mitigation and vaccination decisions. The results for compassion and mitigation may be especially important because they are directly relevant to the problem of determining how to motivate mitigation behaviors such as social distancing without distressing the recipients of public health messages that are intended to motivate such behaviors, as other findings indicate that increased distress in such cases would be counterproductive.

4.2. Vaccination Intentions

Linear regression models designed to test the differential distress hypothesis and assess whether compassion scores predicted vaccination intentions, as was the case with mitigation behaviors in the present study, accounted for less than one percent of the variance and failed to reach significance in both cases. A model that added trust to the independent variables was significant, however, and a model that added voter preference in the 2020 election to the potential predictors of vaccination intentions was not only significant, it more than tripled the variance accounted for in vaccination intentions (R2 = 0.146). Further analysis identified beliefs about the safety and efficacy of current vaccines as the single best predictor of intentions. This measure (Vacc) alone accounted for 34.2% of the variance in vaccination intentions. When Vacc, trust, and voter preference in 2020 were all used as independent variables in a regression model predicting vaccination intentions, all three independent variables were significant.
These findings are consistent with the politicization of vaccination. For example, Biden and Trump voters, perhaps not surprisingly, differed significantly not only in their beliefs in vaccine safety and efficacy but also in their vaccination intentions. In contrast, they did not differ in their levels of psychological distress or trust, although it should be noted that the trust facet of agreeableness measures trust in other people and not in institutions, either in general or specifically, so, for example, it does not apply to trust in science or medicine or the public health system itself.
Taken together these findings suggest that although it is possible that the assumptions of the differential distress hypothesis are correct with respect to psychological distress and vaccination, the role of distress as a predictor may be overwhelmed by the effects of variation in political attitudes, beliefs, and preferences. The same does not seem to be true for individual differences in trust, which appear to vary independently of people’s politics. Notably, trust does appear to play a role in vaccination decisions and in people’s evaluation of present vaccines themselves and even makes similar contributions to the vaccination intentions of Trump and Biden supporters, despite the fact that, on average, their intentions differ markedly.
While information regarding individual differences in vaccination intentions that is more directly applicable to public health messaging will obviously require further research, it seems clear that the differential distress hypothesis is correct about one thing: vaccination and mitigation decisions are affected by different factors. While the hypothesis focused on the differential effects of psychological distress, the present results suggest that there are other variables that can play more prominent roles. These include several aspects of personality traits as well as political preferences and attitudes. With respect to aspects of personality, the present results reveal the importance of individual differences in trust in other people in determining the vaccination intentions of the present sample, regardless of their political inclinations, whereas mitigation behaviors appear to be independent of trust in other people but are significantly affected by differences in compassion (i.e., one’s altruism and sympathy for others). Further, political preferences currently appear to play a major role in one’s vaccination intentions, whereas they do not contribute significantly to mitigation behaviors (e.g., social distancing).

4.3. Implications

What are the implications of the present findings for public health messaging? First, and most obviously, they argue against a one-size-fits-all approach, and more specifically, are in favor of approaching the issues of mitigation and vaccination differently. The present results are consistent with previous studies (e.g., [4,5]) in showing that distress may interfere with social distancing, implying that fear messages must be handled with care, but that is an example of what not to do, or what needs, at least, to be approached carefully. The present exploration of the role of individual differences in personality attributes was, of course, motivated by a search for an alternative approach.
With respect to mitigation, the important role that appears to be played by compassion does not just mean that prosocial appeals may be a good fit for altruistic and sympathetic or empathetic people and that messaging should consider market segmentation approaches to communication. In addition, it should be noted that, while altruism is a propensity for a certain kind of behavior, those with a lower propensity may also be motivated by prosocial appeals. Altruistic appeals for donations, for example, are successful with all kinds of people although, consistent with the age differences observed here, they may be more successful with older adults, who appear to be more compassionate than younger adults (for a meta-analytic review, see [15]). Interestingly, stress may actually increase altruism in younger adults, although not in older adults, perhaps because older adults are less affected by stress [3]. Nevertheless, older adults are more altruistic under both stressed and unstressed conditions [20], suggesting that prosocial appeals may be especially effective with them.
We note in passing that, although we focused on age largely because of older adults’ special vulnerability to COVID-19, and neither income nor education appeared to play significant roles, other demographic groups may also be especially at risk, and further studies targeting such groups is clearly needed. While older adults, fortunately, tend to be less sensitive to pandemic and other stressors, females, for example, appear to be especially sensitive [2,3], which is another factor that perhaps probably should be taken into account in public health messaging.
With respect to vaccination intentions, the present results suggest that while political inclinations can play a major role, the importance of individual differences in trust may present significant opportunities. Indeed, reliance on messengers already trusted in the community is now recognized as very important [21]. Indeed, while the present results showed that, not unexpectedly, beliefs about the safety and efficacy of vaccines play a critical role, the results also suggest that individual differences in trust are important in the formation of those beliefs. Indeed, the corresponding variables trust and Vacc are strongly correlated (r = 0.585), suggesting that trust also plays a critical role in determining those beliefs. Importantly, it appears to have played a similar role in modulating participants’ intentions independently of their political views.
One of the biggest challenges for public health messaging in the immediate future may be that it has become increasingly clear how much lower the risk of serious consequences from SARS-CoV-2 infection is for younger and middle-aged adults compared to that for older adults. Indeed, the CDC website notes that the rate of hospitalization for those 65–74 is 4.9 times that of young adults (18–29 years), increasing to 8.9 times the young adult rate for those aged 75–84 years. The age differences in the rate of deaths from COVID-19 are even starker: 60 and 140 times the young adult rate for those aged 65–74 and 75–84 years, respectively [22], which may suggest to younger adults that only the elderly and those with compromised immune systems really need to maintain their mitigation behaviors. Perhaps exacerbating the problem, public health policies and messaging both have begun emphasizing the importance of everyone evaluating the risks to themselves personally. Unfortunately, this may lead those at high risk, whose safety continues to rely heavily on the precautions taken by others, to believe, perhaps correctly, that the system is beginning to abandon them.
Although new medications (e.g., Paxlovid) and treatment approaches may help, of course, the asymmetry of the problem remains. More specifically, this asymmetry creates what may be termed an altruistic paradox in that, paradoxically, those for whom prosocial, altruistic appeals would seem to be ethically most appropriate (i.e., those at relatively lower risk, such as younger adults) are those whom the present findings suggest may be least responsive to such appeals. This is because compassion appears to be lowest in younger adults and highest in older adults, who should be most motivated by personal concerns. This is the challenge. Although younger adults also appear to be lower in trust than older adults, the age difference is relatively small, which suggests that it will be especially important to use people already trusted by younger adults to communicate the need for them to “play it safe,” acknowledging that it is others who are the most likely to benefit. In other words, what may be required is not just communicating, but also leading. The challenge for public health messaging will be to motivate younger adults to take the consequences of their mitigation decisions for others into account.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.M., L.G., M.J.S., and S.H.; methodology, J.M., L.G., M.J.S., and S.H.; software, B.B.; formal analysis, J.M. and M.J.S.; investigation, B.B.; data curation, B.B.; writing—original draft preparation, J.M. and B.B.; writing—review and editing, J.M., L.G., M.J.S., and S.H.; visualization, J.M.; supervision, J.M.; funding acquisition, J.M. and L.G.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under award number RO1AG058885.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Washington University in St. Louis (protocol code 201806131, approved on 25 October 21).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: osf.io/jmykd/, posted on 1 May 2023.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Aknin, L.B.; De Neve, J.-E.; Dunn, E.W.; Fancourt, D.E.; Goldberg, E.; Helliwell, J.F.; Jones, S.P.; Karam, E.; Layard, R.; Lyubomirsky, S.; et al. Mental Health During the First Year of the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Review and Recommendations for Moving Forward. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 17, 915–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Sun, Y.; Wu, Y.; Fan, S.; Dal Santo, T.; Li, L.; Jiang, X.; Li, K.; Wang, Y.; Tasleem, A.; Krishnan, A.; et al. Comparison of mental health symptoms before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from a systematic review and meta-analysis of 134 cohorts. BMJ 2023, 380, e074224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hale, S.; Myerson, J.; Strube, M.J.; Green, L.; Lewandowski, A.B. Distress Signals: Age Differences in Psychological Distress before and during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 3549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Myerson, J.; Strube, M.J.; Green, L.; Hale, S.; Bernstein, B. Differential effects of psychological distress on mitigation and vaccination: A public health conundrum. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 923056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Myerson, J.; Strube, M.J.; Green, L.; Hale, S. Individual differences in COVID-19 mitigation behaviors: The roles of age, gender, psychological state, and financial status. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Cohen, S.; Williamson, G.M. Stress and infectious disease in humans. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 5–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Nakata, A.; Irie, M.; Takahashi, M. Psychological distress, depressive symptoms, and cellular immunity among healthy individuals: A 1-year prospective study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011, 81, 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Wang, S.; Quan, L.; Ding, M.; Kang, J.H.; Koenen, K.C.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Branch-Elliman, W.; Chavarro, J.E.; Roberts, A.L. Depression, worry, and loneliness are associated with subsequent risk of hospitalization for COVID-19: A prospective study. Psychol. Med. 2022, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Wang, S.; Quan, L.; Chavarro, J.E.; Slopen, N.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Koenen, K.C.; Kang, J.H.; Weisskopf, M.G.; Branch-Elliman, W.; Roberts, A.L. Associations of Depression, Anxiety, Worry, Perceived Stress, and Loneliness Prior to Infection with Risk of Post–COVID-19 Conditions. JAMA Psychiatry 2022, 79, 1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Tsampasian, V.; Elghazaly, H.; Chattopadhyay, R.; Debski, M.; Naing, T.K.P.; Garg, P.; Clark, A.; Ntatsaki, E.; Vassiliou, V.S. Risk Factors Associated with Post−COVID-19 Condition: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA Intern. Med. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Ebrahimi, O.V.; Hoffart, A.; Johnson, S.U. Physical Distancing and Mental Health During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Factors Associated With Psychological Symptoms and Adherence to Pandemic Mitigation Strategies. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 9, 489–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jordan, J.J.; Yoeli, E.; Rand, D.G. Don’t get it or don’t spread it: Comparing self-interested versus prosocial motivations for COVID-19 prevention behaviors. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 20222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Böhm, R.; Betsch, C. Prosocial vaccination. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 43, 307–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ashworth, M.; Thunström, L.; Cherry, T.L.; Newbold, S.C.; Finnoff, D.C. Emphasize personal health benefits to boost COVID-19 vaccination rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2108225118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Sparrow, E.P.; Swirsky, L.T.; Kudus, F.; Spaniol, J. Aging and altruism: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 2021, 36, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Goldberg, L.R. A Broad-Bandwidth, Public Domain, Personality Inventory Measuring the Lower-Level Facets of Several Five-Factor Models. Personal. Psychol. Eur. 1999, 7, 7–28. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rayner, K.; Schotter, E.R.; Masson, M.E.J.; Potter, M.C.; Treiman, R. So Much to Read, So Little Time: How Do We Read, and Can Speed Reading Help? Psychol. Sci. Public Interes. 2016, 17, 4–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Zigmond, A.S.; Snaith, R.P. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983, 67, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Hughes, M.E.; Waite, L.J.; Hawkley, L.C.; Cacioppo, J.T. A Short Scale for Measuring Loneliness in Large Surveys: Results From Two Population-Based Studies. Res. Aging 2004, 26, 655–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Sparrow, E.P.; Armstrong, B.A.; Fiocco, A.J.; Spaniol, J. Acute stress and altruism in younger and older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 100, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. World Health Organization. Risk Communication and Community Engagement Readiness and Response to Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): Interim Guidance, 19 March 2020; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2020; p. 5, No. WHO/2019-nCoV/RCCE/2020.2; Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/331513 (accessed on 4 April 2023).
  22. Risk for COVID-19 Infection, Hospitalization, and Death by Age Group. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 6 February 2023. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/covid-data/investigations-discovery/hospitalization-death-by-age.html (accessed on 4 April 2023).
Figure 1. Box plots of the frequencies of three mitigation behaviors before and during the COVID pandemic.
Figure 1. Box plots of the frequencies of three mitigation behaviors before and during the COVID pandemic.
Covid 03 00055 g001
Figure 2. Distress, compassion, and trust plotted as functions of age. Colored lines represent the results of the linear regression analyses; gray lines indicate the 95% confidence intervals for the regression lines.
Figure 2. Distress, compassion, and trust plotted as functions of age. Colored lines represent the results of the linear regression analyses; gray lines indicate the 95% confidence intervals for the regression lines.
Covid 03 00055 g002
Table 1. Participant Characteristics as a Function of Age Group.
Table 1. Participant Characteristics as a Function of Age Group.
Age Group N Age (M) % Female Ed Yrs (Mdn)
18–29 14925.848.3%16 years
30–39 19433.332.5%16 years
40–49 12143.240.5%16 years
50–59 7356.042.5%16 years
60–69 17364.064.7%16 years
70–80+ 4573.557.8%14 years
M = Mean, Ed Yrs = Years of Education, Mdn = Median.
Table 2. Median weekly frequency of mitigation behaviors before and during the COVID-19 pandemic and percentages reporting CDC-recommended changes (%Δ).
Table 2. Median weekly frequency of mitigation behaviors before and during the COVID-19 pandemic and percentages reporting CDC-recommended changes (%Δ).
VariableBeforeDuringz
Prox5461.05.91
Publ5362.06.56
Hand5549.3−0.35
z = z-score, Prox = close proximity, Publ = public space, Hand = hand hygiene.
Table 3. Means and standard deviations for age, anxiety, and depression scores and scores on altruism, sympathy, and trust facets of agreeableness.
Table 3. Means and standard deviations for age, anxiety, and depression scores and scores on altruism, sympathy, and trust facets of agreeableness.
AgeAnxietyDepressionAltruismSympathyTrust
Mean45.058.976.6333.2236.2033.73
Std Dev16.084.944.006.457.346.97
Table 4. Intercorrelations among age, anxiety, and depression scores and scores on altruism, sympathy, and trust facets of agreeableness.
Table 4. Intercorrelations among age, anxiety, and depression scores and scores on altruism, sympathy, and trust facets of agreeableness.
VariableAgeAnxietyDepressionAltruismSympathyTrust
Age
Anxiety−0.369
Depression−0.2160.663
Altruism0.473−0.588−0.502
Sympathy0.430−0.500−0.4130.768
Trust0.145−0.392−0.4070.4300.355
Note: All correlations were statistically significant, all ps < 0.001; bold italics are used here to denote two facets that were strongly correlated (see text for additional details).
Table 5. Logistic regression analyses predicting the two social distancing measures: Prox and Publ.
Table 5. Logistic regression analyses predicting the two social distancing measures: Prox and Publ.
ProxEstimateS.E.Odds RatioZWald Statisticp
(Intercept)0.2040.2431.2260.8380.7020.402
Distress−0.3680.0910.692−4.03416.271<0.001
Age0.0060.0051.0061.0971.2030.273
PublEstimateS.E.Odds RatioZWald Statisticp
(Intercept)0.1860.2371.2040.7860.6180.432
Distress−0.2630.0890.769−2.9428.6580.003
Age0.0070.0051.0071.3661.8660.172
S.E. = standard error of the mean, Z = Z-score.
Table 6. Results of logistic regression analyses adding trust as a potential predictor of mitigation.
Table 6. Results of logistic regression analyses adding trust as a potential predictor of mitigation.
ProxEstimateS.E.Odds RatioZWald Statisticp
(Intercept)0.8260.2752.2842.3965.7430.017
Distress−0.1100.1130.896−0.9660.9330.334
Age−0.0080.0060.992−1.3291.7670.184
Compassion0.6840.1231.9815.55530.853<0.001
Trust−0.1140.0960.892−1.1921.4210.233
PublEstimateS.E.Odds RatioZWald Statisticp
(Intercept)0.7660.2662.1512.8758.2640.042
Distress0.0160.1131.0160.1400.0200.889
Age−0.0050.0060.995−0.9680.9370.333
Compassion0.6600.1221.9355.40629.223<0.001
Trust−0.0790.0940.924−0.8360.6990.403
S.E. = standard error of the mean, Z = Z-score.
Table 7. Results of multiple regression analyses predicting vaccination intentions (likely) from distress, age, compassion, trust, and votes in the 2020 US presidential election.
Table 7. Results of multiple regression analyses predicting vaccination intentions (likely) from distress, age, compassion, trust, and votes in the 2020 US presidential election.
Predict Likely from Distress, Age, Compassion, and Trust: R2 = 0.043.
UnstandardizedS.E.Standardizedtp
(Intercept)3.7610.146 25.850<0.001
Age0.0000.003−0.003−0.0630.950
Distress−0.0150.061−0.011−0.2480.805
Compassion−0.0340.063−0.027−0.5420.588
Trust0.2560.0490.2125.178<0.001
Predict Likely from Distress, Age, Compassion, Trust, and Trump: R2 = 0.146.
UnstandardizedS.E.Standardizedtp
(Intercept)3.7980.138 27.606<0.001
Age0.0030.0030.0451.1570.248
Distress−0.0030.057−0.003−0.0580.954
Compassion−0.0050.060−0.004−0.0790.937
Trust0.2390.0470.1985.109<0.001
TrmpYN−0.9810.103−0.326−9.502<0.001
S.E. = standard error of the mean, t = Student’s t statistic.
Table 8. Results of a multiple regression analysis predicting vaccination intentions (likely) from trust, ratings of vaccine safety and efficacy (Vacc) and votes in the 2020 US presidential election.
Table 8. Results of a multiple regression analysis predicting vaccination intentions (likely) from trust, ratings of vaccine safety and efficacy (Vacc) and votes in the 2020 US presidential election.
UnstandardizedS.E.Standardizedtp
(Intercept)3.8800.040 98.144<0.001
Trust0.0860.0360.0712.3710.018
TrmpYN−0.4670.092−0.156−5.072<0.001
Vacc0.6280.0390.51816.307<0.001
S.E. = standard error of the mean, t = Student’s t statistic.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Myerson, J.; Strube, M.J.; Green, L.; Hale, S.; Bernstein, B. Decision-Making during the Pandemic: Compassion, Trust, and the Altruistic Paradox. COVID 2023, 3, 744-756. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3050055

AMA Style

Myerson J, Strube MJ, Green L, Hale S, Bernstein B. Decision-Making during the Pandemic: Compassion, Trust, and the Altruistic Paradox. COVID. 2023; 3(5):744-756. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3050055

Chicago/Turabian Style

Myerson, Joel, Michael J. Strube, Leonard Green, Sandra Hale, and Bridget Bernstein. 2023. "Decision-Making during the Pandemic: Compassion, Trust, and the Altruistic Paradox" COVID 3, no. 5: 744-756. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3050055

APA Style

Myerson, J., Strube, M. J., Green, L., Hale, S., & Bernstein, B. (2023). Decision-Making during the Pandemic: Compassion, Trust, and the Altruistic Paradox. COVID, 3(5), 744-756. https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3050055

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop